Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 5/2021

16-06-2020 | Original Article

Do interoceptive accuracy and interoceptive sensibility predict emotion regulation?

Auteurs: Stephanie A. Schuette, Nancy L. Zucker, Moria J. Smoski

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 5/2021

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Introduction

Interoception refers to awareness, interpretation, and integration of sensations in the body. While interoceptive accuracy has long been regarded as a core component of emotional experience, less is known about the relationship of interoceptive accuracy and related facets of interoception to emotion regulation deficits. This study explores how interoceptive accuracy and interoceptive sensibility relate to emotion regulation in a non-clinical sample.

Methods

Undergraduate participants completed a heartbeat perception task and the Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness (Noticing and Body Listening sub-scales), and rated their confidence in performance on the heartbeat perception task. Participants also completed self-report measures of emotional awareness and regulation (Profile of Emotional Competence, intrapersonal emotion identification and emotion regulation sub-scales), and rated their use of different coping strategies (Brief COPE).

Results

Noticing predicted emotion identification, emotion regulation, and the use of adaptive but not maladaptive coping strategies. Heartbeat perception accuracy did not significantly contribute to the prediction of any outcome variables.

Discussion

Future work is needed to extend these findings to clinical populations. The results from this study support the use of interoceptive training interventions to promote emotional wellbeing.
Literatuur
go back to reference Beck, A. T. (1993). Cognitive therapy: past, present, and future. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61(2), 194.CrossRef Beck, A. T. (1993). Cognitive therapy: past, present, and future. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61(2), 194.CrossRef
go back to reference BIOPAC (1997). BIOPAC MP100 psychophysiological data acquisition system: BIOPAC Systems, Inc. BIOPAC (1997). BIOPAC MP100 psychophysiological data acquisition system: BIOPAC Systems, Inc.
go back to reference Carver, C. S. (1997). You want to measure coping but your protocol is too long: consider the brief cope. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 4(1), 92.CrossRef Carver, C. S. (1997). You want to measure coping but your protocol is too long: consider the brief cope. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 4(1), 92.CrossRef
go back to reference Craig, A. (2008). Interoception and emotion: a neuroanatomical perspective. Handbook of Emotions, 3(602), 272–288. Craig, A. (2008). Interoception and emotion: a neuroanatomical perspective. Handbook of Emotions, 3(602), 272–288.
go back to reference Craske, M. G., Lang, A. J., Tsao, J. C., Mystkowski, J. L., & Rowe, M. K. (2001). Reactivity to interoceptive cues in nocturnal panic. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 32(3), 173–190.CrossRef Craske, M. G., Lang, A. J., Tsao, J. C., Mystkowski, J. L., & Rowe, M. K. (2001). Reactivity to interoceptive cues in nocturnal panic. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 32(3), 173–190.CrossRef
go back to reference Damasio, A. R. (1996). The somatic marker hypothesis and the possible functions of the prefrontal cortex. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B Series, 351(1346), 1413–1420.CrossRef Damasio, A. R. (1996). The somatic marker hypothesis and the possible functions of the prefrontal cortex. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B Series, 351(1346), 1413–1420.CrossRef
go back to reference Ehlers, A. (1998). Why Van der Does, Van Dyck and Spinhoven (1997) did not demonstrate that superior heartbeat perception in panic disorder is partly an artefact. Journal of Affective Disorders, 49(3), 241–247.CrossRef Ehlers, A. (1998). Why Van der Does, Van Dyck and Spinhoven (1997) did not demonstrate that superior heartbeat perception in panic disorder is partly an artefact. Journal of Affective Disorders, 49(3), 241–247.CrossRef
go back to reference Ehlers, A., & Breuer, P. (1992). Increased cardiac awareness in panic disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101(3), 371–382.CrossRef Ehlers, A., & Breuer, P. (1992). Increased cardiac awareness in panic disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101(3), 371–382.CrossRef
go back to reference Ehlers, A., Breuer, P., Dohn, D., & Fiegenbaum, W. (1995). Heartbeat perception and panic disorder: Possible explanations for discrepant findings. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33(1), 69–76.CrossRef Ehlers, A., Breuer, P., Dohn, D., & Fiegenbaum, W. (1995). Heartbeat perception and panic disorder: Possible explanations for discrepant findings. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33(1), 69–76.CrossRef
go back to reference Ernst, J., Boker, H., Hattenschwiler, J., Schupbach, D., Northoff, G., Seifritz, E., et al. (2014). The association of interoceptive awareness and alexithymia with neurotransmitter concentrations in insula and anterior cingulate. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(6), 857–863. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nst058.CrossRefPubMed Ernst, J., Boker, H., Hattenschwiler, J., Schupbach, D., Northoff, G., Seifritz, E., et al. (2014). The association of interoceptive awareness and alexithymia with neurotransmitter concentrations in insula and anterior cingulate. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(6), 857–863. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1093/​scan/​nst058.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191.CrossRef Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191.CrossRef
go back to reference Fissler, M., Winnebeck, E., Schroeter, T., Gummersbach, M., Huntenburg, J. M., Gaertner, M., et al. (2016). An investigation of the effects of brief mindfulness training on self-reported interoceptive awareness, the ability to decenter, and their role in the reduction of depressive symptoms. Mindfulness, 7(5), 1170–1181.CrossRef Fissler, M., Winnebeck, E., Schroeter, T., Gummersbach, M., Huntenburg, J. M., Gaertner, M., et al. (2016). An investigation of the effects of brief mindfulness training on self-reported interoceptive awareness, the ability to decenter, and their role in the reduction of depressive symptoms. Mindfulness, 7(5), 1170–1181.CrossRef
go back to reference Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(2), 348–362.CrossRef Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(2), 348–362.CrossRef
go back to reference IBM Corp. Released 2016. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 24.0. NY: IBM Corp. IBM Corp. Released 2016. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 24.0. NY: IBM Corp.
go back to reference Kever, A., Pollatos, O., Vermeulen, N., & Grynberg, D. (2015). Interoceptive sensitivity facilitates both antecedent-and response-focused emotion regulation strategies. Personality and Individual Differences, 87, 20–23.CrossRef Kever, A., Pollatos, O., Vermeulen, N., & Grynberg, D. (2015). Interoceptive sensitivity facilitates both antecedent-and response-focused emotion regulation strategies. Personality and Individual Differences, 87, 20–23.CrossRef
go back to reference Knoll, J. F., & Hodapp, V. (1992). A comparison between two methods for assessing heartbeat perception. Psychophysiology, 29(2), 218–222.CrossRef Knoll, J. F., & Hodapp, V. (1992). A comparison between two methods for assessing heartbeat perception. Psychophysiology, 29(2), 218–222.CrossRef
go back to reference Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. USA: Springer Publishing Company. Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. USA: Springer Publishing Company.
go back to reference Porges, S. (1993). Body perception questionnaire. Laboratory of Developmental Assessment: University of Maryland. Porges, S. (1993). Body perception questionnaire. Laboratory of Developmental Assessment: University of Maryland.
go back to reference Ring, C., & Brener, J. (1996). Influence of beliefs about heart rate and actual heart rate on heartbeat counting. Psychophysiology, 33(5), 541–546.CrossRef Ring, C., & Brener, J. (1996). Influence of beliefs about heart rate and actual heart rate on heartbeat counting. Psychophysiology, 33(5), 541–546.CrossRef
go back to reference Schachter, S., & Singer, J. (1962). Cognitive, social, and physiological determinants of emotional state. Psychological Review, 69(5), 379.CrossRef Schachter, S., & Singer, J. (1962). Cognitive, social, and physiological determinants of emotional state. Psychological Review, 69(5), 379.CrossRef
go back to reference Schandry, R. (1981). Heart beat perception and emotional experience. Psychophysiology, 18(4), 483–488.CrossRef Schandry, R. (1981). Heart beat perception and emotional experience. Psychophysiology, 18(4), 483–488.CrossRef
go back to reference Schultchen, D., Zaudig, M., Krauseneck, T., Berberich, G., & Pollatos, O. (2019). Interoceptive deficits in patients with obsessive–compulsive disorder in the time course of cognitive-behavioral therapy. PLoS ONE, 14(5), e0217237.CrossRef Schultchen, D., Zaudig, M., Krauseneck, T., Berberich, G., & Pollatos, O. (2019). Interoceptive deficits in patients with obsessive–compulsive disorder in the time course of cognitive-behavioral therapy. PLoS ONE, 14(5), e0217237.CrossRef
go back to reference Schwerdtfeger, A. R., Heene, S., & Messner, E. M. (2019). Interoceptive awareness andperceived control moderate the relationship between cognitive reappraisal, self-esteem, and cardiac activity in daily life. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 141, 84–92.CrossRef Schwerdtfeger, A. R., Heene, S., & Messner, E. M. (2019). Interoceptive awareness andperceived control moderate the relationship between cognitive reappraisal, self-esteem, and cardiac activity in daily life. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 141, 84–92.CrossRef
go back to reference Sherrington, C. S. (1909). The integrative action of the nervous system: CUP Archive. Sherrington, C. S. (1909). The integrative action of the nervous system: CUP Archive.
go back to reference Störmer, S. W., Heiligtag, U., & Knoll, J. F. (1989). Heartbeat detection and knowledge of results: a new method and some theoretical thoughts. Journal of Psychophysiology., 3(4), 409–417. Störmer, S. W., Heiligtag, U., & Knoll, J. F. (1989). Heartbeat detection and knowledge of results: a new method and some theoretical thoughts. Journal of Psychophysiology., 3(4), 409–417.
go back to reference Van der Does, A. J., Van Dyck, R., & Spinhoven, P. (1997). Accurate heartbeat perception in panic disorder: fact and artefact. Journal of Affective Disorders, 43(2), 121–130.CrossRef Van der Does, A. J., Van Dyck, R., & Spinhoven, P. (1997). Accurate heartbeat perception in panic disorder: fact and artefact. Journal of Affective Disorders, 43(2), 121–130.CrossRef
go back to reference Van Dyck, Z., Vögele, C., Blechert, J., Lutz, A. P., Schulz, A., & Herbert, B. M. (2016). The water load test as a measure of gastric interoception: development of a two-stage protocol and application to a healthy female population. PLoS ONE, 11(9), e0163574.CrossRef Van Dyck, Z., Vögele, C., Blechert, J., Lutz, A. P., Schulz, A., & Herbert, B. M. (2016). The water load test as a measure of gastric interoception: development of a two-stage protocol and application to a healthy female population. PLoS ONE, 11(9), e0163574.CrossRef
go back to reference Weiss, S., & Pollatos, O. (2014). Ocd and interoception. European Health Psychologist, 16(S), 325. Weiss, S., & Pollatos, O. (2014). Ocd and interoception. European Health Psychologist, 16(S), 325.
go back to reference Wiebking, C., Bauer, A., De Greck, M., Duncan, N. W., Tempelmann, C., & Northoff, G. (2010). Abnormal body perception and neural activity in the insula in depression: an fMRI study of the depressed “material me”. The World Journal of Biological Psychiatry, 11(3), 538–549.CrossRef Wiebking, C., Bauer, A., De Greck, M., Duncan, N. W., Tempelmann, C., & Northoff, G. (2010). Abnormal body perception and neural activity in the insula in depression: an fMRI study of the depressed “material me”. The World Journal of Biological Psychiatry, 11(3), 538–549.CrossRef
go back to reference Wiens, S. (2005). Interoception in emotional experience. Current Opinion in Neurology, 18(4), 442–447.CrossRef Wiens, S. (2005). Interoception in emotional experience. Current Opinion in Neurology, 18(4), 442–447.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Do interoceptive accuracy and interoceptive sensibility predict emotion regulation?
Auteurs
Stephanie A. Schuette
Nancy L. Zucker
Moria J. Smoski
Publicatiedatum
16-06-2020
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 5/2021
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-020-01369-2

Andere artikelen Uitgave 5/2021

Psychological Research 5/2021 Naar de uitgave