Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 3/2020

06-09-2018 | Original Article

The influence of threat on perceived spatial distance to out-group members

Auteurs: Chiara Fini, Pieter Verbeke, Sophie Sieber, Agnes Moors, Marcel Brass, Oliver Genschow

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 3/2020

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

A classic example of discriminatory behavior is keeping spatial distance from an out-group member. To explain this social behavior, the literature offers two alternative theoretical options that we label as the “threat hypothesis” and the “shared-experience hypothesis”. The former relies on studies showing that out-group members create a sense of alertness. Consequently, potentially threatening out-group members are represented as spatially close allowing the prevention of costly errors. The latter hypothesis suggests that the observation of out-group members reduces the sharing of somatosensory experiences and, thus, increases the perceived physical distance between oneself and others. In the present paper, we pitted the two hypotheses against each other. In Experiment 1, Caucasian participants expressed multiple implicit “Near/Far” spatial categorization judgments from a Black-African Avatar and a White-Caucasian Avatar located in a 3D environment. Results indicate that the Black-African Avatar was categorized as closer to oneself, as compared with the White-Caucasian Avatar, providing support for “the threat hypothesis”. In Experiment 2, we tested to which degree perceived threat contributes to this categorization bias by manipulating the avatar’s perceived threat orthogonally to group membership. The results indicate that irrespective of group membership, threatening avatars were categorized as being closer to oneself as compared with no threatening avatars. This suggests that provided information about a person and not the mere group membership influences perceived distance to the person.
Voetnoten
1
Lack of coherence means that participants did not express three consecutive “Near” judgments or three consecutive “Far” judgments, respectively in the descending/ascending series. That is, for example a “Near” judgment in a descending series, was followed again by a “Far” judgment, in such case the shift of judgment cannot be considered as being valid, since it is not stable and consistent.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Ackerman, J. M., Shapiro, J. R., Neuberg, S. L., Kenrick, D. T., Becker, D. V., Griskevicius, V., et al. (2006). They all look the same to me (unless they’re angry) from out-group homogeneity to out-group heterogeneity. Psychological Science, 17(10), 836–840.PubMedCrossRef Ackerman, J. M., Shapiro, J. R., Neuberg, S. L., Kenrick, D. T., Becker, D. V., Griskevicius, V., et al. (2006). They all look the same to me (unless they’re angry) from out-group homogeneity to out-group heterogeneity. Psychological Science, 17(10), 836–840.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Aiello, J. R. (1987). Human spatial behavior. Handbook of Environmental Psychology, 1(1987), 389–504. Aiello, J. R. (1987). Human spatial behavior. Handbook of Environmental Psychology, 1(1987), 389–504.
go back to reference Amodio, D. M., & Devine, P. G. (2006). Stereotyping and evaluation in implicit race bias: Evidence for independent constructs and unique effects on behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(4), 652.PubMedCrossRef Amodio, D. M., & Devine, P. G. (2006). Stereotyping and evaluation in implicit race bias: Evidence for independent constructs and unique effects on behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(4), 652.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Avenanti, A., Sirigu, A., & Aglioti, S. M. (2010). Racial bias reduces empathic sensorimotor resonance with other-race pain. Current Biology, 20(11), 1018–1022.PubMedCrossRef Avenanti, A., Sirigu, A., & Aglioti, S. M. (2010). Racial bias reduces empathic sensorimotor resonance with other-race pain. Current Biology, 20(11), 1018–1022.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bailenson, J. N., Blascovich, J., Beall, A. C., & Loomis, J. M. (2003). Interpersonal distance in immersive virtual environments. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29(7), 819–833.PubMedCrossRef Bailenson, J. N., Blascovich, J., Beall, A. C., & Loomis, J. M. (2003). Interpersonal distance in immersive virtual environments. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29(7), 819–833.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bernhardt, B. C., & Singer, T. (2012). The neural basis of empathy. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 35, 1–23.PubMedCrossRef Bernhardt, B. C., & Singer, T. (2012). The neural basis of empathy. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 35, 1–23.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Blanchard, R. J., & Blanchard, D. C. (1989). Attack and defense in rodents as ethoexperimental models for the study of emotion. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 13, S3–S14.PubMedCrossRef Blanchard, R. J., & Blanchard, D. C. (1989). Attack and defense in rodents as ethoexperimental models for the study of emotion. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 13, S3–S14.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bourgeois, P., & Hess, U. (2008). The impact of social context on mimicry. Biological Psychology, 77(3), 343–352.PubMedCrossRef Bourgeois, P., & Hess, U. (2008). The impact of social context on mimicry. Biological Psychology, 77(3), 343–352.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Buttelmann, D., Zmyj, N., Daum, M., & Carpenter, M. (2013). Selective imitation of in-group over out-group members in 14-month-old infants. Child Development, 84(2), 422–428.PubMedCrossRef Buttelmann, D., Zmyj, N., Daum, M., & Carpenter, M. (2013). Selective imitation of in-group over out-group members in 14-month-old infants. Child Development, 84(2), 422–428.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Coello, Y., Bourgeois, J., & Iachini, T. (2012). Embodied perception of reachable space: how do we manage threatening objects? Cognitive Processing, 13(1), 131–135.CrossRef Coello, Y., Bourgeois, J., & Iachini, T. (2012). Embodied perception of reachable space: how do we manage threatening objects? Cognitive Processing, 13(1), 131–135.CrossRef
go back to reference Coello, Y., & Iachini, T. (2015). Embodied perception of objects and people in space. Perceptual and Emotional Embodiment: Foundations of Embodied Cognition, 1, 198. Coello, Y., & Iachini, T. (2015). Embodied perception of objects and people in space. Perceptual and Emotional Embodiment: Foundations of Embodied Cognition, 1, 198.
go back to reference Coello, Y. E., & Fischer, M. H. (2016). Foundations of embodied cognition: Perceptual and emotional embodiment. Abingdon: Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group. Coello, Y. E., & Fischer, M. H. (2016). Foundations of embodied cognition: Perceptual and emotional embodiment. Abingdon: Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group.
go back to reference Cole, S., Balcetis, E., & Dunning, D. (2013). Affective signals of threat increase perceived proximity. Psychological Science, 24(1), 34–40.PubMedCrossRef Cole, S., Balcetis, E., & Dunning, D. (2013). Affective signals of threat increase perceived proximity. Psychological Science, 24(1), 34–40.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cuddy, A. J., Fiske, S. T., & Glick, P. (2008). Warmth and competence as universal dimensions of social perception: The stereotype content model and the BIAS map. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 61–149. Cuddy, A. J., Fiske, S. T., & Glick, P. (2008). Warmth and competence as universal dimensions of social perception: The stereotype content model and the BIAS map. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 61–149.
go back to reference De Vignemont, F., & Iannetti, G. D. (2015). How many peripersonal spaces? Neuropsychologia, 70, 327–334.PubMedCrossRef De Vignemont, F., & Iannetti, G. D. (2015). How many peripersonal spaces? Neuropsychologia, 70, 327–334.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Dotsch, R., & Wigboldus, D. H. (2008). Virtual prejudice. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(4), 1194–1198.CrossRef Dotsch, R., & Wigboldus, D. H. (2008). Virtual prejudice. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(4), 1194–1198.CrossRef
go back to reference Fazio, R. H., & Dunton, B. C. (1997). Categorization by race: The impact of automatic and controlled components of racial prejudice. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33(5), 451–470.CrossRef Fazio, R. H., & Dunton, B. C. (1997). Categorization by race: The impact of automatic and controlled components of racial prejudice. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33(5), 451–470.CrossRef
go back to reference Ferri, F., Tajadura-Jiménez, A., Väljamäe, A., Vastano, R., & Costantini, M. (2015). Emotion-inducing approaching sounds shape the boundaries of multisensory peripersonal space. Neuropsychologia, 70, 468–475.PubMedCrossRef Ferri, F., Tajadura-Jiménez, A., Väljamäe, A., Vastano, R., & Costantini, M. (2015). Emotion-inducing approaching sounds shape the boundaries of multisensory peripersonal space. Neuropsychologia, 70, 468–475.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7(2), 117–140.CrossRef Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7(2), 117–140.CrossRef
go back to reference Fini, C., Brass, M., & Committeri, G. (2015). Social scaling of extrapersonal space: target objects are judged as closer when the reference frame is a human agent with available movement potentialities. Cognition, 134, 50–56.PubMedCrossRef Fini, C., Brass, M., & Committeri, G. (2015). Social scaling of extrapersonal space: target objects are judged as closer when the reference frame is a human agent with available movement potentialities. Cognition, 134, 50–56.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fini, C., Costantini, M., & Committeri, G. (2014). Sharing space: The presence of other bodies extends the space judged as near. PLoS One, 9(12), e114719.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Fini, C., Costantini, M., & Committeri, G. (2014). Sharing space: The presence of other bodies extends the space judged as near. PLoS One, 9(12), e114719.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Fiske, A. P. (1992). The four elementary forms of sociality: Framework for a unified theory of social relations. Psychological Review, 99(4), 689.PubMedCrossRef Fiske, A. P. (1992). The four elementary forms of sociality: Framework for a unified theory of social relations. Psychological Review, 99(4), 689.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J., & Glick, P. (2007). Universal dimensions of social cognition: Warmth and competence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(2), 77–83.PubMedCrossRef Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J., & Glick, P. (2007). Universal dimensions of social cognition: Warmth and competence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(2), 77–83.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gallese, V., Keysers, C., & Rizzolatti, G. (2004). A unifying view of the basis of social cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(9), 396–403.PubMedCrossRef Gallese, V., Keysers, C., & Rizzolatti, G. (2004). A unifying view of the basis of social cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(9), 396–403.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Genschow, O., Florack, A., & Wänke, M. (2014). Recognition and approach responses toward threatening objects. Social Psychology, 45(2), 86–92.CrossRef Genschow, O., Florack, A., & Wänke, M. (2014). Recognition and approach responses toward threatening objects. Social Psychology, 45(2), 86–92.CrossRef
go back to reference Genschow, O., & Schindler, S. (2016). The influence of group membership on cross-contextual imitation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23(4), 1257–1265.CrossRef Genschow, O., & Schindler, S. (2016). The influence of group membership on cross-contextual imitation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23(4), 1257–1265.CrossRef
go back to reference Goff, P. A., Steele, C. M., & Davies, P. G. (2008). The space between us: Stereotype threat and distance in interracial contexts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(1), 91.PubMedCrossRef Goff, P. A., Steele, C. M., & Davies, P. G. (2008). The space between us: Stereotype threat and distance in interracial contexts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(1), 91.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gogel, W. C., & Tietz, J. D. (1977). Eye fixation and attention as modifiers of perceived distance. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 45(2), 343–362.PubMedCrossRef Gogel, W. C., & Tietz, J. D. (1977). Eye fixation and attention as modifiers of perceived distance. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 45(2), 343–362.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Green, M. C., & Brock, T. C. (2000). The role of transportation in the persuasiveness of public narratives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(5), 701.PubMedCrossRef Green, M. C., & Brock, T. C. (2000). The role of transportation in the persuasiveness of public narratives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(5), 701.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Green, M. C., Brock, T. C., & Kaufman, G. F. (2004). Understanding media enjoyment: The role of transportation into narrative worlds. Communication Theory, 14(4), 311–327.CrossRef Green, M. C., Brock, T. C., & Kaufman, G. F. (2004). Understanding media enjoyment: The role of transportation into narrative worlds. Communication Theory, 14(4), 311–327.CrossRef
go back to reference Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(6), 1464.PubMedCrossRef Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(6), 1464.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gutsell, J. N., & Inzlicht, M. (2010). Empathy constrained: Prejudice predicts reduced mental simulation of actions during observation of outgroups. Journal of experimental social psychology, 46(5), 841–845.CrossRef Gutsell, J. N., & Inzlicht, M. (2010). Empathy constrained: Prejudice predicts reduced mental simulation of actions during observation of outgroups. Journal of experimental social psychology, 46(5), 841–845.CrossRef
go back to reference Haselton, M. G., & Buss, D. M. (2000). Error management theory: A new perspective on biases in cross-sex mind reading. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(1), 81.PubMedCrossRef Haselton, M. G., & Buss, D. M. (2000). Error management theory: A new perspective on biases in cross-sex mind reading. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(1), 81.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hayduk, L. A. (1983). Personal space: Where we now stand. Psychological Bulletin, 94(2), 293.CrossRef Hayduk, L. A. (1983). Personal space: Where we now stand. Psychological Bulletin, 94(2), 293.CrossRef
go back to reference Iachini, T., Coello, Y., Frassinetti, F., & Ruggiero, G. (2014). Body space in social interactions: A comparison of reaching and comfort distance in immersive virtual reality. PLoS One, 9(11), e111511.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Iachini, T., Coello, Y., Frassinetti, F., & Ruggiero, G. (2014). Body space in social interactions: A comparison of reaching and comfort distance in immersive virtual reality. PLoS One, 9(11), e111511.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Iachini, T., Coello, Y., Frassinetti, F., Senese, V. P., Galante, F., & Ruggiero, G. (2016). Peripersonal and interpersonal space in virtual and real environments: Effects of gender and age. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 45, 154–164.CrossRef Iachini, T., Coello, Y., Frassinetti, F., Senese, V. P., Galante, F., & Ruggiero, G. (2016). Peripersonal and interpersonal space in virtual and real environments: Effects of gender and age. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 45, 154–164.CrossRef
go back to reference Iachini, T., Pagliaro, S., & Ruggiero, G. (2015). Near or far? It depends on my impression: Moral information and spatial behavior in virtual interactions. Acta Psychologica, 161, 131–136.PubMedCrossRef Iachini, T., Pagliaro, S., & Ruggiero, G. (2015). Near or far? It depends on my impression: Moral information and spatial behavior in virtual interactions. Acta Psychologica, 161, 131–136.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ito, T. A., Larsen, J. T., Smith, N. K., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1998). Negative information weighs more heavily on the brain: The negativity bias in evaluative categorizations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(4), 887.PubMedCrossRef Ito, T. A., Larsen, J. T., Smith, N. K., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1998). Negative information weighs more heavily on the brain: The negativity bias in evaluative categorizations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(4), 887.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ito, T. A., & Urland, G. R. (2003). Race and gender on the brain: Electrocortical measures of attention to the race and gender of multiply categorizable individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(4), 616.PubMedCrossRef Ito, T. A., & Urland, G. R. (2003). Race and gender on the brain: Electrocortical measures of attention to the race and gender of multiply categorizable individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(4), 616.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1997). International affective picture system (IAPS): Technical manual and affective ratings (pp. 39–58). NIMH Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention. Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1997). International affective picture system (IAPS): Technical manual and affective ratings (pp. 39–58). NIMH Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention.
go back to reference Levy, S. R., West, T. L., Bigler, R. S., Karafantis, D. M., Ramirez, L., & Velilla, E. (2005). Messages about the uniqueness and similarities of people: Impact on US Black and Latino youth. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 26(6), 714–733.CrossRef Levy, S. R., West, T. L., Bigler, R. S., Karafantis, D. M., Ramirez, L., & Velilla, E. (2005). Messages about the uniqueness and similarities of people: Impact on US Black and Latino youth. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 26(6), 714–733.CrossRef
go back to reference Lipp, O. V., & Waters, A. M. (2007). When danger lurks in the background: attentional capture by animal fear-relevant distractors is specific and selectively enhanced by animal fear. Emotion, 7(1), 192.PubMedCrossRef Lipp, O. V., & Waters, A. M. (2007). When danger lurks in the background: attentional capture by animal fear-relevant distractors is specific and selectively enhanced by animal fear. Emotion, 7(1), 192.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Macrae, C. N., Bodenhausen, G. V., Milne, A. B., & Jetten, J. (1994). Out of mind but back in sight: Stereotypes on the rebound. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(5), 808.CrossRef Macrae, C. N., Bodenhausen, G. V., Milne, A. B., & Jetten, J. (1994). Out of mind but back in sight: Stereotypes on the rebound. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(5), 808.CrossRef
go back to reference Mooney, K. M., Cohn, E. S., & Swift, M. B. (1992). Physical distance and AIDS: too close for comfort? Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 22(18), 1442–1452.CrossRef Mooney, K. M., Cohn, E. S., & Swift, M. B. (1992). Physical distance and AIDS: too close for comfort? Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 22(18), 1442–1452.CrossRef
go back to reference Nosek, B. A., Banaji, M. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (2002). Harvesting implicit group attitudes and beliefs from a demonstration web site. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 6(1), 101.CrossRef Nosek, B. A., Banaji, M. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (2002). Harvesting implicit group attitudes and beliefs from a demonstration web site. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 6(1), 101.CrossRef
go back to reference Öhman, A., Flykt, A., & Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: Detecting the snake in the grass. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130(3), 466.CrossRef Öhman, A., Flykt, A., & Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: Detecting the snake in the grass. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130(3), 466.CrossRef
go back to reference Park, R. E. (1928). Human migration and the marginal man. American Journal of Sociology, 33(6), 881–893.CrossRef Park, R. E. (1928). Human migration and the marginal man. American Journal of Sociology, 33(6), 881–893.CrossRef
go back to reference Phelps, E. A., O’Connor, K. J., Cunningham, W. A., Funayama, E. S., Gatenby, J. C., Gore, J. C., et al. (2000). Performance on indirect measures of race evaluation predicts amygdala activation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(5), 729–738.PubMedCrossRef Phelps, E. A., O’Connor, K. J., Cunningham, W. A., Funayama, E. S., Gatenby, J. C., Gore, J. C., et al. (2000). Performance on indirect measures of race evaluation predicts amygdala activation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(5), 729–738.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Quesque, F., Ruggiero, G., Mouta, S., Santos, J., Iachini, T., & Coello, Y. (2017). Keeping you at arm’s length: modifying peripersonal space influences interpersonal distance. Psychological Research, 81(4), 709–720.PubMedCrossRef Quesque, F., Ruggiero, G., Mouta, S., Santos, J., Iachini, T., & Coello, Y. (2017). Keeping you at arm’s length: modifying peripersonal space influences interpersonal distance. Psychological Research, 81(4), 709–720.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ruggiero, G., Frassinetti, F., Coello, Y., Rapuano, M., Di Cola, A. S., & Iachini, T. (2017). The effect of facial expressions on peripersonal and interpersonal spaces. Psychological Research, 81(6):1232–1240.PubMedCrossRef Ruggiero, G., Frassinetti, F., Coello, Y., Rapuano, M., Di Cola, A. S., & Iachini, T. (2017). The effect of facial expressions on peripersonal and interpersonal spaces. Psychological Research, 81(6):1232–1240.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Strack, F., & Deutsch, R. (2004). Reflective and impulsive determinants of social behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8(3), 220–247.PubMedCrossRef Strack, F., & Deutsch, R. (2004). Reflective and impulsive determinants of social behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8(3), 220–247.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Tajadura-Jiménez, A., Pantelidou, G., Rebacz, P., Västfjäll, D., & Tsakiris, M. (2011). I-space: the effects of emotional valence and source of music on interpersonal distance. PLoS One, 6(10), e26083.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Tajadura-Jiménez, A., Pantelidou, G., Rebacz, P., Västfjäll, D., & Tsakiris, M. (2011). I-space: the effects of emotional valence and source of music on interpersonal distance. PLoS One, 6(10), e26083.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Taylor, S. E., Fiske, S. T., Etcoff, N. L., & Ruderman, A. J. (1978). Categorical and contextual bases of person memory and stereotyping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36(7), 778.CrossRef Taylor, S. E., Fiske, S. T., Etcoff, N. L., & Ruderman, A. J. (1978). Categorical and contextual bases of person memory and stereotyping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36(7), 778.CrossRef
go back to reference Tipples, J., Young, A. W., Quinlan, P., Broks, P., & Ellis, A. W. (2002). Searching for threat. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section A, 55(3), 1007–1026.CrossRef Tipples, J., Young, A. W., Quinlan, P., Broks, P., & Ellis, A. W. (2002). Searching for threat. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section A, 55(3), 1007–1026.CrossRef
go back to reference Uzzell, D., & Horne, N. (2006). The influence of biological sex, sexuality and gender role on interpersonal distance. British Journal of Social Psychology, 45(3), 579–597.PubMedCrossRef Uzzell, D., & Horne, N. (2006). The influence of biological sex, sexuality and gender role on interpersonal distance. British Journal of Social Psychology, 45(3), 579–597.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Valdés-Conroy, B., Román, F. J., Hinojosa, J. A., & Shorkey, S. P. (2012). So far so good: Emotion in the peripersonal/extrapersonal space. PLoS One, 7(11), e49162.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Valdés-Conroy, B., Román, F. J., Hinojosa, J. A., & Shorkey, S. P. (2012). So far so good: Emotion in the peripersonal/extrapersonal space. PLoS One, 7(11), e49162.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Van Dantzig, S., Pecher, D., & Zwaan, R. A. (2008). Approach and avoidance as action effects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61(9), 1298–1306.CrossRef Van Dantzig, S., Pecher, D., & Zwaan, R. A. (2008). Approach and avoidance as action effects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61(9), 1298–1306.CrossRef
go back to reference Wist, E. R., & Summons, E. (1976). Spatial and fixation conditions affecting the temporal course of changes in perceived relative distance. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 39(2), 99–112.PubMedCrossRef Wist, E. R., & Summons, E. (1976). Spatial and fixation conditions affecting the temporal course of changes in perceived relative distance. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 39(2), 99–112.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Xiao, Y. J., & Van Bavel, J. J. (2012). See your friends close and your enemies closer: Social identity and identity threat shape the representation of physical distance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38(7), 959–972.PubMedCrossRef Xiao, Y. J., & Van Bavel, J. J. (2012). See your friends close and your enemies closer: Social identity and identity threat shape the representation of physical distance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38(7), 959–972.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Xiao, Y. J., Wohl, M. J., & Van Bavel, J. J. (2016). Proximity under threat: The role of physical distance in intergroup relations. PLoS One, 11(7), e0159792.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Xiao, Y. J., Wohl, M. J., & Van Bavel, J. J. (2016). Proximity under threat: The role of physical distance in intergroup relations. PLoS One, 11(7), e0159792.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Yabar, Y., Johnston, L., Miles, L., & Peace, V. (2006). Implicit behavioral mimicry: Investigating the impact of group membership. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 30(3), 97–113.CrossRef Yabar, Y., Johnston, L., Miles, L., & Peace, V. (2006). Implicit behavioral mimicry: Investigating the impact of group membership. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 30(3), 97–113.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
The influence of threat on perceived spatial distance to out-group members
Auteurs
Chiara Fini
Pieter Verbeke
Sophie Sieber
Agnes Moors
Marcel Brass
Oliver Genschow
Publicatiedatum
06-09-2018
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 3/2020
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1091-7

Andere artikelen Uitgave 3/2020

Psychological Research 3/2020 Naar de uitgave