Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 3/2019

29-08-2017 | Original Article

Set size influences the relationship between ANS acuity and math performance: a result of different strategies?

Auteurs: Julia Felicitas Dietrich, Hans-Christoph Nuerk, Elise Klein, Korbinian Moeller, Stefan Huber

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 3/2019

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Previous research has proposed that the approximate number system (ANS) constitutes a building block for later mathematical abilities. Therefore, numerous studies investigated the relationship between ANS acuity and mathematical performance, but results are inconsistent. Properties of the experimental design have been discussed as a potential explanation of these inconsistencies. In the present study, we investigated the influence of set size and presentation duration on the association between non-symbolic magnitude comparison and math performance. Moreover, we focused on strategies reported as an explanation for these inconsistencies. In particular, we employed a non-symbolic magnitude comparison task and asked participants how they solved the task. We observed that set size was a significant moderator of the relationship between non-symbolic magnitude comparison and math performance, whereas presentation duration of the stimuli did not moderate this relationship. This supports the notion that specific design characteristics contribute to the inconsistent results. Moreover, participants reported different strategies including numerosity-based, visual, counting, calculation-based, and subitizing strategies. Frequencies of these strategies differed between different set sizes and presentation durations. However, we found no specific strategy, which alone predicted arithmetic performance, but when considering the frequency of all reported strategies, arithmetic performance could be predicted. Visual strategies made the largest contribution to this prediction. To conclude, the present findings suggest that different design characteristics contribute to the inconsistent findings regarding the relationship between non-symbolic magnitude comparison and mathematical performance by inducing different strategies and additional processes.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Literatuur
go back to reference Agrillo, C., Piffer, L., & Adriano, A. (2013). Individual differences in non-symbolic numerical abilities predict mathematical achievements but contradict ATOM. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 9(1), 26.CrossRef Agrillo, C., Piffer, L., & Adriano, A. (2013). Individual differences in non-symbolic numerical abilities predict mathematical achievements but contradict ATOM. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 9(1), 26.CrossRef
go back to reference Amthauer, R., Brocke, B., Liepmann, D., & Beauducel, A. (2007). I-S-T 2000 R: Intelligenz-Struktur-Test 2000 R. Göttingen: Hogrefe. Amthauer, R., Brocke, B., Liepmann, D., & Beauducel, A. (2007). I-S-T 2000 R: Intelligenz-Struktur-Test 2000 R. Göttingen: Hogrefe.
go back to reference Anobile, G., Turi, M., Cicchini, G., & Burr, D. (2015). Mechanisms for perception of numerosity or texture-density are governed by crowding-like effects. Journal of Vision, 15, 4.CrossRef Anobile, G., Turi, M., Cicchini, G., & Burr, D. (2015). Mechanisms for perception of numerosity or texture-density are governed by crowding-like effects. Journal of Vision, 15, 4.CrossRef
go back to reference Ansari, D. (2012). Why the “symbol-grounding problem” for number symbols is still problematic. Current Anthropology, 53(2), 212–213. doi:10.1086/664818.CrossRef Ansari, D. (2012). Why the “symbol-grounding problem” for number symbols is still problematic. Current Anthropology, 53(2), 212–213. doi:10.​1086/​664818.CrossRef
go back to reference Assel, M. A., Landry, S. H., Swank, P., Smith, K. E., & Steelman, L. M. (2003). Precursors to mathematical skills: Examining the roles of visual-spatial skills, executive processes, and parenting factors. Applied Developmental Science, 7(1), 27–38. doi:10.1207/S1532480XADS0701_3.CrossRef Assel, M. A., Landry, S. H., Swank, P., Smith, K. E., & Steelman, L. M. (2003). Precursors to mathematical skills: Examining the roles of visual-spatial skills, executive processes, and parenting factors. Applied Developmental Science, 7(1), 27–38. doi:10.​1207/​S1532480XADS0701​_​3.CrossRef
go back to reference Baayen, R. H., & Milin, P. (2010). Analyzing reaction times. International Journal of Psychological Research, 3(2), 12–28.CrossRef Baayen, R. H., & Milin, P. (2010). Analyzing reaction times. International Journal of Psychological Research, 3(2), 12–28.CrossRef
go back to reference Bartelet, D., Vaessen, A., Blomert, L., & Ansari, D. (2014). What basic number processing measures in kindergarten explain unique variability in first-grade arithmetic proficiency? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 117(1), 12–28. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2013.08.010.CrossRefPubMed Bartelet, D., Vaessen, A., Blomert, L., & Ansari, D. (2014). What basic number processing measures in kindergarten explain unique variability in first-grade arithmetic proficiency? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 117(1), 12–28. doi:10.​1016/​j.​jecp.​2013.​08.​010.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society. Series B (Methodological), 57(1), 289–300. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/stable/2346101. Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society. Series B (Methodological), 57(1), 289–300. Retrieved from http://​www.​jstor.​org/​stable/​2346101.
go back to reference Bull, R., & Scerif, G. (2001). Executive functioning as a predictor of children’s mathematics ability: Inhibition, switching, and working memory. Developmental Neuropsychology, 19(3), 273–293.CrossRef Bull, R., & Scerif, G. (2001). Executive functioning as a predictor of children’s mathematics ability: Inhibition, switching, and working memory. Developmental Neuropsychology, 19(3), 273–293.CrossRef
go back to reference Clayton, S., Gilmore, C., & Inglis, M. (2015). Dot comparison stimuli are not all alike: The effect of different visual controls on ANS measurement. Acta Psychologica, 161, 177–184. Clayton, S., Gilmore, C., & Inglis, M. (2015). Dot comparison stimuli are not all alike: The effect of different visual controls on ANS measurement. Acta Psychologica, 161, 177–184.
go back to reference De Oliveira Ferreira, F., Wood, G., Pinheiro-Chagas, P., Lonnemann, J., Krinzinger, H., Willmes, K., & Haase, V. G. (2012). Explaining school mathematics performance from symbolic and nonsymbolic magnitude processing: Similarities and differences between typical and low-achieving children. Psychology and Neuroscience, 5(1), 37–46.CrossRef De Oliveira Ferreira, F., Wood, G., Pinheiro-Chagas, P., Lonnemann, J., Krinzinger, H., Willmes, K., & Haase, V. G. (2012). Explaining school mathematics performance from symbolic and nonsymbolic magnitude processing: Similarities and differences between typical and low-achieving children. Psychology and Neuroscience, 5(1), 37–46.CrossRef
go back to reference De Smedt, B., Noël, M.-P., Gilmore, C., & Ansari, D. (2013). How do symbolic and non-symbolic numerical magnitude processing skills relate to individual differences in children’s mathematical skills? A review of evidence from brain and behavior. Trends in Neuroscience and Education, 2(2), 48–55. doi:10.1016/j.tine.2013.06.001.CrossRef De Smedt, B., Noël, M.-P., Gilmore, C., & Ansari, D. (2013). How do symbolic and non-symbolic numerical magnitude processing skills relate to individual differences in children’s mathematical skills? A review of evidence from brain and behavior. Trends in Neuroscience and Education, 2(2), 48–55. doi:10.​1016/​j.​tine.​2013.​06.​001.CrossRef
go back to reference Dietrich, J. F., Huber, S., Klein, E., Willmes, K., Pixner, S., & Moeller, K. (2016). A systematic investigation of accuracy and response time based measures used to index ANS acuity. PLoS One, 11(9), e0163076.CrossRef Dietrich, J. F., Huber, S., Klein, E., Willmes, K., Pixner, S., & Moeller, K. (2016). A systematic investigation of accuracy and response time based measures used to index ANS acuity. PLoS One, 11(9), e0163076.CrossRef
go back to reference Durgin, F. H. (1995). Texture density adaptation and the perceived numerosity and distribution of texture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 149–169. Durgin, F. H. (1995). Texture density adaptation and the perceived numerosity and distribution of texture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 149–169.
go back to reference Espy, K. A., McDiarmid, M. M., Cwik, M. F., Stalets, M. M., Hamby, A., & Stern, T. E. (2004). The contribution of executive functions to emergent mathematic skills in preschool children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 26(1), 465–486.CrossRef Espy, K. A., McDiarmid, M. M., Cwik, M. F., Stalets, M. M., Hamby, A., & Stern, T. E. (2004). The contribution of executive functions to emergent mathematic skills in preschool children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 26(1), 465–486.CrossRef
go back to reference Fazio, L. K., Bailey, D. H., Thompson, C. A., & Siegler, R. S. (2014). Relations of different types of numerical magnitude representations to each other and to mathematics achievement. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 123(1), 53–72. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2014.01.013.CrossRefPubMed Fazio, L. K., Bailey, D. H., Thompson, C. A., & Siegler, R. S. (2014). Relations of different types of numerical magnitude representations to each other and to mathematics achievement. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 123(1), 53–72. doi:10.​1016/​j.​jecp.​2014.​01.​013.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Fuhs, M. W., & McNeil, N. M. (2013). ANS acuity and mathematics ability in preschoolers from low-income homes: Contributions of inhibitory control. Developmental Science, 16(1), 136–148. doi:10.1111/desc.12013.CrossRefPubMed Fuhs, M. W., & McNeil, N. M. (2013). ANS acuity and mathematics ability in preschoolers from low-income homes: Contributions of inhibitory control. Developmental Science, 16(1), 136–148. doi:10.​1111/​desc.​12013.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Gebuis, T., & Reynvoet, B. (2012). The interplay between nonsymbolic number and its continuous visual properties. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141(4), 642–648. doi:10.1037/a0026218.CrossRef Gebuis, T., & Reynvoet, B. (2012). The interplay between nonsymbolic number and its continuous visual properties. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141(4), 642–648. doi:10.​1037/​a0026218.CrossRef
go back to reference Gilmore, C., Attridge, N., Clayton, S., Cragg, L., Johnson, S., Marlow, N., & Inglis, M. (2013). Individual differences in inhibitory control, not non-verbal number acuity, correlate with mathematics achievement. PLoS One, 8(6), 1–9. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0067374.CrossRef Gilmore, C., Attridge, N., Clayton, S., Cragg, L., Johnson, S., Marlow, N., & Inglis, M. (2013). Individual differences in inhibitory control, not non-verbal number acuity, correlate with mathematics achievement. PLoS One, 8(6), 1–9. doi:10.​1371/​journal.​pone.​0067374.CrossRef
go back to reference Guay, R. B., & McDaniel, E. D. (1977). The relationship between mathematics achievement and spatial abilities among elementary school children. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 8(3), 211–215. doi:10.2307/748522.CrossRef Guay, R. B., & McDaniel, E. D. (1977). The relationship between mathematics achievement and spatial abilities among elementary school children. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 8(3), 211–215. doi:10.​2307/​748522.CrossRef
go back to reference Gunderson, E. A., Ramirez, G., Beilock, S. L., & Levine, S. C. (2012). The relation between spatial skill and early number knowledge: The role of the linear number line. Developmental Psychology, 48(5), 1229–1241. doi:10.1037/a0027433.CrossRefPubMed Gunderson, E. A., Ramirez, G., Beilock, S. L., & Levine, S. C. (2012). The relation between spatial skill and early number knowledge: The role of the linear number line. Developmental Psychology, 48(5), 1229–1241. doi:10.​1037/​a0027433.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Halberda, J., Ly, R., Wilmer, J. B., Naiman, D. Q., & Germine, L. (2012). Number sense across the lifespan as revealed by a massive Internet-based sample. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(28), 11116–11120. doi:10.1073/pnas.1200196109.CrossRef Halberda, J., Ly, R., Wilmer, J. B., Naiman, D. Q., & Germine, L. (2012). Number sense across the lifespan as revealed by a massive Internet-based sample. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(28), 11116–11120. doi:10.​1073/​pnas.​1200196109.CrossRef
go back to reference Inglis, M., Attridge, N., Batchelor, S., & Gilmore, C. (2011). Non-verbal number acuity correlates with symbolic mathematics achievement: But only in children. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(6), 1222–1229. doi:10.3758/s13423-011-0154-1.CrossRef Inglis, M., Attridge, N., Batchelor, S., & Gilmore, C. (2011). Non-verbal number acuity correlates with symbolic mathematics achievement: But only in children. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(6), 1222–1229. doi:10.​3758/​s13423-011-0154-1.CrossRef
go back to reference James, G., Witten, D., Hastie, T., & Tibshirani, R. (2013). An introduction to statistical learning (Vol. 6). New York: Springer.CrossRef James, G., Witten, D., Hastie, T., & Tibshirani, R. (2013). An introduction to statistical learning (Vol. 6). New York: Springer.CrossRef
go back to reference Luwel, K., & Verschaffel, L. (2003). Adapting strategy choices to situational factors: The effect of time pressure on children’s numerosity judgement strategies. Psychologica Belgica, 43, 269–295. Luwel, K., & Verschaffel, L. (2003). Adapting strategy choices to situational factors: The effect of time pressure on children’s numerosity judgement strategies. Psychologica Belgica, 43, 269–295.
go back to reference Luwel, K., Verschaffel, L., Onghena, P., & De Corte, E. (2003a). Flexibility in strategy use: Adaptation of numerosity judgement strategies to task characteristics. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 15(2), 247–266. doi:10.1080/09541440244000139.CrossRef Luwel, K., Verschaffel, L., Onghena, P., & De Corte, E. (2003a). Flexibility in strategy use: Adaptation of numerosity judgement strategies to task characteristics. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 15(2), 247–266. doi:10.​1080/​0954144024400013​9.CrossRef
go back to reference Lyons, I. M., Nuerk, H.-C., & Ansari, D. (2015b). Rethinking the implications of numerical ratio effects for understanding the development of representational precision and numerical processing across formats. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144(5), 1021–1035. doi:10.1037/xge0000094.CrossRef Lyons, I. M., Nuerk, H.-C., & Ansari, D. (2015b). Rethinking the implications of numerical ratio effects for understanding the development of representational precision and numerical processing across formats. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144(5), 1021–1035. doi:10.​1037/​xge0000094.CrossRef
go back to reference Nieder, A. (2011). The neural code for number. In S. Dehaene & E. M. Brannon (Eds.), Space, time and number in the brain: Searching for the foundations of mathematical thought (pp. 103–118). London: Academic Press.CrossRef Nieder, A. (2011). The neural code for number. In S. Dehaene & E. M. Brannon (Eds.), Space, time and number in the brain: Searching for the foundations of mathematical thought (pp. 103–118). London: Academic Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Nieder, A. (2013). Coding of abstract quantity by “number neurons” of the primate brain. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 199(1), 1–16.CrossRef Nieder, A. (2013). Coding of abstract quantity by “number neurons” of the primate brain. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 199(1), 1–16.CrossRef
go back to reference Nieder, A., Freedman, D. J., & Miller, E. K. (2002). Representation of the quantity of visual items in the primate prefrontal cortex. Science, 297(5587), 1708–1711.CrossRef Nieder, A., Freedman, D. J., & Miller, E. K. (2002). Representation of the quantity of visual items in the primate prefrontal cortex. Science, 297(5587), 1708–1711.CrossRef
go back to reference Piazza, M., Izard, V., Pinel, P., Le Bihan, D., & Dehaene, S. (2004). Tuning curves for approximate numerosity in the human intraparietal sulcus. Neuron 44(3), 547–555CrossRef Piazza, M., Izard, V., Pinel, P., Le Bihan, D., & Dehaene, S. (2004). Tuning curves for approximate numerosity in the human intraparietal sulcus. Neuron 44(3), 547–555CrossRef
go back to reference Price, G. R., Palmer, D., Battista, C., & Ansari, D. (2012). Nonsymbolic numerical magnitude comparison: Reliability and validity of different task variants and outcome measures, and their relationship to arithmetic achievement in adults. Acta Psychologica, 140(1), 50–57. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2012.02.008.CrossRefPubMed Price, G. R., Palmer, D., Battista, C., & Ansari, D. (2012). Nonsymbolic numerical magnitude comparison: Reliability and validity of different task variants and outcome measures, and their relationship to arithmetic achievement in adults. Acta Psychologica, 140(1), 50–57. doi:10.​1016/​j.​actpsy.​2012.​02.​008.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Schneider, M., Beeres, K., Coban, L., Merz, S., Susan Schmidt, S., Stricker, J., & De Smedt, B. (2016). Associations of non-symbolic and symbolic numerical magnitude processing with mathematical competence: A meta-analysis. Developmental Science. doi:10.1111/desc.12372.CrossRefPubMed Schneider, M., Beeres, K., Coban, L., Merz, S., Susan Schmidt, S., Stricker, J., & De Smedt, B. (2016). Associations of non-symbolic and symbolic numerical magnitude processing with mathematical competence: A meta-analysis. Developmental Science. doi:10.​1111/​desc.​12372.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Smets, K., Sasanguie, D., Szücs, D., & Reynvoet, B. (2015). The effect of different methods to construct non-symbolic stimuli in numerosity estimation and comparison. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 27(3), 310–325. doi:10.1080/20445911.2014.996568.CrossRef Smets, K., Sasanguie, D., Szücs, D., & Reynvoet, B. (2015). The effect of different methods to construct non-symbolic stimuli in numerosity estimation and comparison. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 27(3), 310–325. doi:10.​1080/​20445911.​2014.​996568.CrossRef
go back to reference Soltész, F., Szucs, D., & Szucs, L. (2010). Relationships between magnitude representation, counting and memory in 4- to 7-year-old children: a developmental study. Behavioral and Brain Functions: BBF, 6, 13. doi:10.1186/1744-9081-6-13.CrossRefPubMed Soltész, F., Szucs, D., & Szucs, L. (2010). Relationships between magnitude representation, counting and memory in 4- to 7-year-old children: a developmental study. Behavioral and Brain Functions: BBF, 6, 13. doi:10.​1186/​1744-9081-6-13.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference St Clair-Thompson, H. L., & Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Executive functions and achievements in school: Shifting, updating, inhibition, and working memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59(4), 745–759. doi:10.1080/17470210500162854.CrossRefPubMed St Clair-Thompson, H. L., & Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Executive functions and achievements in school: Shifting, updating, inhibition, and working memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59(4), 745–759. doi:10.​1080/​1747021050016285​4.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference van den Berg, B. A., Reinders, M. J. T., de Ridder, D., & de Beer, T. A. P. (2015). Insight into neutral and disease-associated human genetic variants through interpretable predictors. PLoS One, 10(3), 1–17. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0120729.CrossRef van den Berg, B. A., Reinders, M. J. T., de Ridder, D., & de Beer, T. A. P. (2015). Insight into neutral and disease-associated human genetic variants through interpretable predictors. PLoS One, 10(3), 1–17. doi:10.​1371/​journal.​pone.​0120729.CrossRef
go back to reference van Marle, K., Chu, F. W., Li, Y., & Geary, D. C. (2014). Acuity of the approximate number system and preschoolers’ quantitative development. Developmental Science, 17(4), 492–505. doi:10.1111/desc.12143.CrossRefPubMed van Marle, K., Chu, F. W., Li, Y., & Geary, D. C. (2014). Acuity of the approximate number system and preschoolers’ quantitative development. Developmental Science, 17(4), 492–505. doi:10.​1111/​desc.​12143.CrossRefPubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Set size influences the relationship between ANS acuity and math performance: a result of different strategies?
Auteurs
Julia Felicitas Dietrich
Hans-Christoph Nuerk
Elise Klein
Korbinian Moeller
Stefan Huber
Publicatiedatum
29-08-2017
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 3/2019
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-017-0907-1

Andere artikelen Uitgave 3/2019

Psychological Research 3/2019 Naar de uitgave