Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 5/2019

09-08-2017 | Original Article

Whose turn is it anyway? The moderating role of response-execution certainty on the joint Simon effect

Auteurs: April Karlinsky, Melanie Y. Lam, Romeo Chua, Nicola J. Hodges

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 5/2019

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

When a two-choice “Simon task” is distributed between two people, performance in the shared go/no-go task resembles performance in the whole task alone. This finding has been described as the joint Simon effect (JSE). Unlike the individual go/no-go task, not only is the typical joint Simon task shared with another person, but also the imperative stimuli dictate whose turn it is to respond. Therefore, in the current study, we asked whether removing the agent discrimination component of the joint Simon task influences co-representation. Participants performed the typical joint Simon task, which was compared to two turn-taking versions of the task. For these turn-taking tasks, pairs predictably alternated turns on consecutive trials, with their respective imperative stimulus presented either on 100% of their turns (fully predictable group) or on 83% of their turns (response-uncertainty group, 17% no-go catch trials). The JSE was absent in the fully predictable, turn-taking task, but emerged similarly under the response-uncertainty condition and the typical joint Simon task condition where there is both turn and response-execution-related uncertainty. These results demonstrate that conflict related to agent discrimination is likely not a critical factor driving the JSE, whereas conflict surrounding the need to execute a response (and hence the degree of preparation) appears fundamental to co-representation.
Voetnoten
1
All three left-handed participants were female. One responded with her dominant left hand while the other two responded with their right hand.
 
2
The erroneous responses to catch trials were distributed across the experiment, with 26 responses in Block 1 (33.3% of errors), 14 responses in Block 2 (18.0% of errors), 21 responses in Block 3 (26.9% of errors), and 17 responses in Block 4 (21.8% of errors).
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Atmaca, S., Sebanz, N., & Knoblich, G. (2011). The joint flanker effect: Sharing tasks with real and imagined co-actors. Experimental Brain Research, 211, 371–385.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Atmaca, S., Sebanz, N., & Knoblich, G. (2011). The joint flanker effect: Sharing tasks with real and imagined co-actors. Experimental Brain Research, 211, 371–385.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Atmaca, S., Sebanz, N., Prinz, W., & Knoblich, G. (2008). Action co-representation: The joint SNARC effect. Social Neuroscience, 3(3–4), 410–420.CrossRefPubMed Atmaca, S., Sebanz, N., Prinz, W., & Knoblich, G. (2008). Action co-representation: The joint SNARC effect. Social Neuroscience, 3(3–4), 410–420.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Baess, P., & Prinz, W. (2015). My partner is also on my mind: Social context modulates the N1 response. Experimental Brain Research, 233(1), 105–113.CrossRefPubMed Baess, P., & Prinz, W. (2015). My partner is also on my mind: Social context modulates the N1 response. Experimental Brain Research, 233(1), 105–113.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Böckler, A., Knoblich, G., & Sebanz, N. (2012). Effects of a coactor’s focus of attention on task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 38(6), 1404–1405.CrossRefPubMed Böckler, A., Knoblich, G., & Sebanz, N. (2012). Effects of a coactor’s focus of attention on task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 38(6), 1404–1405.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Böckler, A., & Sebanz, N. (2012). A co-actor’s focus of attention affects stimulus processing and task performance: An ERP study. Social Neuroscience, 7(6), 565–577.CrossRefPubMed Böckler, A., & Sebanz, N. (2012). A co-actor’s focus of attention affects stimulus processing and task performance: An ERP study. Social Neuroscience, 7(6), 565–577.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Carlsen, A. N., Chua, R., Dakin, C. J., Sanderson, D. J., Inglis, J. T., & Franks, I. M. (2008). Startle reveals an absence of advance motor programming in a go/no-go task. Neuroscience Letters, 434, 61–65.CrossRefPubMed Carlsen, A. N., Chua, R., Dakin, C. J., Sanderson, D. J., Inglis, J. T., & Franks, I. M. (2008). Startle reveals an absence of advance motor programming in a go/no-go task. Neuroscience Letters, 434, 61–65.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Carlsen, A. N., Chua, R., Inglis, J. T., Sanderson, D. J., & Franks, I. M. (2004). Can prepared responses be stored subcortically? Experimental Brain Research, 159(3), 301–309.CrossRefPubMed Carlsen, A. N., Chua, R., Inglis, J. T., Sanderson, D. J., & Franks, I. M. (2004). Can prepared responses be stored subcortically? Experimental Brain Research, 159(3), 301–309.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Dittrich, K., Bossert, M.-L., Rothe-Wulf, A., & Klauer, K. C. (2016). The joint flanker effect and the joint Simon effect: On the comparability of processes underlying joint compatibility effects. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70(9), 1808–1823.CrossRefPubMed Dittrich, K., Bossert, M.-L., Rothe-Wulf, A., & Klauer, K. C. (2016). The joint flanker effect and the joint Simon effect: On the comparability of processes underlying joint compatibility effects. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70(9), 1808–1823.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Dolk, T., Hommel, B., Colzato, L. S., Schütz-Bosbach, S., Prinz, W., & Liepelt, R. (2011). How “social” is the social Simon effect? Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 1–9.CrossRef Dolk, T., Hommel, B., Colzato, L. S., Schütz-Bosbach, S., Prinz, W., & Liepelt, R. (2011). How “social” is the social Simon effect? Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 1–9.CrossRef
go back to reference Dolk, T., Hommel, B., Colzato, L. S., Schütz-Bosbach, S., Prinz, W., & Liepelt, R. (2014a). The joint Simon effect: A review and theoretical integration. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1–10.CrossRef Dolk, T., Hommel, B., Colzato, L. S., Schütz-Bosbach, S., Prinz, W., & Liepelt, R. (2014a). The joint Simon effect: A review and theoretical integration. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1–10.CrossRef
go back to reference Dolk, T., Hommel, B., Prinz, W., & Liepelt, R. (2013). The (not so) social Simon effect: A referential coding account. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 39(5), 1248–1260.CrossRefPubMed Dolk, T., Hommel, B., Prinz, W., & Liepelt, R. (2013). The (not so) social Simon effect: A referential coding account. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 39(5), 1248–1260.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Dolk, T., Hommel, B., Prinz, W., & Liepelt, R. (2014b). The joint flanker effect: Less social than previously thought. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 21(5), 1224–1230.CrossRefPubMed Dolk, T., Hommel, B., Prinz, W., & Liepelt, R. (2014b). The joint flanker effect: Less social than previously thought. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 21(5), 1224–1230.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Elekes, F., Bródy, G., Halász, E., & Király, I. (2016). Enhanced encoding of the co-actor’s target stimuli during a shared non-motor task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69(12), 2376–2389.CrossRefPubMed Elekes, F., Bródy, G., Halász, E., & Király, I. (2016). Enhanced encoding of the co-actor’s target stimuli during a shared non-motor task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69(12), 2376–2389.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Eskenazi, T., Doerrfeld, A., Logan, G. D., Knoblich, G., & Sebanz, N. (2013). Your words are my words: Effects of acting together on encoding. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(5), 1026–1034.CrossRefPubMed Eskenazi, T., Doerrfeld, A., Logan, G. D., Knoblich, G., & Sebanz, N. (2013). Your words are my words: Effects of acting together on encoding. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(5), 1026–1034.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Guagnano, D., Rusconi, E., & Umiltà, C. A. (2010). Sharing a task or sharing space? On the effect of the confederate in action coding in a detection task. Cognition, 114(3), 348–355.CrossRefPubMed Guagnano, D., Rusconi, E., & Umiltà, C. A. (2010). Sharing a task or sharing space? On the effect of the confederate in action coding in a detection task. Cognition, 114(3), 348–355.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hommel, B., Colzato, L. S., & van den Wildenberg, W. P. M. (2009). How social are task representations? Psychological Science, 20(7), 794–798.CrossRefPubMed Hommel, B., Colzato, L. S., & van den Wildenberg, W. P. M. (2009). How social are task representations? Psychological Science, 20(7), 794–798.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Iani, C., Anelli, F., Nicoletti, R., Arcuri, L., & Rubichi, S. (2011). The role of group membership on the modulation of joint action. Experimental Brain Research, 211(3–4), 439–445.CrossRefPubMed Iani, C., Anelli, F., Nicoletti, R., Arcuri, L., & Rubichi, S. (2011). The role of group membership on the modulation of joint action. Experimental Brain Research, 211(3–4), 439–445.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Iani, C., Anelli, F., Nicoletti, R., & Rubichi, S. (2014). The carry-over effect of competition in task-sharing: Evidence from the joint Simon task. PLoS One, 9(6), 1–8.CrossRef Iani, C., Anelli, F., Nicoletti, R., & Rubichi, S. (2014). The carry-over effect of competition in task-sharing: Evidence from the joint Simon task. PLoS One, 9(6), 1–8.CrossRef
go back to reference Lam, M. Y., & Chua, R. (2010). Influence of stimulus-response assignment on the joint-action correspondence effect. Psychological Research, 74(5), 476–480.CrossRefPubMed Lam, M. Y., & Chua, R. (2010). Influence of stimulus-response assignment on the joint-action correspondence effect. Psychological Research, 74(5), 476–480.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Lu, C.-H., & Proctor, R. W. (1995). The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 2(2), 174–207.CrossRefPubMed Lu, C.-H., & Proctor, R. W. (1995). The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 2(2), 174–207.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Maslovat, D., Carlsen, A. N., & Franks, I. M. (2012). Subcortical motor circuit excitability during simple and choice reaction time. Behavioral Neuroscience, 126(3), 499–503.CrossRefPubMed Maslovat, D., Carlsen, A. N., & Franks, I. M. (2012). Subcortical motor circuit excitability during simple and choice reaction time. Behavioral Neuroscience, 126(3), 499–503.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference McClung, J. S., Jentzsch, I., & Reicher, S. D. (2013). Group membership affects spontaneous mental representation: Failure to represent the out-group in a joint action task. PLoS One, 8(11), 1–9.CrossRef McClung, J. S., Jentzsch, I., & Reicher, S. D. (2013). Group membership affects spontaneous mental representation: Failure to represent the out-group in a joint action task. PLoS One, 8(11), 1–9.CrossRef
go back to reference Milanese, N., Iani, C., Sebanz, N., & Rubichi, S. (2011). Contextual determinants of the social-transfer-of-learning effect. Experimental Brain Research, 211(3–4), 415–422.CrossRefPubMed Milanese, N., Iani, C., Sebanz, N., & Rubichi, S. (2011). Contextual determinants of the social-transfer-of-learning effect. Experimental Brain Research, 211(3–4), 415–422.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Müller, B. C. N., Kühn, S., van Baaren, R. B., Dotsch, R., Brass, M., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2011). Perspective taking eliminates differences in co-representation of out-group members’ actions. Experimental Brain Research, 211(3–4), 423–428.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Müller, B. C. N., Kühn, S., van Baaren, R. B., Dotsch, R., Brass, M., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2011). Perspective taking eliminates differences in co-representation of out-group members’ actions. Experimental Brain Research, 211(3–4), 423–428.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Philipp, A. M., & Prinz, W. (2010). Evidence for a role of the responding agent in the joint compatibility effect. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63(11), 2159–2171.CrossRefPubMed Philipp, A. M., & Prinz, W. (2010). Evidence for a role of the responding agent in the joint compatibility effect. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63(11), 2159–2171.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Sebanz, N., Knoblich, G., & Prinz, W. (2003). Representing others’ actions: Just like one’s own? Cognition, 88, B11–B21.CrossRefPubMed Sebanz, N., Knoblich, G., & Prinz, W. (2003). Representing others’ actions: Just like one’s own? Cognition, 88, B11–B21.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Sebanz, N., Rebbechi, D., Knoblich, G., Prinz, W., & Frith, C. (2007). Is it really my turn? An event-related fMRI study of task sharing. Social Neuroscience, 2(2), 81–95.CrossRefPubMed Sebanz, N., Rebbechi, D., Knoblich, G., Prinz, W., & Frith, C. (2007). Is it really my turn? An event-related fMRI study of task sharing. Social Neuroscience, 2(2), 81–95.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Simon, J. R. (1969). Reactions towards the source of stimulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81(1), 174–176.CrossRefPubMed Simon, J. R. (1969). Reactions towards the source of stimulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81(1), 174–176.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Tsai, C. C., Kuo, W. J., Jing, J. T., Hung, D. L., & Tzeng, O. J. L. (2006). A common coding framework in self-other interaction: Evidence from joint action task. Experimental Brain Research, 175(2), 353–362. Tsai, C. C., Kuo, W. J., Jing, J. T., Hung, D. L., & Tzeng, O. J. L. (2006). A common coding framework in self-other interaction: Evidence from joint action task. Experimental Brain Research, 175(2), 353–362.
go back to reference Vesper, C., Butterfill, S., Knoblich, G., & Sebanz, N. (2010). A minimal architecture for joint action. Neural Networks, 23(8–9), 998–1003.CrossRefPubMed Vesper, C., Butterfill, S., Knoblich, G., & Sebanz, N. (2010). A minimal architecture for joint action. Neural Networks, 23(8–9), 998–1003.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Welsh, T. N., Kiernan, D., Neyedli, H. F., Ray, M., Pratt, J., Potruff, A., & Weeks, D. J. (2013). Joint Simon effects in extrapersonal space. Journal of Motor Behavior, 45(1), 1–5.CrossRefPubMed Welsh, T. N., Kiernan, D., Neyedli, H. F., Ray, M., Pratt, J., Potruff, A., & Weeks, D. J. (2013). Joint Simon effects in extrapersonal space. Journal of Motor Behavior, 45(1), 1–5.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wenke, D., Atmaca, S., Holländer, A., Liepelt, R., Baess, P., & Prinz, W. (2011). What is shared in joint action? Issues of co-representation, response conflict, and agent identification. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 2(2), 147–172.CrossRef Wenke, D., Atmaca, S., Holländer, A., Liepelt, R., Baess, P., & Prinz, W. (2011). What is shared in joint action? Issues of co-representation, response conflict, and agent identification. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 2(2), 147–172.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Whose turn is it anyway? The moderating role of response-execution certainty on the joint Simon effect
Auteurs
April Karlinsky
Melanie Y. Lam
Romeo Chua
Nicola J. Hodges
Publicatiedatum
09-08-2017
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 5/2019
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-017-0901-7

Andere artikelen Uitgave 5/2019

Psychological Research 5/2019 Naar de uitgave