Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 4/2018

10-03-2017 | Original Article

Contingency and contiguity of imitative behaviour affect social affiliation

Auteurs: David Dignath, Paul Lotze-Hermes, Harry Farmer, Roland Pfister

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 4/2018

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Actions of others automatically prime similar responses in an agent’s behavioural repertoire. As a consequence, perceived or anticipated imitation facilitates own action control and, at the same time, imitation boosts social affiliation and rapport with others. It has previously been suggested that basic mechanisms of associative learning can account for behavioural effects of imitation, whereas a possible role of associative learning for affiliative processes is poorly understood at present. Therefore, this study examined whether contingency and contiguity, the principles of associative learning, affect also the social effects of imitation. Two experiments yielded evidence in favour of this hypothesis by showing more social affiliation in conditions with high contingency (as compared to low contingency) and in conditions of high contiguity (compared to low contiguity).
Voetnoten
1
Due to an error when naming and saving the video files, the video of one of the 27 models showed only vertical movements in all conditions. Data from this block was excluded from the analysis.
 
2
Please note, that the design of this study was not intended to test for automatic imitation effects in performance data.
 
3
Two raters coded the answers of the participants. Participants were identified as being aware of the experimental manipulation when they affirmed at least one of two questions (question 1: “Did the movement of the person in the video influence your judgement of the other person?”; question 2: Did the frequency of similar or dissimilar movements have an influence on your judgement about the other person?”).
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Adank, P., Stewart, A.J., Connell, L., & Wood, J. (2013). Accent imitation positively affects language attitudes. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 280. Adank, P., Stewart, A.J., Connell, L., & Wood, J. (2013). Accent imitation positively affects language attitudes. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 280.
go back to reference Aron, A., & Aron, E. N. (1986). Love and the expansion of self: Understanding attraction and satisfaction. Washington, DC: Hemisphere. Aron, A., & Aron, E. N. (1986). Love and the expansion of self: Understanding attraction and satisfaction. Washington, DC: Hemisphere.
go back to reference Bailenson, J.N., Beall, A.C., Loomis, J., Blascovich, J., & Turk, M. (2004). Transformed social interaction: decoupling representation from behavior and form in collaborative virtual environments. Presence, 13(4), 428–441.CrossRef Bailenson, J.N., Beall, A.C., Loomis, J., Blascovich, J., & Turk, M. (2004). Transformed social interaction: decoupling representation from behavior and form in collaborative virtual environments. Presence, 13(4), 428–441.CrossRef
go back to reference Bailenson, J.N., Yee, N., Patel, K., & Beall, A.C. (2008). Detecting digital chameleons. Computers in Human Behavior, 24(1), 66–87.CrossRef Bailenson, J.N., Yee, N., Patel, K., & Beall, A.C. (2008). Detecting digital chameleons. Computers in Human Behavior, 24(1), 66–87.CrossRef
go back to reference Brass, M., Bekkering, H., Wohlschläger, A., & Prinz, W. (2000). Compatibility between observed and executed finger movements: comparing symbolic, spatial, and imitative cues. Brain and Cognition, 44(2), 124–143.CrossRefPubMed Brass, M., Bekkering, H., Wohlschläger, A., & Prinz, W. (2000). Compatibility between observed and executed finger movements: comparing symbolic, spatial, and imitative cues. Brain and Cognition, 44(2), 124–143.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Brass, M., & Heyes, C. (2005). Imitation: is cognitive neuroscience solving the correspondence problem? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(10), 489–495.CrossRefPubMed Brass, M., & Heyes, C. (2005). Imitation: is cognitive neuroscience solving the correspondence problem? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(10), 489–495.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Catmur, C., Gillmeister, H., Bird, G., Liepelt, R., Brass, M., & Heyes, C. (2008). Through the looking glass: counter-mirror activation following incompatible sensorimotor learning. European Journal of Neuroscience, 28(6), 1208–1215.CrossRefPubMed Catmur, C., Gillmeister, H., Bird, G., Liepelt, R., Brass, M., & Heyes, C. (2008). Through the looking glass: counter-mirror activation following incompatible sensorimotor learning. European Journal of Neuroscience, 28(6), 1208–1215.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Catmur, C., & Heyes, C. (2013). Is It What You Do, or When You Do It? The roles of contingency and similarity in pro-social effects of imitation. Cognitive Science, 37(8), 1541–1552.CrossRefPubMed Catmur, C., & Heyes, C. (2013). Is It What You Do, or When You Do It? The roles of contingency and similarity in pro-social effects of imitation. Cognitive Science, 37(8), 1541–1552.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Catmur, C., Walsh, V., & Heyes, C. (2009). Associative sequence learning: the role of experience in the development of imitation and the mirror system. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 364(1528), 2369–2380.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Catmur, C., Walsh, V., & Heyes, C. (2009). Associative sequence learning: the role of experience in the development of imitation and the mirror system. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 364(1528), 2369–2380.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Catmur, C. (2017). Automatic imitation? Imitative compatibility affects responses at high perceptual load. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance (in press). Catmur, C. (2017). Automatic imitation? Imitative compatibility affects responses at high perceptual load. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance (in press).
go back to reference Chartrand, T.L., & Bargh, J.A. (1999). The chameleon effect: the perception–behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(6), 893–910.CrossRefPubMed Chartrand, T.L., & Bargh, J.A. (1999). The chameleon effect: the perception–behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(6), 893–910.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Chartrand, T.L., & Lakin, J.L. (2013). The antecedents and consequences of human behavioral mimicry. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 285–308.CrossRefPubMed Chartrand, T.L., & Lakin, J.L. (2013). The antecedents and consequences of human behavioral mimicry. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 285–308.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Cook, J. L., & Bird, G. (2012). Atypical social modulation of imitation in autism spectrum conditions. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(6), 1045–10511 Cook, J. L., & Bird, G. (2012). Atypical social modulation of imitation in autism spectrum conditions. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(6), 1045–10511
go back to reference Cook, R., Press, C., Dickinson, A., & Heyes, C. (2010). Acquisition of automatic imitation is sensitive to sensorimotor contingency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36(4), 840–852.PubMed Cook, R., Press, C., Dickinson, A., & Heyes, C. (2010). Acquisition of automatic imitation is sensitive to sensorimotor contingency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36(4), 840–852.PubMed
go back to reference De Coster, L., Verschuere, B., Goubert, L., Tsakiris, M., & Brass, M. (2013). I suffer more from your pain when you act like me: being imitated enhances affective responses to seeing someone else in pain. Cognitive Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 13(3), 519–532.CrossRef De Coster, L., Verschuere, B., Goubert, L., Tsakiris, M., & Brass, M. (2013). I suffer more from your pain when you act like me: being imitated enhances affective responses to seeing someone else in pain. Cognitive Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 13(3), 519–532.CrossRef
go back to reference Dignath, D., & Eder, A.B. (2013). Recall of observed actions modulates the end-state comfort effect just like recall of one’s own actions. Experimental Brain Research, 231(1), 75–83.CrossRefPubMed Dignath, D., & Eder, A.B. (2013). Recall of observed actions modulates the end-state comfort effect just like recall of one’s own actions. Experimental Brain Research, 231(1), 75–83.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Dignath, D., Pfister, R., Eder, A.B., Kiesel, A., & Kunde, W. (2014). Representing the hyphen in action–effect associations: automatic acquisition and bidirectional retrieval of action–effect intervals. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition 40(6), 1701–1712.CrossRef Dignath, D., Pfister, R., Eder, A.B., Kiesel, A., & Kunde, W. (2014). Representing the hyphen in action–effect associations: automatic acquisition and bidirectional retrieval of action–effect intervals. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition 40(6), 1701–1712.CrossRef
go back to reference Eitam, B., Kennedy, P.M., & Higgins, E.T. (2013). Motivation from control. Experimental Brain Research, 229(3), 475–484.CrossRefPubMed Eitam, B., Kennedy, P.M., & Higgins, E.T. (2013). Motivation from control. Experimental Brain Research, 229(3), 475–484.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Elsner, B., & Hommel, B. (2001). Effect anticipation and action control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 27(1), 229–240.PubMed Elsner, B., & Hommel, B. (2001). Effect anticipation and action control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 27(1), 229–240.PubMed
go back to reference Genschow, O., & Brass, M. (2015). The predictive chameleon: evidence for anticipated social action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 41(2), 265–268.PubMed Genschow, O., & Brass, M. (2015). The predictive chameleon: evidence for anticipated social action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 41(2), 265–268.PubMed
go back to reference Gillmeister, H., Catmur, C., Liepelt, R., Brass, M., & Heyes, C. (2008). Experience-based priming of body parts: a study of action imitation. Brain Research, 1217, 157–170.CrossRefPubMed Gillmeister, H., Catmur, C., Liepelt, R., Brass, M., & Heyes, C. (2008). Experience-based priming of body parts: a study of action imitation. Brain Research, 1217, 157–170.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hajcak, G., & Foti, D. (2008). Errors are aversive defensive motivation and the error-related negativity. Psychological Science, 19(2), 103–108.CrossRefPubMed Hajcak, G., & Foti, D. (2008). Errors are aversive defensive motivation and the error-related negativity. Psychological Science, 19(2), 103–108.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hale, J., & Hamilton, A.F. (2016). Cognitive mechanisms for responding to mimicry from others. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 63, 106–123.CrossRefPubMed Hale, J., & Hamilton, A.F. (2016). Cognitive mechanisms for responding to mimicry from others. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 63, 106–123.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hasler, B. S., Hirschberger, G., Shani-Sherman, T., & Friedman, D. A. (2014). Virtual peacemakers: Mimicry increases empathy in simulated contact with virtual outgroup members. Cyberpsychology, Behavior and Social Networking, 17(12), 766–771. Hasler, B. S., Hirschberger, G., Shani-Sherman, T., & Friedman, D. A. (2014). Virtual peacemakers: Mimicry increases empathy in simulated contact with virtual outgroup members. Cyberpsychology, Behavior and Social Networking, 17(12), 766–771.
go back to reference Hayes, A.E., Paul, M.A., Beuger, B., & Tipper, S.P. (2008). Self produced and observed actions influence emotion: the roles of action fluency and eye gaze. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 72(4), 461–472.CrossRefPubMed Hayes, A.E., Paul, M.A., Beuger, B., & Tipper, S.P. (2008). Self produced and observed actions influence emotion: the roles of action fluency and eye gaze. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 72(4), 461–472.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Heyes, C. (2001). Causes and consequences of imitation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5(6), 253–261.CrossRefPubMed Heyes, C. (2001). Causes and consequences of imitation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5(6), 253–261.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Heyes, C. (2011). Automatic imitation. Psychological Bulletin, 137(3), 463. Heyes, C. (2011). Automatic imitation. Psychological Bulletin, 137(3), 463.
go back to reference Heyes, C. (2016). Homo imitans? Seven reasons why imitation couldn’t possibly be associative. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 371, 20150069.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Heyes, C. (2016). Homo imitans? Seven reasons why imitation couldn’t possibly be associative. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 371, 20150069.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Heyes, C., Bird, G., Johnson, H., & Haggard, P. (2005). Experience modulates automatic imitation. Cognitive Brain Research, 22(2), 233–240.CrossRefPubMed Heyes, C., Bird, G., Johnson, H., & Haggard, P. (2005). Experience modulates automatic imitation. Cognitive Brain Research, 22(2), 233–240.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Heyes, C. (2012). Imitation: associative and context Dependent. In W. Prinz, M. Beisert, A. Herwig (Eds.), Tutorials in action science. Cambridge: MIT Press. Heyes, C. (2012). Imitation: associative and context Dependent. In W. Prinz, M. Beisert, A. Herwig (Eds.), Tutorials in action science. Cambridge: MIT Press.
go back to reference Inzlicht, M., Gutsell, J.N., & Legault, L. (2012). Mimicry reduces racial prejudice. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(1), 361–365.CrossRef Inzlicht, M., Gutsell, J.N., & Legault, L. (2012). Mimicry reduces racial prejudice. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(1), 361–365.CrossRef
go back to reference Jansson, E., Wilson, A.D., Williams, J.H., & Mon-Williams, M. (2007). Methodological problems undermine tests of the ideo-motor conjecture. Experimental Brain Research, 182(4), 549–558.CrossRefPubMed Jansson, E., Wilson, A.D., Williams, J.H., & Mon-Williams, M. (2007). Methodological problems undermine tests of the ideo-motor conjecture. Experimental Brain Research, 182(4), 549–558.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kilner, J., Hamilton, A.F., de, C., & Blakemore, S.-J. (2007). Interference effect of observed human movement on action is due to velocity profile of biological motion. Social Neuroscience, 2(3–4), 158–166.CrossRefPubMed Kilner, J., Hamilton, A.F., de, C., & Blakemore, S.-J. (2007). Interference effect of observed human movement on action is due to velocity profile of biological motion. Social Neuroscience, 2(3–4), 158–166.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kulesza, W., Szypowska, Z., Jarman, M.S., & Dolinski, D. (2014). Attractive chameleons sell: the mimicry-attractiveness link. Psychology and Marketing, 31(7), 549–561.CrossRef Kulesza, W., Szypowska, Z., Jarman, M.S., & Dolinski, D. (2014). Attractive chameleons sell: the mimicry-attractiveness link. Psychology and Marketing, 31(7), 549–561.CrossRef
go back to reference Lakens, D., & Stel, M. (2011). If they move in sync, they must feel in sync: Movement synchrony leads to attributions of rapport and entitativity. Social Cognition, 29(1), 1–14.CrossRef Lakens, D., & Stel, M. (2011). If they move in sync, they must feel in sync: Movement synchrony leads to attributions of rapport and entitativity. Social Cognition, 29(1), 1–14.CrossRef
go back to reference Lakin, J.L., & Chartrand, T.L. (2003). Using nonconscious behavioral mimicry to create affiliation and rapport. Psychological Science, 14(4), 334–339.CrossRefPubMed Lakin, J.L., & Chartrand, T.L. (2003). Using nonconscious behavioral mimicry to create affiliation and rapport. Psychological Science, 14(4), 334–339.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Leighton, J., Bird, G., Orsini, C., & Heyes, C. (2010). Social attitudes modulate automatic imitation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(6), 905–910.CrossRef Leighton, J., Bird, G., Orsini, C., & Heyes, C. (2010). Social attitudes modulate automatic imitation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(6), 905–910.CrossRef
go back to reference Logan, F.A. (1965). Decision making by rats: uncertain outcome choices. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 59(2), 246–251.CrossRefPubMed Logan, F.A. (1965). Decision making by rats: uncertain outcome choices. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 59(2), 246–251.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Meltzoff, A.N., Moore, M.K., & others (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198(4312), 75–78.CrossRefPubMed Meltzoff, A.N., Moore, M.K., & others (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198(4312), 75–78.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Müller, R. (2016). Does the anticipation of compatible partner reactions facilitate action planning in joint tasks? Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 80(4), 464–486.CrossRefPubMed Müller, R. (2016). Does the anticipation of compatible partner reactions facilitate action planning in joint tasks? Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 80(4), 464–486.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Oppenheimer, D.M., Meyvis, T., & Davidenko, N. (2009). Instructional manipulation checks: detecting satisficing to increase statistical power. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(4), 867–872.CrossRef Oppenheimer, D.M., Meyvis, T., & Davidenko, N. (2009). Instructional manipulation checks: detecting satisficing to increase statistical power. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(4), 867–872.CrossRef
go back to reference Pearce, J.M., & Hall, G. (1980). A model for Pavlovian learning: variations in the effectiveness of conditioned but not of unconditioned stimuli. Psychological Review, 87(6), 532–552.CrossRefPubMed Pearce, J.M., & Hall, G. (1980). A model for Pavlovian learning: variations in the effectiveness of conditioned but not of unconditioned stimuli. Psychological Review, 87(6), 532–552.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Pfister, R., Dignath, D., Hommel, B., & Kunde, W. (2013). It takes two to imitate anticipation and imitation in social interaction. Psychological Science, 24(10), 2117–2121.CrossRefPubMed Pfister, R., Dignath, D., Hommel, B., & Kunde, W. (2013). It takes two to imitate anticipation and imitation in social interaction. Psychological Science, 24(10), 2117–2121.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Pfister, R., Kiesel, A., & Hoffmann, J. (2011). Learning at any rate: action–effect learning for stimulus-based actions. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 75(1), 61–65.CrossRefPubMed Pfister, R., Kiesel, A., & Hoffmann, J. (2011). Learning at any rate: action–effect learning for stimulus-based actions. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 75(1), 61–65.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Prinz, W. (2002). Experimental approaches to imitation. In A. N. Meltzoff & W. Prinz (Eds.), The imitative mind: development, evolution, and brain bases (pp. 143–162). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Prinz, W. (2002). Experimental approaches to imitation. In A. N. Meltzoff & W. Prinz (Eds.), The imitative mind: development, evolution, and brain bases (pp. 143–162). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Reber, R., Winkielman, P., & Schwarz, N. (1998). Effects of perceptual fluency on affective judgments. Psychological Science, 9(1), 45–48.CrossRef Reber, R., Winkielman, P., & Schwarz, N. (1998). Effects of perceptual fluency on affective judgments. Psychological Science, 9(1), 45–48.CrossRef
go back to reference Rescorla, R.A., & Wagner, A.R. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In A. H. Black & W. F. Prokasy (Eds.), Classical conditioning II (pp. 64–99). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Rescorla, R.A., & Wagner, A.R. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In A. H. Black & W. F. Prokasy (Eds.), Classical conditioning II (pp. 64–99). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
go back to reference Schwarz, N., Bless, H., Strack, F., Klumpp, G., Rittenauer-Schatka, H., & Simons, A. (1991). Ease of retrieval as information: Another look at the availability heuristic. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(2), 195–202.CrossRef Schwarz, N., Bless, H., Strack, F., Klumpp, G., Rittenauer-Schatka, H., & Simons, A. (1991). Ease of retrieval as information: Another look at the availability heuristic. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(2), 195–202.CrossRef
go back to reference Sparenberg, P., Topolinski, S., Springer, A., & Prinz, W. (2012). Minimal mimicry: Mere effector matching induces preference. Brain and Cognition, 80(3), 291–300.CrossRefPubMed Sparenberg, P., Topolinski, S., Springer, A., & Prinz, W. (2012). Minimal mimicry: Mere effector matching induces preference. Brain and Cognition, 80(3), 291–300.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Spengler, S., von Cramon, D.Y., & Brass, M. (2009). Was it me or was it you? How the sense of agency originates from ideomotor learning revealed by fMRI. Neuroimage, 46(1), 290–298.CrossRefPubMed Spengler, S., von Cramon, D.Y., & Brass, M. (2009). Was it me or was it you? How the sense of agency originates from ideomotor learning revealed by fMRI. Neuroimage, 46(1), 290–298.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Stephens, J.M. (1934). The influence of punishment on learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 17(4), 536–555.CrossRef Stephens, J.M. (1934). The influence of punishment on learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 17(4), 536–555.CrossRef
go back to reference Topolinski, S., & Reber, R. (2010). Immediate truth–Temporal contiguity between a cognitive problem and its solution determines experienced veracity of the solution. Cognition, 114(1), 117–122.CrossRefPubMed Topolinski, S., & Reber, R. (2010). Immediate truth–Temporal contiguity between a cognitive problem and its solution determines experienced veracity of the solution. Cognition, 114(1), 117–122.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Van Baaren, R.B., Holland, R.W., Steenaert, B., & van Knippenberg, A. (2003). Mimicry for money: behavioral consequences of imitation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39(4), 393–398.CrossRef Van Baaren, R.B., Holland, R.W., Steenaert, B., & van Knippenberg, A. (2003). Mimicry for money: behavioral consequences of imitation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39(4), 393–398.CrossRef
go back to reference Van Baaren, R.B., Holland, R.W., Kawakami, K., & Van Knippenberg, A. (2004). Mimicry and prosocial behavior. Psychological Science, 15(1), 71–74.CrossRefPubMed Van Baaren, R.B., Holland, R.W., Kawakami, K., & Van Knippenberg, A. (2004). Mimicry and prosocial behavior. Psychological Science, 15(1), 71–74.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wang, Y., & Hamilton, A.F. (2012). Social top-down response modulation (STORM): a model of the control of mimicry in social interaction. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6(153). Wang, Y., & Hamilton, A.F. (2012). Social top-down response modulation (STORM): a model of the control of mimicry in social interaction. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6(153).
go back to reference Winkielman, P., & Cacioppo, J.T. (2001). Mind at ease puts a smile on the face: psychophysiological evidence that processing facilitation elicits positive affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(6), 989–1000.CrossRefPubMed Winkielman, P., & Cacioppo, J.T. (2001). Mind at ease puts a smile on the face: psychophysiological evidence that processing facilitation elicits positive affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(6), 989–1000.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Zajonc, R.B. (1968). Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9(2), 1–27.CrossRef Zajonc, R.B. (1968). Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9(2), 1–27.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Contingency and contiguity of imitative behaviour affect social affiliation
Auteurs
David Dignath
Paul Lotze-Hermes
Harry Farmer
Roland Pfister
Publicatiedatum
10-03-2017
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 4/2018
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-017-0854-x

Andere artikelen Uitgave 4/2018

Psychological Research 4/2018 Naar de uitgave