Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 6/2017

17-10-2016 | Original Article

Orthogonal-compatibility effects confound automatic imitation: implications for measuring self–other distinction

Auteurs: Daniel Joel Shaw, Kristína Czekóová, Michaela Porubanová

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 6/2017

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Accurate distinction between self and other representations is fundamental to a range of social cognitive capacities, and understanding individual differences in this ability is an important aim for psychological research. This demands accurate measures of self–other distinction (SOD). The present study examined an experimental paradigm employed frequently to measure SOD in the action domain; specifically, we evaluated the rotated finger-action stimuli used increasingly to measure automatic imitation (AI). To assess the suitability of these stimuli, we compared AI elicited by different action stimuli to the performance on a perspective-taking task believed to measure SOD in the perception domain. In two separate experiments we reveal three important findings: firstly, we demonstrate a strong confounding influence of orthogonal-compatibility effects on AI elicited by certain rotated stimuli. Second, we demonstrate the potential for this confounding influence to mask important relationships between AI and other measures of SOD; we observed a relationship between AI and perspective-taking performance only when the former was measured in isolation of orthogonality compatibility. Thirdly, we observed a relationship between these two performance measures only in a sub-group of individuals exhibiting a pure form of AI. Furthermore, this relationship revealed a self-bias in SOD—reduced AI was associated with increased egocentric misattributions in perspective taking. Together, our findings identify an important methodological consideration for measures of AI and extend previous research by showing an egocentric style of SOD across action and perception domains.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Literatuur
go back to reference Apperly, I. A., Carroll, D. J., Samson, D., Humphreys, G. W., Qureshi, A., & Moffitt, G. (2010). Why are there limits on theory of mind use? Evidence from adults’ ability to follow instructions from an ignorant speaker. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 1201–1217. doi:10.1080/17470210903281582.CrossRef Apperly, I. A., Carroll, D. J., Samson, D., Humphreys, G. W., Qureshi, A., & Moffitt, G. (2010). Why are there limits on theory of mind use? Evidence from adults’ ability to follow instructions from an ignorant speaker. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 1201–1217. doi:10.​1080/​1747021090328158​2.CrossRef
go back to reference Bien, N., Roebroeck, A., Goebel, R., & Sack, A. T. (2009). The brain’s intention to imitate: The neurobiology of intentional versus automatic imitation. Cerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y.: 1991), 19(10), 2338–2351. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhn251.CrossRef Bien, N., Roebroeck, A., Goebel, R., & Sack, A. T. (2009). The brain’s intention to imitate: The neurobiology of intentional versus automatic imitation. Cerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y.: 1991), 19(10), 2338–2351. doi:10.​1093/​cercor/​bhn251.CrossRef
go back to reference Boyer, T. W., Scheutz, M., & Bertenthal, B. I. (2009). Dissociating ideomotor and spatial compatibility: Empirical evidence and connectionist models. In: Proceedings of the 31st annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 2280–2285). Cognitive Science Society Austin, TX. Boyer, T. W., Scheutz, M., & Bertenthal, B. I. (2009). Dissociating ideomotor and spatial compatibility: Empirical evidence and connectionist models. In: Proceedings of the 31st annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 2280–2285). Cognitive Science Society Austin, TX.
go back to reference Brass, M., Heyes, C., (2005) Imitation: is cognitive neuroscience solving the correspondence problem? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(10), 489–495.CrossRefPubMed Brass, M., Heyes, C., (2005) Imitation: is cognitive neuroscience solving the correspondence problem? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(10), 489–495.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Brass, M., Bekkering, H., & Prinz, W. (2001). Movement observation affects movement execution in a simple response task. Acta Psychologica, 106(1), 3–22.CrossRefPubMed Brass, M., Bekkering, H., & Prinz, W. (2001). Movement observation affects movement execution in a simple response task. Acta Psychologica, 106(1), 3–22.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Brass, M., Bekkering, H., Wohlschläger, A., & Prinz, W. (2000). Compatibility between observed and executed finger movements: Comparing symbolic, spatial, and imitative cues. Brain and Cognition, 44(2), 124–143. doi:10.1006/brcg.2000.1225.CrossRefPubMed Brass, M., Bekkering, H., Wohlschläger, A., & Prinz, W. (2000). Compatibility between observed and executed finger movements: Comparing symbolic, spatial, and imitative cues. Brain and Cognition, 44(2), 124–143. doi:10.​1006/​brcg.​2000.​1225.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Brass, M., Ruby, P., & Spengler, S. (2009). Inhibition of imitative behaviour and social cognition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B Biological sciences, 364(1528), 2359–2367. doi:10.1098/rstb.2009.0066.CrossRefPubMed Brass, M., Ruby, P., & Spengler, S. (2009). Inhibition of imitative behaviour and social cognition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B Biological sciences, 364(1528), 2359–2367. doi:10.​1098/​rstb.​2009.​0066.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Bruyer, R., & Brysbaert, M. (2011). Combining speed and accuracy in cognitive psychology: Is the inverse efficiency score (IES) a better dependent variable than the mean reaction time (RT) and the percentage of errors (PE)? Psychologica Belgica, 51(1), 5–13.CrossRef Bruyer, R., & Brysbaert, M. (2011). Combining speed and accuracy in cognitive psychology: Is the inverse efficiency score (IES) a better dependent variable than the mean reaction time (RT) and the percentage of errors (PE)? Psychologica Belgica, 51(1), 5–13.CrossRef
go back to reference Catmur, C., & Heyes, C. (2011). Time course analyses confirm independence of imitative and spatial compatibility. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 37(2), 409–421. doi:10.1037/a0019325.CrossRefPubMed Catmur, C., & Heyes, C. (2011). Time course analyses confirm independence of imitative and spatial compatibility. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 37(2), 409–421. doi:10.​1037/​a0019325.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Catmur, C., Walsh, V., & Heyes, C. (2009). Associative sequence learning: The role of experience in the development of imitation and the mirror system. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B Biological Sciences, 364(1528), 2369–2380. doi:10.1098/rstb.2009.0048.CrossRefPubMed Catmur, C., Walsh, V., & Heyes, C. (2009). Associative sequence learning: The role of experience in the development of imitation and the mirror system. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B Biological Sciences, 364(1528), 2369–2380. doi:10.​1098/​rstb.​2009.​0048.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference de Guzman, M., Bird, G., Banissy, M. J., & Catmur, C. (2016). Self–other control processes in social cognition: From imitation to empathy. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 371(1686), 20150079. doi:10.1098/rstb.2015.0079.CrossRef de Guzman, M., Bird, G., Banissy, M. J., & Catmur, C. (2016). Self–other control processes in social cognition: From imitation to empathy. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 371(1686), 20150079. doi:10.​1098/​rstb.​2015.​0079.CrossRef
go back to reference Decety, J., & Lamm, C. (2007). The role of the right temporoparietal junction in social interaction: How low-level computational processes contribute to meta-cognition. Neuroscientist, 13, 580–593.CrossRefPubMed Decety, J., & Lamm, C. (2007). The role of the right temporoparietal junction in social interaction: How low-level computational processes contribute to meta-cognition. Neuroscientist, 13, 580–593.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Gazzola, V., Aziz-Zadeh, L., & Keysers, C. (2006). Empathy and the somatotopic auditory mirror system in humans. Current Biology, 16(18), 1824–1829.CrossRefPubMed Gazzola, V., Aziz-Zadeh, L., & Keysers, C. (2006). Empathy and the somatotopic auditory mirror system in humans. Current Biology, 16(18), 1824–1829.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Heyes, C. (2014). Submentalizing: I Am Not Really Reading Your Mind. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9(2), 131–143.CrossRefPubMed Heyes, C. (2014). Submentalizing: I Am Not Really Reading Your Mind. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9(2), 131–143.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hogeveen, J., & Obhi, S. S. (2013). Automatic imitation is automatic, but less so for narcissists. Experimental Brain Research. Experimentelle Hirnforschung. Expérimentation Cérébrale, 224(4), 613–621. doi:10.1007/s00221-012-3339-6.CrossRefPubMed Hogeveen, J., & Obhi, S. S. (2013). Automatic imitation is automatic, but less so for narcissists. Experimental Brain Research. Experimentelle Hirnforschung. Expérimentation Cérébrale, 224(4), 613–621. doi:10.​1007/​s00221-012-3339-6.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hogeveen, J., Obhi, S. S., Banissy, M. J., Santiesteban, I., Press, C., Catmur, C., & Bird, G. (2014). Task-dependent and distinct roles of the temporoparietal junction and inferior frontal cortex in the control of imitation. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience,. doi:10.1093/scan/nsu148.PubMedPubMedCentral Hogeveen, J., Obhi, S. S., Banissy, M. J., Santiesteban, I., Press, C., Catmur, C., & Bird, G. (2014). Task-dependent and distinct roles of the temporoparietal junction and inferior frontal cortex in the control of imitation. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience,. doi:10.​1093/​scan/​nsu148.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Lamm, C., Bukowski, H., & Silani, G. (2016). From shared to distinct self—other representations in empathy: Evidence from neurotypical function and socio-cognitive disorders. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 371(1686), 20150083. doi:10.1098/rstb.2015.0083.CrossRef Lamm, C., Bukowski, H., & Silani, G. (2016). From shared to distinct self—other representations in empathy: Evidence from neurotypical function and socio-cognitive disorders. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 371(1686), 20150083. doi:10.​1098/​rstb.​2015.​0083.CrossRef
go back to reference Obhi, S. S., Hogeveen, J., Giacomin, M., & Jordan, C. H. (2014). Automatic imitation is reduced in narcissists. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 40(3), 920–928. doi:10.1037/a0034056.CrossRefPubMed Obhi, S. S., Hogeveen, J., Giacomin, M., & Jordan, C. H. (2014). Automatic imitation is reduced in narcissists. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 40(3), 920–928. doi:10.​1037/​a0034056.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Press, C., Bird, G., Walsh, E., & Heyes, C. (2008). Automatic imitation of intransitive actions. Brain and Cognition, 67, 44–50.CrossRefPubMed Press, C., Bird, G., Walsh, E., & Heyes, C. (2008). Automatic imitation of intransitive actions. Brain and Cognition, 67, 44–50.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Press, C., Gillmeister, H., & Heyes, C. (2007). Sensorimotor experience enhances automatic imitation of robotic action. Proceedings Biological Sciences/The Royal Society, 274(1625), 2509–2514. doi:10.1098/rspb.2007.0774.CrossRef Press, C., Gillmeister, H., & Heyes, C. (2007). Sensorimotor experience enhances automatic imitation of robotic action. Proceedings Biological Sciences/The Royal Society, 274(1625), 2509–2514. doi:10.​1098/​rspb.​2007.​0774.CrossRef
go back to reference Proctor, R. W., & Vu, K. P. L. (2012). Stimulus-response compatibility principles: Data, theory, and application. Boca Raton: CRC Press. Proctor, R. W., & Vu, K. P. L. (2012). Stimulus-response compatibility principles: Data, theory, and application. Boca Raton: CRC Press.
go back to reference Santiesteban, I., Banissy, M. J., Catmur, C., & Bird, G. (2012a). Enhancing social ability by stimulating right temporoparietal junction. Current Bioology, 22(23), 2274–2277.CrossRef Santiesteban, I., Banissy, M. J., Catmur, C., & Bird, G. (2012a). Enhancing social ability by stimulating right temporoparietal junction. Current Bioology, 22(23), 2274–2277.CrossRef
go back to reference Santiesteban, I., Shah, P., White, S., Bird, G., & Heyes, C. (2015). Mentalizing or submentalizing in a communication task? Evidence from autism and a camera control. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 22(3), 844–849. doi:10.3758/s13423-014-0716-0.CrossRefPubMed Santiesteban, I., Shah, P., White, S., Bird, G., & Heyes, C. (2015). Mentalizing or submentalizing in a communication task? Evidence from autism and a camera control. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 22(3), 844–849. doi:10.​3758/​s13423-014-0716-0.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Sowden, S., & Catmur, C. (2015). The role of the right temporoparietal junction in the control of imitation. Cerebral Cortex, 25(4), 1107–1113.CrossRefPubMed Sowden, S., & Catmur, C. (2015). The role of the right temporoparietal junction in the control of imitation. Cerebral Cortex, 25(4), 1107–1113.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Spengler, S., von Cramon, D. Y., & Brass, M. (2009). Control of shared representations relies on key processes involved in mental state attribution. Human Brain Mapping, 30(11), 3704–3718. doi:10.1002/hbm.20800.CrossRefPubMed Spengler, S., von Cramon, D. Y., & Brass, M. (2009). Control of shared representations relies on key processes involved in mental state attribution. Human Brain Mapping, 30(11), 3704–3718. doi:10.​1002/​hbm.​20800.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Steinbeis, N. (2016). The role of self–other distinction in understanding others’ mental and emotional states: Neurocognitive mechanisms in children and adults. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 371(1686), 20150074.CrossRef Steinbeis, N. (2016). The role of self–other distinction in understanding others’ mental and emotional states: Neurocognitive mechanisms in children and adults. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 371(1686), 20150074.CrossRef
go back to reference Tomova, L., von Dawans, B., Heinrichs, M., Silani, G., & Lamm, C. (2014). Is stress affecting our ability to tune into others? Evidence for gender differences in the effects of stress on self-other distinction. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 43, 95–104. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.02.006.CrossRefPubMed Tomova, L., von Dawans, B., Heinrichs, M., Silani, G., & Lamm, C. (2014). Is stress affecting our ability to tune into others? Evidence for gender differences in the effects of stress on self-other distinction. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 43, 95–104. doi:10.​1016/​j.​psyneuen.​2014.​02.​006.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference West, B. T., Welch, K. B., & Galecki, A. T. (2007). Linear mixed models: A practical guide using statistical software. Florida: Taylor Francis Group. West, B. T., Welch, K. B., & Galecki, A. T. (2007). Linear mixed models: A practical guide using statistical software. Florida: Taylor Francis Group.
Metagegevens
Titel
Orthogonal-compatibility effects confound automatic imitation: implications for measuring self–other distinction
Auteurs
Daniel Joel Shaw
Kristína Czekóová
Michaela Porubanová
Publicatiedatum
17-10-2016
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 6/2017
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-016-0814-x

Andere artikelen Uitgave 6/2017

Psychological Research 6/2017 Naar de uitgave