Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 6/2017

12-10-2016 | Original Article

Reading sentences describing high- or low-pitched auditory events: only pianists show evidence for a horizontal space-pitch association

Auteurs: Sibylla Wolter, Carolin Dudschig, Barbara Kaup

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 6/2017

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

This study explored differences between pianists and non-musicians during reading of sentences describing high- or low-pitched auditory events. Based on the embodied model of language comprehension, it was hypothesized that the experience of playing the piano encourages a corresponding association between high-pitched sounds and the right and low-pitched sounds and the left. This pitch-space association is assumed to become elicited during understanding of sentences describing either a high- or low-pitched auditory event. In this study, pianists and non-musicians were tested based on the hypothesis that only pianists show a compatibility effect between implied pitch height and horizontal space, because only pianists have the corresponding experience with the piano keyboard. Participants read pitch-related sentences (e.g., the bear growls deeply, the soprano singer sings an aria) and judged whether the sentence was sensible or not by pressing either a left or right response key. The results indicated that only the pianists showed the predicted compatibility effect between implied pitch height and response location. Based on the results, it can be inferred that the experience of playing the piano led to an association between horizontal space and pitch height in pianists, while no such spatial association was elicited in non-musicians.
Voetnoten
1
The compatibility effect was not affected by the switch of response mapping (‘sensible right’ vs. ‘sensible left’) nor by the repetition of blocks 1 and 2.
 
2
Importantly, data interpretation was not affected by this exclusion. The analyses with and without the problematic item showed qualitatively similar results.
 
3
The group of non-musicians is most likely a less homogeneous group than the group of pianists, and might therefore have higher error variability. To account for this issue, we log-transformed the data (see e.g., Keppel & Wickens, 2004; Winter, 2013) and ran an additional ANOVA (including the same factors) with logRTs. The analyses showed qualitatively similar results to the analyses with untransformed RTs (Interaction between group and compatibility: F 1 (1,78) = 6.11, p = 0.01; F 2 (1,117) = 3.5, p = 0.06, Post-hoc tests for pianists: F 1 (1,39) = 7.9, p = 0.008; F 2 (1,117) = 5.9, p = 0.02; Post-hoc tests for non-musicians: F S < 1).
 
4
Additionally, we performed a mixed effect modeling analysis using R and lme4 (Bates, Maechler, Bolker, & Walker, 2014). As fixed effects we entered compatibility and group into the model. As random effects we had random intercepts for subjects and items as well as by-subject random slopes for the effect of compatibility and by-item random slopes for the effect of compatibility and group. In a first step, we compared this model M0 to a model M1, which additionally included the effect of interest, i.e., the interaction between compatibility and group. The results of the likelihood ratio tests confirmed the findings from the ANOVAs: Model M1 explained the data significantly better than model M0 [χ 2(1) = 3.9, p < 0.05]. We again ran separate mixed effect modeling analyses for the group of pianists and non-musicians. For the group of pianists, the model containing the compatibility effect explained the data significantly better [χ 2(5) = 15.16, p = 0.01], which was not the case for the group of non-musicians [χ 2(5) = 6.31, p = 0.28].
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Areshenkoff, C. N., Bub, D., & Masson, M. E. J. (2017). Task-dependent motor representations evoked by spatial words: Implications for embodied accounts of word meaning. Journal of Memory and Language, 92, 158–169.CrossRef Areshenkoff, C. N., Bub, D., & Masson, M. E. J. (2017). Task-dependent motor representations evoked by spatial words: Implications for embodied accounts of word meaning. Journal of Memory and Language, 92, 158–169.CrossRef
go back to reference Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 577–660.PubMed Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 577–660.PubMed
go back to reference Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2014). lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4. R package version 1.1-5. Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2014). lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4. R package version 1.1-5.
go back to reference Bub, D. N., & Masson, M. E. (2010). On the nature of hand-action representations evoked during written sentence comprehension. Cognition, 116(3), 394–408.CrossRefPubMed Bub, D. N., & Masson, M. E. (2010). On the nature of hand-action representations evoked during written sentence comprehension. Cognition, 116(3), 394–408.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Cho, Y. S., Proctor, R. W., & Bae, G. Y. (2012). Referential coding contributes to the horizontal SMARC effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38(3), 726–734.PubMed Cho, Y. S., Proctor, R. W., & Bae, G. Y. (2012). Referential coding contributes to the horizontal SMARC effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38(3), 726–734.PubMed
go back to reference Connell, L. (2007). Representing object colour in language comprehension. Cognition, 102, 476–485.CrossRefPubMed Connell, L. (2007). Representing object colour in language comprehension. Cognition, 102, 476–485.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference de la Vega, I., De Filippis, M., Lachmair, M., Dudschig, C., & Kaup, B. (2012). Emotional valence and physical space: Limits of interaction. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 375–385.PubMed de la Vega, I., De Filippis, M., Lachmair, M., Dudschig, C., & Kaup, B. (2012). Emotional valence and physical space: Limits of interaction. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 375–385.PubMed
go back to reference Dolscheid, S., Hunnius, S., Casasanto, D., & Majid, A. (2014). Prelinguistic infants are sensitive to space-pitch associations found across cultures. Psychological Science, 25, 1256–1261.CrossRefPubMed Dolscheid, S., Hunnius, S., Casasanto, D., & Majid, A. (2014). Prelinguistic infants are sensitive to space-pitch associations found across cultures. Psychological Science, 25, 1256–1261.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Dudschig, C., de la Vega, I., & Kaup, B. (2015). What’s up? Emotion-specific activation of vertical space during language processing. Acta Psychologica, 156, 143–155.CrossRefPubMed Dudschig, C., de la Vega, I., & Kaup, B. (2015). What’s up? Emotion-specific activation of vertical space during language processing. Acta Psychologica, 156, 143–155.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Dudschig, C., & Kaup, B. (2016). Is it all task-specific? The role of binary responses, verbal mediation and saliency for eliciting language-space associations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition. doi:10.1037/xlm0000297 Dudschig, C., & Kaup, B. (2016). Is it all task-specific? The role of binary responses, verbal mediation and saliency for eliciting language-space associations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition. doi:10.​1037/​xlm0000297
go back to reference Dudschig, C., Mackenzie, I. G., Strozyk, J., Kaup, B., & Leuthold, H. (2016). The sound of sentences: differentiating the influence of physical sound, sound imagery, and linguistically implied sounds on physical sound processing. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 16, 940–961. Dudschig, C., Mackenzie, I. G., Strozyk, J., Kaup, B., & Leuthold, H. (2016). The sound of sentences: differentiating the influence of physical sound, sound imagery, and linguistically implied sounds on physical sound processing. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 16, 940–961.
go back to reference Eitan, Z., & Timmers, R. (2010). Beethoven's last piano sonata and those who follow crocodiles: cross-domain mappings of auditory pitch in musical context. Cognition, 114, 405–422. Eitan, Z., & Timmers, R. (2010). Beethoven's last piano sonata and those who follow crocodiles: cross-domain mappings of auditory pitch in musical context. Cognition, 114, 405–422.
go back to reference Glenberg, A. M., & Kaschak, M. P. (2002). Grounding language in action. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 558–565.CrossRef Glenberg, A. M., & Kaschak, M. P. (2002). Grounding language in action. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 558–565.CrossRef
go back to reference Hauk, O., Johnsrude, I., & Pulvermüller, F. (2004). Somatotopic representation of action words in human motor and premotor cortex. Neuron, 41, 301–307.CrossRefPubMed Hauk, O., Johnsrude, I., & Pulvermüller, F. (2004). Somatotopic representation of action words in human motor and premotor cortex. Neuron, 41, 301–307.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Holt, L. E., & Beilock, S. L. (2006). Expertise and its embodiment: Examining the impact of sensorimotor skill expertise on the representation of action-related text. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13(4), 694–701.CrossRef Holt, L. E., & Beilock, S. L. (2006). Expertise and its embodiment: Examining the impact of sensorimotor skill expertise on the representation of action-related text. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13(4), 694–701.CrossRef
go back to reference Hutchinson, S., & Louwerse, M. M. (2014). Language statistics explain the spatial–numerical association of response codes. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21(2), 470–478.CrossRef Hutchinson, S., & Louwerse, M. M. (2014). Language statistics explain the spatial–numerical association of response codes. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21(2), 470–478.CrossRef
go back to reference Kaup, B., Lüdtke, J., & Zwaan, R. A. (2006). Processing negated sentences with contradictory predicates: Is a door that is not open mentally closed? Journal of Pragmatics, 38, 1033–1050.CrossRef Kaup, B., Lüdtke, J., & Zwaan, R. A. (2006). Processing negated sentences with contradictory predicates: Is a door that is not open mentally closed? Journal of Pragmatics, 38, 1033–1050.CrossRef
go back to reference Keppel, G., & Wickens, T. (2004). Design and analysis. A researcher’s handbook. New Jersey: Pearson Education Internationl. Keppel, G., & Wickens, T. (2004). Design and analysis. A researcher’s handbook. New Jersey: Pearson Education Internationl.
go back to reference Kintsch, W. (1988). The role of knowledge in discourse comprehension: A construction-integration model. Psychological Review, 95(2), 163–182.CrossRefPubMed Kintsch, W. (1988). The role of knowledge in discourse comprehension: A construction-integration model. Psychological Review, 95(2), 163–182.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kintsch, W., & Van Dijk, T. (1978). Towards a model of text comprehension and production. Psychological Review, 85, 363–394.CrossRef Kintsch, W., & Van Dijk, T. (1978). Towards a model of text comprehension and production. Psychological Review, 85, 363–394.CrossRef
go back to reference Lachmair, M., Dudschig, C., De Filippis, M., de la Vega, I., & Kaup, B. (2011). Root versus roof: automatic activation of location information during word processing. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 1180–1188.CrossRef Lachmair, M., Dudschig, C., De Filippis, M., de la Vega, I., & Kaup, B. (2011). Root versus roof: automatic activation of location information during word processing. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 1180–1188.CrossRef
go back to reference Lai, V. T., Willems, R. M., & Hagoort, P. (2015). Feel between the lines: Implied emotion in sentence comprehension. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 27(8), 1528–1541.CrossRefPubMed Lai, V. T., Willems, R. M., & Hagoort, P. (2015). Feel between the lines: Implied emotion in sentence comprehension. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 27(8), 1528–1541.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). The metaphorical structure of the human conceptual system. Cognitive Science, 4, 195–208.CrossRef Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). The metaphorical structure of the human conceptual system. Cognitive Science, 4, 195–208.CrossRef
go back to reference Lebois, L. A., Wilson-Mendenhall, S. J., & Barsalou, J. W. (2015). Are automatic conceptual cores the gold standard of semantic processing? The context-dependence of spatial meaning in grounded congruency effects. Cognitive Science, 39(8), 1764–1801.CrossRefPubMed Lebois, L. A., Wilson-Mendenhall, S. J., & Barsalou, J. W. (2015). Are automatic conceptual cores the gold standard of semantic processing? The context-dependence of spatial meaning in grounded congruency effects. Cognitive Science, 39(8), 1764–1801.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Lidji, P., Kolinsky, R., Lochy, A., & Morais, J. (2007). Spatial associations for musical stimuli: A piano in the head? Journal of Experimental Psychology, 33, 1189–1207.PubMed Lidji, P., Kolinsky, R., Lochy, A., & Morais, J. (2007). Spatial associations for musical stimuli: A piano in the head? Journal of Experimental Psychology, 33, 1189–1207.PubMed
go back to reference Masson, M. E. J., & Loftus, G. R. (2003). Using confidence intervals FPR graphically based data interpretation. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57(3), 203–220.CrossRefPubMed Masson, M. E. J., & Loftus, G. R. (2003). Using confidence intervals FPR graphically based data interpretation. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57(3), 203–220.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Miller, J., & Ulrich, R. (2013). Mental chronometry and individual differences: Modeling reliabilities and intercorrelations of reaction time means and effect sizes. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 819–858. Miller, J., & Ulrich, R. (2013). Mental chronometry and individual differences: Modeling reliabilities and intercorrelations of reaction time means and effect sizes. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 819–858.
go back to reference Möhring, W., Ramsook, K. A., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M., & Newcombe, N. S. (2016). Where music meets space: Children’s sensitivity to pitch intervals is related to their mental spatial transformation skills. Cognition, 151, 1–5.CrossRefPubMed Möhring, W., Ramsook, K. A., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M., & Newcombe, N. S. (2016). Where music meets space: Children’s sensitivity to pitch intervals is related to their mental spatial transformation skills. Cognition, 151, 1–5.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Myung, J., Blumstein, S. E., & Sedivy, J. C. (2006). Playing on the typewriter, typing on the piano: manipulation knowledge of objects. Cognition, 98(3), 223–243.CrossRefPubMed Myung, J., Blumstein, S. E., & Sedivy, J. C. (2006). Playing on the typewriter, typing on the piano: manipulation knowledge of objects. Cognition, 98(3), 223–243.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Nishimura, A., & Yokosawa, K. (2009). Effects of laterality and pitch height of an auditory accessory stimulus on horizontal response selection: The Simon effect and the SMARC effect. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16(4), 666–670.CrossRef Nishimura, A., & Yokosawa, K. (2009). Effects of laterality and pitch height of an auditory accessory stimulus on horizontal response selection: The Simon effect and the SMARC effect. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16(4), 666–670.CrossRef
go back to reference Parkinson, C., Kohler, P. J., Sievers, B., & Wheatley, T. (2012). Associations between auditory pitch and visual elevation do not depend on language: Evidence from a remote population. Perception, 41(7), 854–861.CrossRefPubMed Parkinson, C., Kohler, P. J., Sievers, B., & Wheatley, T. (2012). Associations between auditory pitch and visual elevation do not depend on language: Evidence from a remote population. Perception, 41(7), 854–861.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Rusconi, E., Kwan, B., Giordano, B. L., Umiltá, C., & Butterworth, B. (2006). Spatial representation of pitch height: The SMARC effect. Cognition, 99, 113–129.CrossRefPubMed Rusconi, E., Kwan, B., Giordano, B. L., Umiltá, C., & Butterworth, B. (2006). Spatial representation of pitch height: The SMARC effect. Cognition, 99, 113–129.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Santiago, J., Lupiáñez, J., Pérez, E., & Funes, M. J. (2007). Time (also) flies from left to right. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 512–516.CrossRef Santiago, J., Lupiáñez, J., Pérez, E., & Funes, M. J. (2007). Time (also) flies from left to right. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 512–516.CrossRef
go back to reference Simon, J. R. (1969). Reactions toward the source of stimulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81(1), 174–176.CrossRefPubMed Simon, J. R. (1969). Reactions toward the source of stimulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81(1), 174–176.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Spence, C. (2011). Crossmodal correspondences: A tutorial review. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73(4), 971–995.CrossRef Spence, C. (2011). Crossmodal correspondences: A tutorial review. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73(4), 971–995.CrossRef
go back to reference Tettamanti, M., Buccino, G., Saccuman, M. C., Gallese, V., Danna, M., Scifo, P., & Perani, D. (2005). Listening to action-related sentences activates fronto-parietal motor circuits. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(2), 273–281.CrossRefPubMed Tettamanti, M., Buccino, G., Saccuman, M. C., Gallese, V., Danna, M., Scifo, P., & Perani, D. (2005). Listening to action-related sentences activates fronto-parietal motor circuits. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(2), 273–281.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Torralbo, A., Santiago, J., & Lupiáñez, J. (2006). Flexible conceptual projection of time onto spatial frames of reference. Cognitive Science, 30(4), 745–757.CrossRefPubMed Torralbo, A., Santiago, J., & Lupiáñez, J. (2006). Flexible conceptual projection of time onto spatial frames of reference. Cognitive Science, 30(4), 745–757.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Trimarchi, P. D., & Luzatti, C. (2011). Implicit chord processing and motor representation in pianists. Psychological Research, 75, 122–128.CrossRefPubMed Trimarchi, P. D., & Luzatti, C. (2011). Implicit chord processing and motor representation in pianists. Psychological Research, 75, 122–128.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Ulrich, R., Eikmeier, V., de la Vega, I., Ruiz Fernández, S., Alex-Ruf, S., & Maienborn, C. (2012). With the past behind and the future ahead: Back-to-front representation of past and future sentences. Memory & Cognition, 40(3), 483–495.CrossRef Ulrich, R., Eikmeier, V., de la Vega, I., Ruiz Fernández, S., Alex-Ruf, S., & Maienborn, C. (2012). With the past behind and the future ahead: Back-to-front representation of past and future sentences. Memory & Cognition, 40(3), 483–495.CrossRef
go back to reference Ulrich, R., & Maienborn, C. (2010). Left-right coding of past and future in language: The mental timeline during sentences processing. Cognition, 117, 126–138.CrossRefPubMed Ulrich, R., & Maienborn, C. (2010). Left-right coding of past and future in language: The mental timeline during sentences processing. Cognition, 117, 126–138.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Walker, P., Bremner, J. G., Mason, U., Spring, J., Mattock, K., Slater, A., & Johnson, S. P. (2010). Preverbal infants’ sensitivity to synaesthetic cross-modality correspondences. Psychological Science, 21, 21–25.CrossRefPubMed Walker, P., Bremner, J. G., Mason, U., Spring, J., Mattock, K., Slater, A., & Johnson, S. P. (2010). Preverbal infants’ sensitivity to synaesthetic cross-modality correspondences. Psychological Science, 21, 21–25.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wallace, R. J. (1971). SR compatibility and the idea of a response code. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 88(3), 354–360.CrossRef Wallace, R. J. (1971). SR compatibility and the idea of a response code. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 88(3), 354–360.CrossRef
go back to reference Winter, B. (2013). Linear models and linear mixed effects models in R with linguistic application. arXiv:1308.5499. Winter, B. (2013). Linear models and linear mixed effects models in R with linguistic application. arXiv:1308.5499.
go back to reference Wolter, S., Dudschig, C., de la Vega, I., & Kaup, B. (2015). Musical metaphors: Evidence for a spatial grounding of non-literal sentences describing auditory events. Acta Psychologica, 156, 126–135.CrossRefPubMed Wolter, S., Dudschig, C., de la Vega, I., & Kaup, B. (2015). Musical metaphors: Evidence for a spatial grounding of non-literal sentences describing auditory events. Acta Psychologica, 156, 126–135.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Zwaan, R. A., & Madden, C. J. (2005). Embodied sentence comprehension. In D. Pecher & R. A. Zwaan (Eds.), Grounding Cognition: The role of perception and action in memory, language, and thinking (pp. 224–245). Cambridge: CUP.CrossRef Zwaan, R. A., & Madden, C. J. (2005). Embodied sentence comprehension. In D. Pecher & R. A. Zwaan (Eds.), Grounding Cognition: The role of perception and action in memory, language, and thinking (pp. 224–245). Cambridge: CUP.CrossRef
go back to reference Zwaan, R. A., Stanfield, R. A., & Yaxley, R. H. (2002). Do language comprehenders routinely represent the shapes of objects? Psychological Science, 13, 168–171.CrossRefPubMed Zwaan, R. A., Stanfield, R. A., & Yaxley, R. H. (2002). Do language comprehenders routinely represent the shapes of objects? Psychological Science, 13, 168–171.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Zwaan, R. A., & Taylor, L. J. (2006). Seeing, acting, understanding: motor resonance in language comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135(1), 1939–2222. Zwaan, R. A., & Taylor, L. J. (2006). Seeing, acting, understanding: motor resonance in language comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135(1), 1939–2222.
Metagegevens
Titel
Reading sentences describing high- or low-pitched auditory events: only pianists show evidence for a horizontal space-pitch association
Auteurs
Sibylla Wolter
Carolin Dudschig
Barbara Kaup
Publicatiedatum
12-10-2016
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 6/2017
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-016-0812-z

Andere artikelen Uitgave 6/2017

Psychological Research 6/2017 Naar de uitgave