Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 4/2017

24-06-2016 | Original Article

Impact of optokinetic stimulation on mental arithmetic

Auteurs: Nicolas Masson, Mauro Pesenti, Valérie Dormal

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 4/2017

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Solving arithmetic problems has been shown to induce shifts of spatial attention, subtraction problems orienting attention to the left side, and addition problems to the right side of space. At the neurofunctional level, the activations elicited by the solving of arithmetical problems resemble those elicited by horizontal eye movements. Whether overt orientation of attention (i.e., eye movements) can be linked to the solving procedure is, however, still under debate. In the present study, we used optokinetic stimulation (OKS) to trigger automatic eye movements to orient participants’ overt attention to the right or to the left of their visual field while they were solving addition or subtraction problems. The results show that, in comparison to leftward OKS and a control condition, rightward OKS facilitates the solving of addition problems that necessitate a carrying procedure. Subtraction solving was unaffected by leftward or rightward OKS. These results converge with previous findings to show that attentional shifts are functionally related to mental arithmetic processing.
Literatuur
go back to reference Ashcraft, M. H. (1992). Cognitive arithmetic: A review of data and theory. Cognition, 44, 75–106.CrossRefPubMed Ashcraft, M. H. (1992). Cognitive arithmetic: A review of data and theory. Cognition, 44, 75–106.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Berberovic, N., & Mattingley, J. B. (2003). Effects of prismatic adaptation on judgements of spatial extent in peripersonal and extrapersonal space. Neuropsychologia, 41(4), 493–503.CrossRefPubMed Berberovic, N., & Mattingley, J. B. (2003). Effects of prismatic adaptation on judgements of spatial extent in peripersonal and extrapersonal space. Neuropsychologia, 41(4), 493–503.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Binder, J., Marshall, R., Lazar, R., Benjamin, J., & Morh, J. P. (1992). Distinct syndromes of hemineglect. Archives of Neurology, 49(11), 1187–1194.CrossRefPubMed Binder, J., Marshall, R., Lazar, R., Benjamin, J., & Morh, J. P. (1992). Distinct syndromes of hemineglect. Archives of Neurology, 49(11), 1187–1194.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Bisiach, E., & Luzzatti, C. (1978). Unilateral neglect of representational space. Cortex, 14(1), 129–133.CrossRefPubMed Bisiach, E., & Luzzatti, C. (1978). Unilateral neglect of representational space. Cortex, 14(1), 129–133.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Bisiach, E., Luzzatti, C., & Perani, D. (1979). Unilateral neglect, representational schema and consciousness. Brain, 102(3), 609–618.CrossRefPubMed Bisiach, E., Luzzatti, C., & Perani, D. (1979). Unilateral neglect, representational schema and consciousness. Brain, 102(3), 609–618.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Bonato, M., Priftis, K., Marenzi, R., & Zorzi, M. (2009). Normal and impaired reflexive orienting of attention after central nonpredictive cues. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(4), 745–759.CrossRefPubMed Bonato, M., Priftis, K., Marenzi, R., & Zorzi, M. (2009). Normal and impaired reflexive orienting of attention after central nonpredictive cues. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(4), 745–759.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Bowers, D., & Heilman, K. M. (1980). Pseudoneglect: Effects of hemispace on a tactile line bisection task. Neuropsychologia, 18(4), 491–498.CrossRefPubMed Bowers, D., & Heilman, K. M. (1980). Pseudoneglect: Effects of hemispace on a tactile line bisection task. Neuropsychologia, 18(4), 491–498.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Brandt, S. A., & Stark, L. W. (1997). Spontaneous eye movements during visual imagery reflect the content of the visual scene. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9(1), 27–38.CrossRefPubMed Brandt, S. A., & Stark, L. W. (1997). Spontaneous eye movements during visual imagery reflect the content of the visual scene. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9(1), 27–38.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Campbell, J. I. D. (2005). Handbook of mathematical cognition. New York: Psychology Press. Campbell, J. I. D. (2005). Handbook of mathematical cognition. New York: Psychology Press.
go back to reference Casarotti, M., Michielin, M., Zorzi, M., & Umiltà, C. (2007). Temporal order judgment reveals how number magnitude affects visuospatial attention. Cognition, 102, 101–117.CrossRefPubMed Casarotti, M., Michielin, M., Zorzi, M., & Umiltà, C. (2007). Temporal order judgment reveals how number magnitude affects visuospatial attention. Cognition, 102, 101–117.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Colent, C., Pisella, L., Bernieri, C., Rode, G., & Rossetti, Y. (2000). Cognitive bias induced by visuo-motor adaptation to prisms: a simulation of unilateral neglect in normal individuals? NeuroReport, 11(9), 1899–1902.CrossRefPubMed Colent, C., Pisella, L., Bernieri, C., Rode, G., & Rossetti, Y. (2000). Cognitive bias induced by visuo-motor adaptation to prisms: a simulation of unilateral neglect in normal individuals? NeuroReport, 11(9), 1899–1902.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference de Vito, S., Buonocore, A., Bonnefon, J. F., & Della Sala, S. (2014). Eye movements disrupt spatial but not visual mental imagery. Cognitive Processing, 15(4), 543–549.CrossRefPubMed de Vito, S., Buonocore, A., Bonnefon, J. F., & Della Sala, S. (2014). Eye movements disrupt spatial but not visual mental imagery. Cognitive Processing, 15(4), 543–549.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Dehaene, S. (1992). Varieties of numerical abilities. Cognition, 4, 1–42. Dehaene, S. (1992). Varieties of numerical abilities. Cognition, 4, 1–42.
go back to reference Fischer, M. H., Castel, A. D., Dodd, M. D., & Pratt, J. (2003). Perceiving numbers causes shifts of spatial attention. Nature Neuroscience, 6(6), 555–556.CrossRefPubMed Fischer, M. H., Castel, A. D., Dodd, M. D., & Pratt, J. (2003). Perceiving numbers causes shifts of spatial attention. Nature Neuroscience, 6(6), 555–556.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Fischer, M. H., Warlop, N., Hill, R. L., & Fias, W. (2004). Oculomotor bias induced by number perception. Experimental Psychology, 51(2), 91–97.CrossRefPubMed Fischer, M. H., Warlop, N., Hill, R. L., & Fias, W. (2004). Oculomotor bias induced by number perception. Experimental Psychology, 51(2), 91–97.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Gallace, A., Auvray, M., & Spence, C. (2007). The modulation of haptic line bisection by a visual illusion and optokinetic stimulation. Perception, 36(7), 1003–1018. doi:10.1068/p5457.CrossRefPubMed Gallace, A., Auvray, M., & Spence, C. (2007). The modulation of haptic line bisection by a visual illusion and optokinetic stimulation. Perception, 36(7), 1003–1018. doi:10.​1068/​p5457.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Geary, D. C., Frensch, P. A., & Wiley, J. G. (1993). Simple and complex mental subtraction: strategy choice and speed-of-processing differences in younger and older adults. Psychology and Aging, 8(2), 242.CrossRefPubMed Geary, D. C., Frensch, P. A., & Wiley, J. G. (1993). Simple and complex mental subtraction: strategy choice and speed-of-processing differences in younger and older adults. Psychology and Aging, 8(2), 242.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Guariglia, C., Palermo, L., Piccardi, L., Iaria, G., & Incoccia, C. (2013). Neglecting the left side of a city square but not the left side of its clock: prevalence and characteristics of representational neglect. PLoS One, 8(7), e67390.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Guariglia, C., Palermo, L., Piccardi, L., Iaria, G., & Incoccia, C. (2013). Neglecting the left side of a city square but not the left side of its clock: prevalence and characteristics of representational neglect. PLoS One, 8(7), e67390.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Halligan, P. W., Fink, G. R., Marshall, J. C., & Vallar, G. (2003). Spatial cognition: evidence from visual neglect. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(3), 125–133.CrossRefPubMed Halligan, P. W., Fink, G. R., Marshall, J. C., & Vallar, G. (2003). Spatial cognition: evidence from visual neglect. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(3), 125–133.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hartmann, M. (2015). Numbers in the eye of the beholder: What do eye movements reveal about numerical cognition? Cognitive Processing, 16(1), 245–248.CrossRefPubMed Hartmann, M. (2015). Numbers in the eye of the beholder: What do eye movements reveal about numerical cognition? Cognitive Processing, 16(1), 245–248.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hoeckner, S. H., Moeller, K., Zauner, H., Wood, G., Haider, C., Gabner, A., & Nuerk, H. C. (2008). Impairments of the mental line for two-digits numbers in neglect. Cortex, 44(4), 429–438.CrossRefPubMed Hoeckner, S. H., Moeller, K., Zauner, H., Wood, G., Haider, C., Gabner, A., & Nuerk, H. C. (2008). Impairments of the mental line for two-digits numbers in neglect. Cortex, 44(4), 429–438.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Jewell, G., & McCourt, M. E. (2000). Pseudoneglect: a review and meta-analysis of performance factors in line bisection tasks. Neuropsychologia, 38(1), 93–110.CrossRefPubMed Jewell, G., & McCourt, M. E. (2000). Pseudoneglect: a review and meta-analysis of performance factors in line bisection tasks. Neuropsychologia, 38(1), 93–110.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Johansson, R., Holsanova, J., & Holmqvist, K. (2006). Pictures and spoken descriptions elicit similar eye movements during mental imagery, both in light and in complete darkness. Cognitive Science, 30(6), 1053–1079.CrossRefPubMed Johansson, R., Holsanova, J., & Holmqvist, K. (2006). Pictures and spoken descriptions elicit similar eye movements during mental imagery, both in light and in complete darkness. Cognitive Science, 30(6), 1053–1079.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Johansson, R., & Johansson, M. (2014). Look here, eye movements play a functional role in memory retrieval. Psychological Science, 25(1), 236–242.CrossRefPubMed Johansson, R., & Johansson, M. (2014). Look here, eye movements play a functional role in memory retrieval. Psychological Science, 25(1), 236–242.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kerkhoff, G. (2003). Modulation and rehabilitation of spatial neglect by sensory stimulation. In C. Prablanc, D. Pélisson, & Y. Rossetti (Eds.), Neural control of space coding and action production (pp. 257–271). Amsterdam: Elsevier.CrossRef Kerkhoff, G. (2003). Modulation and rehabilitation of spatial neglect by sensory stimulation. In C. Prablanc, D. Pélisson, & Y. Rossetti (Eds.), Neural control of space coding and action production (pp. 257–271). Amsterdam: Elsevier.CrossRef
go back to reference Kerkhoff, G., Keller, I., Ritter, V., & Marquardt, C. (2006). Repetitive optokinetic stimulation induces lasting recovery from visual neglect. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 24(4), 357–370.PubMed Kerkhoff, G., Keller, I., Ritter, V., & Marquardt, C. (2006). Repetitive optokinetic stimulation induces lasting recovery from visual neglect. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 24(4), 357–370.PubMed
go back to reference Knops, A., Dehaene, S., Berteletti, I., & Zorzi, M. (2014). Can approximate mental calculation account for operational momentum in addition and subtraction? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(8), 1541–1556.CrossRef Knops, A., Dehaene, S., Berteletti, I., & Zorzi, M. (2014). Can approximate mental calculation account for operational momentum in addition and subtraction? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(8), 1541–1556.CrossRef
go back to reference Knops, A., Thirion, B., Hubbard, E. M., Michel, V., & Dehaene, S. (2009a). Recruitment of an area involved in eye movements during mental arithmetic. Science, 324, 1583–1585.CrossRefPubMed Knops, A., Thirion, B., Hubbard, E. M., Michel, V., & Dehaene, S. (2009a). Recruitment of an area involved in eye movements during mental arithmetic. Science, 324, 1583–1585.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Knops, A., Viarouge, A., & Dehaene, S. (2009b). Dynamic representations underlying symbolic and nonsymbolic calculation: Evidence from the operational momentum effect. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 71(4), 803–821.CrossRef Knops, A., Viarouge, A., & Dehaene, S. (2009b). Dynamic representations underlying symbolic and nonsymbolic calculation: Evidence from the operational momentum effect. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 71(4), 803–821.CrossRef
go back to reference Laeng, B., & Teodorescu, D. S. (2002). Eye scanpaths during visual imagery reenact those of perception of the same visual scene. Cognitive Science, 26(2), 207–231.CrossRef Laeng, B., & Teodorescu, D. S. (2002). Eye scanpaths during visual imagery reenact those of perception of the same visual scene. Cognitive Science, 26(2), 207–231.CrossRef
go back to reference LeFevre, J. A., DeStefano, D., Penner-Wilger, M., & Daley, K. E. (2006). Selection of procedures in mental subtraction. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60(3), 209.CrossRefPubMed LeFevre, J. A., DeStefano, D., Penner-Wilger, M., & Daley, K. E. (2006). Selection of procedures in mental subtraction. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60(3), 209.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Lindemann, O., & Tira, M. (2011). Operational momentum in numerosity production judgments of multi-digit number problems. Journal of Psychology, 219(1), 50–57. Lindemann, O., & Tira, M. (2011). Operational momentum in numerosity production judgments of multi-digit number problems. Journal of Psychology, 219(1), 50–57.
go back to reference Loetscher, T., Bockisch, C. J., Nicholls, M. E., & Brugger, P. (2010). Eye position predicts what number you have in mind. Current Biology, 20(6), R264–R265.CrossRefPubMed Loetscher, T., Bockisch, C. J., Nicholls, M. E., & Brugger, P. (2010). Eye position predicts what number you have in mind. Current Biology, 20(6), R264–R265.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Lugli, L., Baroni, G., Anelli, F., Borghi, A. M., & Nicoletti, R. (2013). Counting is easier while experiencing a congruent motion. PLoS One, 8(5), e64500.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Lugli, L., Baroni, G., Anelli, F., Borghi, A. M., & Nicoletti, R. (2013). Counting is easier while experiencing a congruent motion. PLoS One, 8(5), e64500.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Marghetis, T., Nunez, R., & Bergen, B. (2014). Hand movements during exact arithmetic reveal systematic, dynamic spatial processing. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(8), 1579–1596.CrossRefPubMed Marghetis, T., Nunez, R., & Bergen, B. (2014). Hand movements during exact arithmetic reveal systematic, dynamic spatial processing. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(8), 1579–1596.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Marshall, J. C., & Halligan, P. W. (1989). When right goes left: an investigation of line bisection in a case of visual neglect. Cortex, 25(3), 503–515.CrossRefPubMed Marshall, J. C., & Halligan, P. W. (1989). When right goes left: an investigation of line bisection in a case of visual neglect. Cortex, 25(3), 503–515.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Masson, N., & Pesenti, M. (2014). Attentional bias induced by solving simple and complex addition and subtraction problems. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(8), 1514–1526.CrossRefPubMed Masson, N., & Pesenti, M. (2014). Attentional bias induced by solving simple and complex addition and subtraction problems. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(8), 1514–1526.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Masson, N., & Pesenti, M. (2016). Interference of lateralized distractors on arithmetic problem solving: a functional role for attention shifts in mental calculation. Psychological Research, 80(4), 640–651. doi:10.1007/s00426-015-0668-7.CrossRefPubMed Masson, N., & Pesenti, M. (2016). Interference of lateralized distractors on arithmetic problem solving: a functional role for attention shifts in mental calculation. Psychological Research, 80(4), 640–651. doi:10.​1007/​s00426-015-0668-7.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Masson, N., Pesenti, M., & Dormal, V. (2015). Duration and numerical estimation in right brain-damaged patients with and without neglect: Lack of support for a mental time line. British Journal of Psychology. doi:10.1111/bjop.12155. Masson, N., Pesenti, M., & Dormal, V. (2015). Duration and numerical estimation in right brain-damaged patients with and without neglect: Lack of support for a mental time line. British Journal of Psychology. doi:10.​1111/​bjop.​12155.
go back to reference Mathieu, R., Gourjon, A., Couderc, A., Thevenot, C., & Prado, J. (2016). Running the number line: Rapid shifts of attention in single-digit arithmetic. Cognition, 146, 229–239.CrossRefPubMed Mathieu, R., Gourjon, A., Couderc, A., Thevenot, C., & Prado, J. (2016). Running the number line: Rapid shifts of attention in single-digit arithmetic. Cognition, 146, 229–239.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Mattingley, J. B., Bradshaw, J. L., & Bradshaw, J. A. (1994). Horizontal visual motion modulates focal attention in left unilateral spatial neglect. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 57(10), 1228–1235.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Mattingley, J. B., Bradshaw, J. L., & Bradshaw, J. A. (1994). Horizontal visual motion modulates focal attention in left unilateral spatial neglect. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 57(10), 1228–1235.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference McCrink, K., Dehaene, S., & Dehaene-Lambertz, G. (2007). Moving along the number line: operational momentum in nonsymbolic arithmetic. Perception and Psychophysics, 69(8), 1324–1333.CrossRefPubMed McCrink, K., Dehaene, S., & Dehaene-Lambertz, G. (2007). Moving along the number line: operational momentum in nonsymbolic arithmetic. Perception and Psychophysics, 69(8), 1324–1333.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference McCrink, K., & Wynn, K. (2009). Operational momentum in large number addition and subtraction by 9-month-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103(4), 400–408.CrossRefPubMed McCrink, K., & Wynn, K. (2009). Operational momentum in large number addition and subtraction by 9-month-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103(4), 400–408.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Michel, C., Pisella, L., Halligan, P. W., Luauté, J., Rode, G., Boisson, D., & Rossetti, Y. (2003). Simulating unilateral neglect in normals using prism adaptation: implications for theory. Neuropsychologia, 41(1), 25–39.CrossRefPubMed Michel, C., Pisella, L., Halligan, P. W., Luauté, J., Rode, G., Boisson, D., & Rossetti, Y. (2003). Simulating unilateral neglect in normals using prism adaptation: implications for theory. Neuropsychologia, 41(1), 25–39.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Nicholls, M. E., Bradshaw, J. L., & Mattingley, J. B. (1999). Free-viewing perceptual asymmetries for the judgement of brightness, numerosity and size. Neuropsychologia, 37(3), 307–314.CrossRefPubMed Nicholls, M. E., Bradshaw, J. L., & Mattingley, J. B. (1999). Free-viewing perceptual asymmetries for the judgement of brightness, numerosity and size. Neuropsychologia, 37(3), 307–314.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Ogden, J. A. (1985). Anterior-posterior interhemispheric differences in the loci of lesions producing visual hemineglect. Brain and Cognition, 4(1), 59–75.CrossRefPubMed Ogden, J. A. (1985). Anterior-posterior interhemispheric differences in the loci of lesions producing visual hemineglect. Brain and Cognition, 4(1), 59–75.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Pizzamiglio, L., Frasca, R., Guariglia, C., Incoccia, C., & Antonucci, G. (1990). Effect of optokinetic stimulation in patients with visual neglect. Cortex, 26(4), 535–541.CrossRefPubMed Pizzamiglio, L., Frasca, R., Guariglia, C., Incoccia, C., & Antonucci, G. (1990). Effect of optokinetic stimulation in patients with visual neglect. Cortex, 26(4), 535–541.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Ranzini, M., Lisi, M., Blini, E., Pitteri, M., Treccani, B., Priftis, K., & Zorzi, M. (2015). Larger, smaller, odd or even? Task-specific effects of optokinetic stimulation on the mental number space. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 27(4), 459–470.CrossRef Ranzini, M., Lisi, M., Blini, E., Pitteri, M., Treccani, B., Priftis, K., & Zorzi, M. (2015). Larger, smaller, odd or even? Task-specific effects of optokinetic stimulation on the mental number space. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 27(4), 459–470.CrossRef
go back to reference Rode, G., Rossetti, Y., & Boisson, D. (2001). Prism adaptation improves representational neglect. Neuropsychologia, 39(11), 1250–1254.CrossRefPubMed Rode, G., Rossetti, Y., & Boisson, D. (2001). Prism adaptation improves representational neglect. Neuropsychologia, 39(11), 1250–1254.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Ruiz Fernandez, S., Rahona, J. J., Hervas, G., Vazquez, C., & Ulrich, R. (2011). Number magnitude determine gaze direction. Cortex, 47, 617–620.CrossRefPubMed Ruiz Fernandez, S., Rahona, J. J., Hervas, G., Vazquez, C., & Ulrich, R. (2011). Number magnitude determine gaze direction. Cortex, 47, 617–620.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Salillas, E., Granà, A., Juncadella, M., Rico, I., & Semenza, C. (2009). Leftward motion restores number space in neglect. Cortex, 45(6), 730–737.CrossRefPubMed Salillas, E., Granà, A., Juncadella, M., Rico, I., & Semenza, C. (2009). Leftward motion restores number space in neglect. Cortex, 45(6), 730–737.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Salvato, G., Sedda, A., & Bottini, G. (2014). In search of the disappeared half of it: 35 years of studies on representational neglect. Neuropsychology, 28(5), 706.CrossRefPubMed Salvato, G., Sedda, A., & Bottini, G. (2014). In search of the disappeared half of it: 35 years of studies on representational neglect. Neuropsychology, 28(5), 706.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Thimm, M., Fink, G. R., Küst, J., Karbe, H., Willmes, K., & Sturm, W. (2009). Recovery from hemineglect: differential neurobiological effects of optokinetic stimulation and alertness training. Cortex, 45(7), 850–862.CrossRefPubMed Thimm, M., Fink, G. R., Küst, J., Karbe, H., Willmes, K., & Sturm, W. (2009). Recovery from hemineglect: differential neurobiological effects of optokinetic stimulation and alertness training. Cortex, 45(7), 850–862.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Werner, K., & Raab, M. (2014). Moving your eyes to solution: Effects of movement on the perception of problem-solving task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(8), 1571–1578.CrossRefPubMed Werner, K., & Raab, M. (2014). Moving your eyes to solution: Effects of movement on the perception of problem-solving task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(8), 1571–1578.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wiemers, M., Bekkering, H., & Lindemann, O. (2014). Spatial interferences in mental arithmetic: Evidence from the motion-arithmetic compatibility effect. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(8), 1557–1570.CrossRefPubMed Wiemers, M., Bekkering, H., & Lindemann, O. (2014). Spatial interferences in mental arithmetic: Evidence from the motion-arithmetic compatibility effect. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(8), 1557–1570.CrossRefPubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Impact of optokinetic stimulation on mental arithmetic
Auteurs
Nicolas Masson
Mauro Pesenti
Valérie Dormal
Publicatiedatum
24-06-2016
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 4/2017
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-016-0784-z

Andere artikelen Uitgave 4/2017

Psychological Research 4/2017 Naar de uitgave