Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 2/2017

04-02-2016 | Original Article

Influence of verbal instructions on effect-based action control

Auteurs: Andreas B. Eder, David Dignath

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 2/2017

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

According to ideomotor theory, people use bidirectional associations between movements and their effects for action selection and initiation. Our experiments examined how verbal instructions of action effects influence response selection without prior experience of action effects in a separate acquisition phase. Instructions for different groups of participants specified whether they should ignore, attend, learn, or intentionally produce acoustic effects produced by button presses. Results showed that explicit instructions of action–effect relations trigger effect-congruent action tendencies in the first trials following the instruction; in contrast, no evidence for effect-based action control was observed in these trials when instructions were to ignore or to attend to the action effects. These findings show that action-effect knowledge acquired through verbal instruction and direct experience is similarly effective for effect-based action control as long as the relation between the movement and the effect is clearly spelled out in the instruction.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Literatuur
go back to reference Ansorge, U., & Wühr, P. (2004). A response-discrimination account of the Simon effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 30, 365–377.CrossRefPubMed Ansorge, U., & Wühr, P. (2004). A response-discrimination account of the Simon effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 30, 365–377.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Cohen-Kdoshay, O., & Meiran, N. (2007). The representation of instructions in working memory leads to autonomous response activation: evidence from the first trials in the flanker paradigm. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 1140–1154. Cohen-Kdoshay, O., & Meiran, N. (2007). The representation of instructions in working memory leads to autonomous response activation: evidence from the first trials in the flanker paradigm. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 1140–1154.
go back to reference Cohen-Kdoshay, O., & Meiran, N. (2009). The representation of instructions operates like a prepared reflex: Flanker compatibility effects found in first trial following S–R instructions. Experimental Psychology, 56, 128–133. doi:10.1027/1618-3169.56.2.128.CrossRefPubMed Cohen-Kdoshay, O., & Meiran, N. (2009). The representation of instructions operates like a prepared reflex: Flanker compatibility effects found in first trial following S–R instructions. Experimental Psychology, 56, 128–133. doi:10.​1027/​1618-3169.​56.​2.​128.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference De Houwer, J., Beckers, T., Vandorpe, S., & Custers, R. (2005). Further evidence for the role of mode-independent short-term associations in spatial Simon effects. Perception and Psychophysics, 67, 659–666.CrossRefPubMed De Houwer, J., Beckers, T., Vandorpe, S., & Custers, R. (2005). Further evidence for the role of mode-independent short-term associations in spatial Simon effects. Perception and Psychophysics, 67, 659–666.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Dignath, D., Pfister, R., Eder, A. B., Kiesel, A., & Kunde, W. (2014). Representing the hyphen in action–effect associations: automatic acquisition and bidirectional retrieval of action–effect intervals. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40, 1701–1712. doi:10.1037/xlm0000022.CrossRefPubMed Dignath, D., Pfister, R., Eder, A. B., Kiesel, A., & Kunde, W. (2014). Representing the hyphen in action–effect associations: automatic acquisition and bidirectional retrieval of action–effect intervals. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40, 1701–1712. doi:10.​1037/​xlm0000022.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Elsner, B., & Hommel, B. (2001). Effect anticipation and action control. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 27, 229–240.CrossRefPubMed Elsner, B., & Hommel, B. (2001). Effect anticipation and action control. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 27, 229–240.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Elsner, B., & Hommel, B. (2004). Contiguity and contingency in action–effect learning. Psychological Research, 68, 138–154.CrossRefPubMed Elsner, B., & Hommel, B. (2004). Contiguity and contingency in action–effect learning. Psychological Research, 68, 138–154.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Everaert, T., Theeuwes, M., Liefooghe, B., & De Houwer, J. (2014). Automatic motor activation by mere instruction. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 14, 1300–1309. doi:10.3758/s13415-014-0294-7.CrossRef Everaert, T., Theeuwes, M., Liefooghe, B., & De Houwer, J. (2014). Automatic motor activation by mere instruction. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 14, 1300–1309. doi:10.​3758/​s13415-014-0294-7.CrossRef
go back to reference Flach, R., Osman, M., Dickinson, A., & Heyes, C. (2006). The interaction between response effects during the acquisition of response priming. Acta Psychologica, 122, 11–26.CrossRefPubMed Flach, R., Osman, M., Dickinson, A., & Heyes, C. (2006). The interaction between response effects during the acquisition of response priming. Acta Psychologica, 122, 11–26.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999). Implementation intentions: strong effects of simple plans. American Psychologist, 54, 493–503.CrossRef Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999). Implementation intentions: strong effects of simple plans. American Psychologist, 54, 493–503.CrossRef
go back to reference Herwig, A., Prinz, W., & Waszak, F. (2007). Two modes of sensorimotor integration in intention-based and stimulus-based actions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 1540–1554.CrossRef Herwig, A., Prinz, W., & Waszak, F. (2007). Two modes of sensorimotor integration in intention-based and stimulus-based actions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 1540–1554.CrossRef
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1993). Inverting the Simon effect by intention: determinants of direction and extent of effects of irrelevant spatial information. Psychological Research, 55, 270–279.CrossRef Hommel, B. (1993). Inverting the Simon effect by intention: determinants of direction and extent of effects of irrelevant spatial information. Psychological Research, 55, 270–279.CrossRef
go back to reference Hommel, B. (2000). The prepared reflex: automaticity and control in stimulus-response translation. In S. Monsell & J. Driver (Eds.), Control of cognitive processes: attention and performance (Vol. XVIII, pp. 247–273). Cambridge: MIT Press. Hommel, B. (2000). The prepared reflex: automaticity and control in stimulus-response translation. In S. Monsell & J. Driver (Eds.), Control of cognitive processes: attention and performance (Vol. XVIII, pp. 247–273). Cambridge: MIT Press.
go back to reference Hommel, B. (2013). Ideomotor action control: on the perceptual grounding of voluntary actions and agents. In W. Prinz, M. Beisert, & A. Herwig (Eds.), Action science: foundations of an emerging discipline (pp. 113–136). Cambridge: MIT Press. Hommel, B. (2013). Ideomotor action control: on the perceptual grounding of voluntary actions and agents. In W. Prinz, M. Beisert, & A. Herwig (Eds.), Action science: foundations of an emerging discipline (pp. 113–136). Cambridge: MIT Press.
go back to reference Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–937.CrossRefPubMed Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–937.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Liefooghe, B., Wenke, D., & De Houwer, J. (2012). Instruction-based task-rule congruency effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38, 1325–1335. doi:10.1037/a0028148.CrossRefPubMed Liefooghe, B., Wenke, D., & De Houwer, J. (2012). Instruction-based task-rule congruency effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38, 1325–1335. doi:10.​1037/​a0028148.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Meiran, N., Pereg, M., Kessler, Y., Cole, M. W., & Braver, T. S. (2015a). The power of instructions: proactive configuration of stimulus–response translation. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 41(3), 768–786. doi:10.1037/xlm0000063.CrossRefPubMed Meiran, N., Pereg, M., Kessler, Y., Cole, M. W., & Braver, T. S. (2015a). The power of instructions: proactive configuration of stimulus–response translation. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 41(3), 768–786. doi:10.​1037/​xlm0000063.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Meiran, N., Pereg, M., Kessler, Y., Cole, M. W., & Braver, T. S. (2015b). Reflexive activation of newly instructed stimulus–response rules: evidence from lateralized readiness potentials in no-go trials. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 15, 365–373. doi:10.3758/s13415-014-0321-8.CrossRefPubMed Meiran, N., Pereg, M., Kessler, Y., Cole, M. W., & Braver, T. S. (2015b). Reflexive activation of newly instructed stimulus–response rules: evidence from lateralized readiness potentials in no-go trials. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 15, 365–373. doi:10.​3758/​s13415-014-0321-8.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Nattkemper, D., Ziessler, M., & Frensch, P. A. (2010). Binding in voluntary action control. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 34, 1092–1101.CrossRefPubMed Nattkemper, D., Ziessler, M., & Frensch, P. A. (2010). Binding in voluntary action control. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 34, 1092–1101.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Pfister, R., Kiesel, A., & Hoffmann, J. (2011). Learning at any rate: action–effect learning for stimulus-based actions. Psychological Research, 75, 61–65.CrossRefPubMed Pfister, R., Kiesel, A., & Hoffmann, J. (2011). Learning at any rate: action–effect learning for stimulus-based actions. Psychological Research, 75, 61–65.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Pfister, R., Kiesel, A., & Melcher, T. (2010). Adaptive control of ideomotor effect anticipations. Acta Psychologica, 135, 316–322.CrossRefPubMed Pfister, R., Kiesel, A., & Melcher, T. (2010). Adaptive control of ideomotor effect anticipations. Acta Psychologica, 135, 316–322.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Shin, Y. K., Proctor, R. W., & Capaldi, E. J. (2010). A review of contemporary ideomotor theory. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 943–974.CrossRefPubMed Shin, Y. K., Proctor, R. W., & Capaldi, E. J. (2010). A review of contemporary ideomotor theory. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 943–974.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Theeuwes, M., Liefooghe, B., & De Houwer, J. (2014). Eliminating the Simon effect by instruction. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40, 1470–1480. doi:10.1037/a0036913.CrossRefPubMed Theeuwes, M., Liefooghe, B., & De Houwer, J. (2014). Eliminating the Simon effect by instruction. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40, 1470–1480. doi:10.​1037/​a0036913.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Tukey, J. W. (1977). Exploratory data analysis. Reading, MA: Addison Wesley Tukey, J. W. (1977). Exploratory data analysis. Reading, MA: Addison Wesley
go back to reference Wenke, D., De Houwer, J., Winne, J., & Liefooghe, B. (2014). Learning through instructions vs. learning through practice: Flanker congruency effects from instructed and applied S–R mappings. Psychological Research,. doi:10.1007/s00426-014-0621-1. (Online advance publication).PubMedPubMedCentral Wenke, D., De Houwer, J., Winne, J., & Liefooghe, B. (2014). Learning through instructions vs. learning through practice: Flanker congruency effects from instructed and applied S–R mappings. Psychological Research,. doi:10.​1007/​s00426-014-0621-1. (Online advance publication).PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Wenke, D., Gaschler, R., & Nattkemper, D. (2007). Instruction-induced feature binding. Psychological Research, 71, 92–106.CrossRefPubMed Wenke, D., Gaschler, R., & Nattkemper, D. (2007). Instruction-induced feature binding. Psychological Research, 71, 92–106.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wolfensteller, U., & Ruge, H. (2011). On the timescale of stimulus-based action–effect learning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 1273–1289.CrossRef Wolfensteller, U., & Ruge, H. (2011). On the timescale of stimulus-based action–effect learning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 1273–1289.CrossRef
go back to reference Wulf, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). Directing attention to movement effects enhances learning: a review. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 8, 648–660.CrossRefPubMed Wulf, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). Directing attention to movement effects enhances learning: a review. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 8, 648–660.CrossRefPubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Influence of verbal instructions on effect-based action control
Auteurs
Andreas B. Eder
David Dignath
Publicatiedatum
04-02-2016
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 2/2017
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-016-0745-6

Andere artikelen Uitgave 2/2017

Psychological Research 2/2017 Naar de uitgave