Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 6/2016

26-09-2015 | Original Article

Latent profiles of executive functioning in healthy young adults: evidence of individual differences in hemispheric asymmetry

Auteurs: Holly K. Rau, Yana Suchy, Jonathan E. Butner, Paula G. Williams

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 6/2016

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Two competing theoretical models of individual differences in executive functioning (EF) were examined: the Prefrontal Convexity Model and the Hemispheric Asymmetry Model. Neurologically healthy individuals (N = 315; mean age 20.8) completed a modified switching task (MST) and the Attention Network Test (ANT) in a single testing session. Data analysis was conducted in two phases. In the first phase (model identification), latent profile analysis was applied to MST variables measuring the abilities to form, switch, and maintain mental sets under conditions designed to tax left or right hemisphere resources. In the second phase (model validation), participant clusters obtained from the first phase were compared on the ANT. The Model Identification phase yielded a 3-profile solution consistent with the Hemispheric Asymmetry Model. Profile 1 (N = 203) was characterized by average EF performances. Profile 2 (N = 43) revealed a set maintenance weakness under non-verbal conditions. Profile 3 (N = 38) demonstrated weaknesses in cognitive flexibility combined with poor executive performances under verbal conditions. The Model Validation phase confirmed group differences. Profile 1 demonstrated average EF performances. Profile 2 demonstrated distractibility and decreased alertness, consistent with a right hemisphere weakness. Profile 3 demonstrated cognitive rigidity in the absence of external cues, consistent with a left hemisphere weakness. Individual differences in EF appear to follow a Hemispheric Asymmetry Model of EF among neurologically healthy adults. Investigating the relationship between hemispherically mediated executive functions and other individual difference factors known to confer health risk or resilience could inform numerous disciplines within the field of psychology.
Literatuur
go back to reference Adolfsdottir, S., Sorensen, L., & Lundervold, A. J. (2008). The attention network test: a characteristic pattern of deficits in children with ADHD. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 4, 9.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Adolfsdottir, S., Sorensen, L., & Lundervold, A. J. (2008). The attention network test: a characteristic pattern of deficits in children with ADHD. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 4, 9.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Adrover-Roig, D., Sese, A., Barcelo, F., & Palmer, A. (2012). A latent variable approach to executive control in healthy ageing. Brain and Cognition, 78(3), 284–299.PubMedCrossRef Adrover-Roig, D., Sese, A., Barcelo, F., & Palmer, A. (2012). A latent variable approach to executive control in healthy ageing. Brain and Cognition, 78(3), 284–299.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Allport, D. A., Styles, E. A., & Hsieh, S. (1994). Shifting intentional set: exploring the dynamic control of tasks. In Attention and performance: Conscious and nonconscious information processing (Vol. 15, pp. 421-452). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Allport, D. A., Styles, E. A., & Hsieh, S. (1994). Shifting intentional set: exploring the dynamic control of tasks. In Attention and performance: Conscious and nonconscious information processing (Vol. 15, pp. 421-452). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
go back to reference Almerigi, J. B., Carbary, T. J., & Harris, L. J. (2002). Most adults show opposite-side biases in the imagined holding of infants and objects. Brain and Cognition, 48(2–3), 258–263.PubMed Almerigi, J. B., Carbary, T. J., & Harris, L. J. (2002). Most adults show opposite-side biases in the imagined holding of infants and objects. Brain and Cognition, 48(2–3), 258–263.PubMed
go back to reference Asanowicz, D., Marzecova, A., Jaskowski, P., & Wolski, P. (2012). Hemispheric asymmetry in the efficiency of attentional networks. Brain and Cognition, 79, 117–128.PubMedCrossRef Asanowicz, D., Marzecova, A., Jaskowski, P., & Wolski, P. (2012). Hemispheric asymmetry in the efficiency of attentional networks. Brain and Cognition, 79, 117–128.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Baddeley, A. D., Chincotta, D., & Adlam, A. (2001). Working memory and the control of action: evidence from task switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 641–657.CrossRef Baddeley, A. D., Chincotta, D., & Adlam, A. (2001). Working memory and the control of action: evidence from task switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 641–657.CrossRef
go back to reference Baker, S. C., Rogers, R. D., Owen, A. M., Frith, C. D., Dolan, R. J., Frackowiak, R. S., et al. (1996). Neural systems engaged by planning: a PET study of the Tower of London task. Neuropsychologia, 34(6), 515–526.PubMedCrossRef Baker, S. C., Rogers, R. D., Owen, A. M., Frith, C. D., Dolan, R. J., Frackowiak, R. S., et al. (1996). Neural systems engaged by planning: a PET study of the Tower of London task. Neuropsychologia, 34(6), 515–526.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bamdad, M. J., Ryan, L. M., & Warden, D. L. (2003). Functional assessment of executive abilities following traumatic brain injury. Brain Injury, 17(12), 1011–1020.PubMedCrossRef Bamdad, M. J., Ryan, L. M., & Warden, D. L. (2003). Functional assessment of executive abilities following traumatic brain injury. Brain Injury, 17(12), 1011–1020.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Barbey, A. K., Colom, R., Solomon, J., Krueger, F., Forbes, C., & Grafman, J. (2012). An integrative architecture for general intelligence and executive function revealed by lesion mapping. Brain, 135, 1154–1164.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Barbey, A. K., Colom, R., Solomon, J., Krueger, F., Forbes, C., & Grafman, J. (2012). An integrative architecture for general intelligence and executive function revealed by lesion mapping. Brain, 135, 1154–1164.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Bauer, D. J. (2007). Observations on the use of growth mixture models in psychological research. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 42(4), 757–786.CrossRef Bauer, D. J. (2007). Observations on the use of growth mixture models in psychological research. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 42(4), 757–786.CrossRef
go back to reference Beane, M., & Marrocco, R. T. (2004). Norepinephrine and acetylcholine mediation of the components of reflexive attention: Implications for attention deficit disorders. Progress in Neurobiology, 74(3), 167–181.PubMedCrossRef Beane, M., & Marrocco, R. T. (2004). Norepinephrine and acetylcholine mediation of the components of reflexive attention: Implications for attention deficit disorders. Progress in Neurobiology, 74(3), 167–181.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bedson, E., & Turnbull, O. H. (2002). Hemispheric asymmetry for global and local processing: language is less important than one might think. Brain and Cognition, 48, 272–277.PubMed Bedson, E., & Turnbull, O. H. (2002). Hemispheric asymmetry for global and local processing: language is less important than one might think. Brain and Cognition, 48, 272–277.PubMed
go back to reference Beer, J. S., John, O. P., Scabini, D., & Knight, R. T. (2006). Orbitofrontal cortex and social behavior: Integrating self-monitoring and emotion-cognition interactions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(6), 871–879.PubMedCrossRef Beer, J. S., John, O. P., Scabini, D., & Knight, R. T. (2006). Orbitofrontal cortex and social behavior: Integrating self-monitoring and emotion-cognition interactions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(6), 871–879.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Berthoz, S., Armony, J. L., Blair, R. J., & Dolan, R. J. (2002). An fMRI study of intentional and unintentional (embarrassing) violations of social norms. Brain, 125(Pt 8), 1696–1708.PubMedCrossRef Berthoz, S., Armony, J. L., Blair, R. J., & Dolan, R. J. (2002). An fMRI study of intentional and unintentional (embarrassing) violations of social norms. Brain, 125(Pt 8), 1696–1708.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Blumenfeld, R. S., & Ranganath, C. (2006). Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex promotes long-term memory formation through its role in working memory organization. The Journal of Neuroscience, 26(3), 916–925.PubMedCrossRef Blumenfeld, R. S., & Ranganath, C. (2006). Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex promotes long-term memory formation through its role in working memory organization. The Journal of Neuroscience, 26(3), 916–925.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Boone, K. B., Ponton, M. O., Gorsuch, R. L., Gonzalez, J. J., & Miller, B. L. (1998). Factor analysis of four measures of prefrontal lobe functioning. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 13(7), 585–595.PubMedCrossRef Boone, K. B., Ponton, M. O., Gorsuch, R. L., Gonzalez, J. J., & Miller, B. L. (1998). Factor analysis of four measures of prefrontal lobe functioning. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 13(7), 585–595.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Brass, M., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2002). The role of the frontal cortex in task preparation. Cerebral Cortex, 12, 908–914.PubMedCrossRef Brass, M., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2002). The role of the frontal cortex in task preparation. Cerebral Cortex, 12, 908–914.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Brazil, I. A., de Bruijn, E. R., Bulten, B. H., von Borries, A. K., van Lankveld, J. J., Buitelaar, J. K., et al. (2009). Early and late components of error monitoring in violent offenders with psychopathy. Biological Psychiatray, 65(2), 137–143.CrossRef Brazil, I. A., de Bruijn, E. R., Bulten, B. H., von Borries, A. K., van Lankveld, J. J., Buitelaar, J. K., et al. (2009). Early and late components of error monitoring in violent offenders with psychopathy. Biological Psychiatray, 65(2), 137–143.CrossRef
go back to reference Burgess, P. W., Alderman, N., Evans, J., Emslie, H., & Wilson, B. A. (1998). The ecological validity of tests of executive function. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 4(6), 547–558.PubMedCrossRef Burgess, P. W., Alderman, N., Evans, J., Emslie, H., & Wilson, B. A. (1998). The ecological validity of tests of executive function. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 4(6), 547–558.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Busch, R. M., McBride, A., Curtiss, G., & Vanderploeg, R. D. (2005). The components of executive functioning in traumatic brain injury. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 27(8), 1022–1032.PubMedCrossRef Busch, R. M., McBride, A., Curtiss, G., & Vanderploeg, R. D. (2005). The components of executive functioning in traumatic brain injury. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 27(8), 1022–1032.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bush, G., Luu, P., & Posner, M. I. (2000). Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(6), 215–222.PubMedCrossRef Bush, G., Luu, P., & Posner, M. I. (2000). Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(6), 215–222.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cepeda, N. J., Kramer, A. F., & Gonzalez de Sather, J. C. (2001). Changes in executive control across the life span: examination of task-switching performance. Developmental Psychology, 37(5), 715–730.PubMedCrossRef Cepeda, N. J., Kramer, A. F., & Gonzalez de Sather, J. C. (2001). Changes in executive control across the life span: examination of task-switching performance. Developmental Psychology, 37(5), 715–730.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cohen, G. (1972). Hemispheric differences in a letter classification task. Perception & Psychophysics, 11(2), 139–142. Cohen, G. (1972). Hemispheric differences in a letter classification task. Perception & Psychophysics, 11(2), 139–142.
go back to reference Collette, F., Van der Linden, M., Laureys, S., Delfiore, G., Degueldre, C., Luxen, A., & Salmon, E. (2005). Exploring the unity and diversity of the neural substrates of executive functioning. Human Brain Mapping, 25(4), 409–423. Collette, F., Van der Linden, M., Laureys, S., Delfiore, G., Degueldre, C., Luxen, A., & Salmon, E. (2005). Exploring the unity and diversity of the neural substrates of executive functioning. Human Brain Mapping, 25(4), 409–423.
go back to reference Colvin, M. K., Dunbar, K., & Grafman, J. (2001). The effects of frontal lobe lesions on goal achievement in the water jug task. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13(8), 1129–1147.PubMedCrossRef Colvin, M. K., Dunbar, K., & Grafman, J. (2001). The effects of frontal lobe lesions on goal achievement in the water jug task. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13(8), 1129–1147.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Connors, C. K. (2000). Continuous performance test ii. Toronto: Multi-Health System. Connors, C. K. (2000). Continuous performance test ii. Toronto: Multi-Health System.
go back to reference Coull, J. T., Frith, C. D., Buchel, C., & Nobre, A. C. (2000). Orienting attention in time: behavioural and neuroanatomical distinction between exogenous and endogenous shifts. Neuropsychologia, 38(6), 808–819.PubMedCrossRef Coull, J. T., Frith, C. D., Buchel, C., & Nobre, A. C. (2000). Orienting attention in time: behavioural and neuroanatomical distinction between exogenous and endogenous shifts. Neuropsychologia, 38(6), 808–819.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Coull, J. T., Frith, C. D., Frackowiak, R. S., & Grasby, P. M. (1996). A fronto-parietal network for rapid visual information processing: a PET study of sustained attention and working memory. Neuropsychologia, 34(11), 1085–1095.PubMedCrossRef Coull, J. T., Frith, C. D., Frackowiak, R. S., & Grasby, P. M. (1996). A fronto-parietal network for rapid visual information processing: a PET study of sustained attention and working memory. Neuropsychologia, 34(11), 1085–1095.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference D’Espositio, M., Aguirre, G.K., Zarahn, E., Ballard, D., Shin, R.K., & Lease, J. (1998). Functional MRI studies of spatial and nonspatial working memory. Cognitive Brain Research, 7, 1-13. D’Espositio, M., Aguirre, G.K., Zarahn, E., Ballard, D., Shin, R.K., & Lease, J. (1998). Functional MRI studies of spatial and nonspatial working memory. Cognitive Brain Research, 7, 1-13.
go back to reference Davidson, R. J. (1992). Emotion and affective style: hemispheric substrates. Psychological Science, 3(1), 39–43.CrossRef Davidson, R. J. (1992). Emotion and affective style: hemispheric substrates. Psychological Science, 3(1), 39–43.CrossRef
go back to reference Davis, R. N., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2000). Cognitive inflexibility among ruminators and nonruminators. Psychological Medicine, 24, 699–711. Davis, R. N., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2000). Cognitive inflexibility among ruminators and nonruminators. Psychological Medicine, 24, 699–711.
go back to reference Doiseau, F., & Isingrini, M. (2005). Updating information in verbal working memory and executive functioning. Psychological Reports, 96(1), 67–76.PubMedCrossRef Doiseau, F., & Isingrini, M. (2005). Updating information in verbal working memory and executive functioning. Psychological Reports, 96(1), 67–76.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Dreher, J. C., & Grafman, J. (2003). Dissociating the roles of the rostral anterior cingulate and the lateral prefrontal cortices in performing two tasks simultaneously or successively. Cerebral Cortex, 13(4), 329–339.PubMedCrossRef Dreher, J. C., & Grafman, J. (2003). Dissociating the roles of the rostral anterior cingulate and the lateral prefrontal cortices in performing two tasks simultaneously or successively. Cerebral Cortex, 13(4), 329–339.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Duffy, J. D., & Campbell, J. J, 3rd. (1994). The regional prefrontal syndromes: a theoretical and clinical overview. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 6(4), 379–387.CrossRef Duffy, J. D., & Campbell, J. J, 3rd. (1994). The regional prefrontal syndromes: a theoretical and clinical overview. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 6(4), 379–387.CrossRef
go back to reference Duffy, J. D., Campbell, J. J, 3rd, Salloway, S. P., & Malloy, P. F. (2001). Regional prefrontal syndromes: a theoretical and clinical overview. In S. P. Salloway, P. F. Malloy, & J. D. Duffy (Eds.), The frontal lobes and neuropsychiatric illness (pp. 113–123). Arlington: American Psychiatric Publishing. Duffy, J. D., Campbell, J. J, 3rd, Salloway, S. P., & Malloy, P. F. (2001). Regional prefrontal syndromes: a theoretical and clinical overview. In S. P. Salloway, P. F. Malloy, & J. D. Duffy (Eds.), The frontal lobes and neuropsychiatric illness (pp. 113–123). Arlington: American Psychiatric Publishing.
go back to reference Durston, S., Davidson, M. C., Tottenham, N., Galvan, A., Spicer, J., Fossella, J. A., & Casey, B. J. (2006). A shift from diffuse to focal cortical activity with development. Developmental Science, 9(1), 1–8.PubMedCrossRef Durston, S., Davidson, M. C., Tottenham, N., Galvan, A., Spicer, J., Fossella, J. A., & Casey, B. J. (2006). A shift from diffuse to focal cortical activity with development. Developmental Science, 9(1), 1–8.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Emerson, M. J., & Miyake, A. (2003). The role of inner speech in task switching: a dual-task investigation. Journal of Memory and Language, 48, 148–168.CrossRef Emerson, M. J., & Miyake, A. (2003). The role of inner speech in task switching: a dual-task investigation. Journal of Memory and Language, 48, 148–168.CrossRef
go back to reference Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception and Psychophysics, 16, 143–149.CrossRef Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception and Psychophysics, 16, 143–149.CrossRef
go back to reference Eslinger, P. J., Biddle, K., Pennington, B., & Page, R. B. (1999). Cognitive and behavioral development up to 4 years after early right frontal lobe lesion. Developmental Neuropsychology, 15(2), 157–191.CrossRef Eslinger, P. J., Biddle, K., Pennington, B., & Page, R. B. (1999). Cognitive and behavioral development up to 4 years after early right frontal lobe lesion. Developmental Neuropsychology, 15(2), 157–191.CrossRef
go back to reference Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Fossella, J., Flombaum, J. I., & Posner, M. I. (2005). The activation of attentional networks. Neuroimage, 26(2), 471–479.PubMedCrossRef Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Fossella, J., Flombaum, J. I., & Posner, M. I. (2005). The activation of attentional networks. Neuroimage, 26(2), 471–479.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., & Posner, M. I. (2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(3), 340–347.PubMedCrossRef Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., & Posner, M. I. (2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(3), 340–347.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Farah, M. J., Gazzaniga, M. S., Holtzman, J. D., & Kosslyn, S. M. (1985). A left hemisphere basis for visual mental imagery? Neuropsychologia, 23(1), 115–118.PubMedCrossRef Farah, M. J., Gazzaniga, M. S., Holtzman, J. D., & Kosslyn, S. M. (1985). A left hemisphere basis for visual mental imagery? Neuropsychologia, 23(1), 115–118.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fasotti, L., Bremer, J. J. C. B., & Eling, P. A. T. M. (1992). Influence of improved text encoding on arithmetical word problem solving after frontal lobe damage. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation: An International Journal, 2(1), 3–20.CrossRef Fasotti, L., Bremer, J. J. C. B., & Eling, P. A. T. M. (1992). Influence of improved text encoding on arithmetical word problem solving after frontal lobe damage. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation: An International Journal, 2(1), 3–20.CrossRef
go back to reference Fernandez-Duque, D., & Posner, M. I. (2001). Brain imaging of attentional networks in normal and pathological states. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 23(1), 74–93.PubMedCrossRef Fernandez-Duque, D., & Posner, M. I. (2001). Brain imaging of attentional networks in normal and pathological states. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 23(1), 74–93.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Goel, V., Gold, B., Kapur, S., & Houle, S. (1997). The seats of reason? An imaging study of deductive and inductive reasoning. Neuroreport, 8(5), 1305–1310.PubMedCrossRef Goel, V., Gold, B., Kapur, S., & Houle, S. (1997). The seats of reason? An imaging study of deductive and inductive reasoning. Neuroreport, 8(5), 1305–1310.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Goel, V., Gold, B., Kapur, S., & Houle, S. (1998). Neuroanatomical correlates of human reasoning. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10(3), 293–302.PubMedCrossRef Goel, V., Gold, B., Kapur, S., & Houle, S. (1998). Neuroanatomical correlates of human reasoning. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10(3), 293–302.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Goldman, R. S., Axelrod, B. N., Heaton, R. K., & Chelune, G. J. (1996). Latent structure of the WCST with the standardization samples. Assessment, 3(1), 73. Goldman, R. S., Axelrod, B. N., Heaton, R. K., & Chelune, G. J. (1996). Latent structure of the WCST with the standardization samples. Assessment, 3(1), 73.
go back to reference Gooding, D. C., Braun, J. G., & Studer, J. A. (2006). Attentional network task performance in patients with schizophrenia-spectrum disorders: Evidence of a specific deficit. Schizophrenia Research, 88(1–3), 169–178.PubMedCrossRef Gooding, D. C., Braun, J. G., & Studer, J. A. (2006). Attentional network task performance in patients with schizophrenia-spectrum disorders: Evidence of a specific deficit. Schizophrenia Research, 88(1–3), 169–178.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gootjes, L., Raij, T., Salmelin, R., & Hari, R. (1999). Left-hemisphere dominance for processing of vowels: a whole-scalp neuromagnetic study. NeuroReport, 10(14), 2987–2991.PubMedCrossRef Gootjes, L., Raij, T., Salmelin, R., & Hari, R. (1999). Left-hemisphere dominance for processing of vowels: a whole-scalp neuromagnetic study. NeuroReport, 10(14), 2987–2991.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gopher, D., Armony, L., & Greenshpan, Y. (2000). Switching tasks and attention policies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129(3), 308–339.CrossRef Gopher, D., Armony, L., & Greenshpan, Y. (2000). Switching tasks and attention policies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129(3), 308–339.CrossRef
go back to reference Greene, D. J., Barnea, A., Herzberg, K., Rassis, A., Neta, M., Raz, A., et al. (2008). Measuring attention in the hemispheres: the lateralized attention network test (LANT). Brain and Cognition, 66(1), 21–31.PubMedCrossRef Greene, D. J., Barnea, A., Herzberg, K., Rassis, A., Neta, M., Raz, A., et al. (2008). Measuring attention in the hemispheres: the lateralized attention network test (LANT). Brain and Cognition, 66(1), 21–31.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Grimshaw, G. M. (1998). Integration and interference in the cerebral hemispheres: relations with hemispheric specialization. Brain and Cognition, 36(2), 108–127.PubMedCrossRef Grimshaw, G. M. (1998). Integration and interference in the cerebral hemispheres: relations with hemispheric specialization. Brain and Cognition, 36(2), 108–127.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Grinband, J., Savitskaya, J., Wager, T. D., Teichert, T., Ferrera, V. P., & Hirsch, J. (2011). The dorsal medial frontal cortex is sensitive to time on task, not response conflict or error likelihood. Neuroimage, 57(2), 303–311.PubMedCrossRef Grinband, J., Savitskaya, J., Wager, T. D., Teichert, T., Ferrera, V. P., & Hirsch, J. (2011). The dorsal medial frontal cortex is sensitive to time on task, not response conflict or error likelihood. Neuroimage, 57(2), 303–311.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gur, R. (1980). Cognitive task effects on hemispheric blood flow in humans: evidence for individual differences in hemispheric activation. Brain and Language, 9(1), 78–92.PubMedCrossRef Gur, R. (1980). Cognitive task effects on hemispheric blood flow in humans: evidence for individual differences in hemispheric activation. Brain and Language, 9(1), 78–92.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hagemann, D., Naumann, E., Lurken, A., Becker, G., Maier, S., & Bartussek, D. (1999). EEG asymmetry, dispositional mood and personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 27(3), 541–568.CrossRef Hagemann, D., Naumann, E., Lurken, A., Becker, G., Maier, S., & Bartussek, D. (1999). EEG asymmetry, dispositional mood and personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 27(3), 541–568.CrossRef
go back to reference Halterman, C. I., Langan, J., Drew, A., Rodriguez, E., Osternig, L. R., Chous, L., & van Donkelaar, P. (2006). Tracking the recovery of visuospatial attention deficits in mild traumatic brain injury. Brain, 129, 747–753.PubMedCrossRef Halterman, C. I., Langan, J., Drew, A., Rodriguez, E., Osternig, L. R., Chous, L., & van Donkelaar, P. (2006). Tracking the recovery of visuospatial attention deficits in mild traumatic brain injury. Brain, 129, 747–753.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hardman, E., Gruzelier, J., Cheesman, K., Jones, C., Liddiard, D., Schleichert, H., & Birbaumer, N. (1997). Frontal interhemispheric asymmetry: self regulation and individual differences in humans. Neuroscience Letters, 221(2–3), 117–120.PubMedCrossRef Hardman, E., Gruzelier, J., Cheesman, K., Jones, C., Liddiard, D., Schleichert, H., & Birbaumer, N. (1997). Frontal interhemispheric asymmetry: self regulation and individual differences in humans. Neuroscience Letters, 221(2–3), 117–120.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Heaton, R. K., Chelune, G. J., Talley, J. L., Kay, G. G., & Curtiss, G. (1993). Wisconsin card sorting test manual: revised and expanded. Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc. Heaton, R. K., Chelune, G. J., Talley, J. L., Kay, G. G., & Curtiss, G. (1993). Wisconsin card sorting test manual: revised and expanded. Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.
go back to reference Henson, J. M., Reise, S. P., & Kim, K. H. (2007). Detecting mixtures from structural model differences using latent variable mixture modeling: a comparison of relative model fit statistics. Structural Equation Modeling, 14(2), 202–226.CrossRef Henson, J. M., Reise, S. P., & Kim, K. H. (2007). Detecting mixtures from structural model differences using latent variable mixture modeling: a comparison of relative model fit statistics. Structural Equation Modeling, 14(2), 202–226.CrossRef
go back to reference Hornak, J., O’Doherty, J., Bramham, J., Rolls, E. T., Morris, R. G., Bullock, P. R., et al. (2004). Reward-related reversal learning after surgical excisions in orbito-frontal or dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(3), 463–478.PubMedCrossRef Hornak, J., O’Doherty, J., Bramham, J., Rolls, E. T., Morris, R. G., Bullock, P. R., et al. (2004). Reward-related reversal learning after surgical excisions in orbito-frontal or dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(3), 463–478.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hull, R., Martin, R. C., Beier, M. E., Lane, D., & Hamilton, A. C. (2008). Executive function in older adults: a structural equation modeling approach. Neuropsychology, 22(4), 508–522.PubMedCrossRef Hull, R., Martin, R. C., Beier, M. E., Lane, D., & Hamilton, A. C. (2008). Executive function in older adults: a structural equation modeling approach. Neuropsychology, 22(4), 508–522.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Jager, G., & Postma, A. (2003). On the hemispheric specialization for categorical and coordinate spatial relations: a review of the current evidence. Neuropsychologia, 41(4), 504–515.PubMedCrossRef Jager, G., & Postma, A. (2003). On the hemispheric specialization for categorical and coordinate spatial relations: a review of the current evidence. Neuropsychologia, 41(4), 504–515.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Jersild, A. T. (1927). Mental set and shift. Archives of Psychology, 89, 5–82. Jersild, A. T. (1927). Mental set and shift. Archives of Psychology, 89, 5–82.
go back to reference Johnson, K. A., Robertson, I. H., Barry, E., Mulligan, A., Daibhis, A., Daly, M., et al. (2008). Impaired conflict resolution and alerting in children with ADHD: evidence from the attention network task (ANT). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49(12), 1339–1347.PubMedCrossRef Johnson, K. A., Robertson, I. H., Barry, E., Mulligan, A., Daibhis, A., Daly, M., et al. (2008). Impaired conflict resolution and alerting in children with ADHD: evidence from the attention network task (ANT). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49(12), 1339–1347.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Karnath, H.-O., & Kammer, T. (2003). Manifestationen von Frontalhirnschadigungen. In H.-O. Karnath & P. Thier (Eds.), Neuropsychologie (pp. 515–528). Berlin: Springer.CrossRef Karnath, H.-O., & Kammer, T. (2003). Manifestationen von Frontalhirnschadigungen. In H.-O. Karnath & P. Thier (Eds.), Neuropsychologie (pp. 515–528). Berlin: Springer.CrossRef
go back to reference Keele, S. W., & Rafal, R. (2000). Deficits of task-set in patients with left prefrontal cortex lesions. In S. Monsell & J. S. Driver (Eds.), Control of cognitive processes: Attention and performance (Vol. XVIII, pp. 627–651). Cambridge: MIT Press. Keele, S. W., & Rafal, R. (2000). Deficits of task-set in patients with left prefrontal cortex lesions. In S. Monsell & J. S. Driver (Eds.), Control of cognitive processes: Attention and performance (Vol. XVIII, pp. 627–651). Cambridge: MIT Press.
go back to reference Keita, L., & Bedoin, N. (2011). Hemispheric asymmetries in hierarchical stimulus processing are modulated by stimulus categories and their predictability. Laterality, 16, 333–355.PubMed Keita, L., & Bedoin, N. (2011). Hemispheric asymmetries in hierarchical stimulus processing are modulated by stimulus categories and their predictability. Laterality, 16, 333–355.PubMed
go back to reference Keita, L., Bedoin, N., Burack, J. A., & Lepore, F. (2014). Switching between global and local levels: the level repetition effect and its hemispheric asymmetry. Frontiers in Psychology, 5(252), 1–9. Keita, L., Bedoin, N., Burack, J. A., & Lepore, F. (2014). Switching between global and local levels: the level repetition effect and its hemispheric asymmetry. Frontiers in Psychology, 5(252), 1–9.
go back to reference Kim, J., & Ragozzino, M. E. (2005). The involvement of the orbitofrontal cortex in learning under changing task contingencies. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 83(2), 125–133.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Kim, J., & Ragozzino, M. E. (2005). The involvement of the orbitofrontal cortex in learning under changing task contingencies. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 83(2), 125–133.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Kinsbourne, M. (1982). Hemispheric specialization and the growth of human understanding. American Psychologist, 37(4), 411–420.PubMedCrossRef Kinsbourne, M. (1982). Hemispheric specialization and the growth of human understanding. American Psychologist, 37(4), 411–420.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Knecht, S., Deppe, M., Drager, B., Bobe, L., Lohmann, H., Ringelstein, E. B., et al. (2000a). Language lateralization in healthy right-handers. Brain, 123, 74–81.PubMedCrossRef Knecht, S., Deppe, M., Drager, B., Bobe, L., Lohmann, H., Ringelstein, E. B., et al. (2000a). Language lateralization in healthy right-handers. Brain, 123, 74–81.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Knecht, S., Drager, B., Deppe, M., Bobe, L., Lohmann, H., Ringelstein, E. B., et al. (2000b). Handedness and hemispheric language dominance in healthy humans. Brain, 123, 2512–2518.PubMedCrossRef Knecht, S., Drager, B., Deppe, M., Bobe, L., Lohmann, H., Ringelstein, E. B., et al. (2000b). Handedness and hemispheric language dominance in healthy humans. Brain, 123, 2512–2518.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kosslyn, S. M., Koenig, O., Barrett, A., Cave, C. B., Tang, J., & Gabrieli, J. D. (1989). Evidence for two types of spatial representations: Hemispheric specialization for categorical and coordinate relations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15(4), 723–735.PubMed Kosslyn, S. M., Koenig, O., Barrett, A., Cave, C. B., Tang, J., & Gabrieli, J. D. (1989). Evidence for two types of spatial representations: Hemispheric specialization for categorical and coordinate relations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15(4), 723–735.PubMed
go back to reference Kouneiher, F., Charron, S., & Koechlin, E. (2009). Motivation and cognitive control in the human prefrontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 12(7), 939–945.PubMedCrossRef Kouneiher, F., Charron, S., & Koechlin, E. (2009). Motivation and cognitive control in the human prefrontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 12(7), 939–945.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kray, J., Li, K. Z., & Lindenberger, U. (2002). Age-related changes in task-switching components: the role of task uncertainty. Brain and Cognition, 49(3), 363–381.PubMedCrossRef Kray, J., Li, K. Z., & Lindenberger, U. (2002). Age-related changes in task-switching components: the role of task uncertainty. Brain and Cognition, 49(3), 363–381.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kray, J., & Lindenberger, U. (2000). Adult age differences in task switching. Psychology and Aging, 15(1), 126–147.PubMedCrossRef Kray, J., & Lindenberger, U. (2000). Adult age differences in task switching. Psychology and Aging, 15(1), 126–147.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kringelbach, M. L. (2005). The human orbitofrontal cortex: Linking reward to hedonic experience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6(9), 691–702.PubMedCrossRef Kringelbach, M. L. (2005). The human orbitofrontal cortex: Linking reward to hedonic experience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6(9), 691–702.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kroger, J. K., Sabb, F. W., Fales, C. L., Bookheimer, S. Y., Cohen, M. S., & Holyoak, K. J. (2002). Recruitment of anterior dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in human reasoning: a parametric study of relational complexity. Cerebral Cortex, 12(5), 477–485.PubMedCrossRef Kroger, J. K., Sabb, F. W., Fales, C. L., Bookheimer, S. Y., Cohen, M. S., & Holyoak, K. J. (2002). Recruitment of anterior dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in human reasoning: a parametric study of relational complexity. Cerebral Cortex, 12(5), 477–485.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Langdon, D., & Warrington, E. K. (2000). The role of the left hemisphere in verbal and spatial reasoning tasks. Cortex, 36(5), 691–702.PubMedCrossRef Langdon, D., & Warrington, E. K. (2000). The role of the left hemisphere in verbal and spatial reasoning tasks. Cortex, 36(5), 691–702.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Langner, R., & Eickhoff, S. B. (2013). Sustaining attention to simple tasks: a meta-analytic review of the neural mechanisms of vigilant attention. Psychological Bulletin, 139(4), 870–900.PubMedCrossRef Langner, R., & Eickhoff, S. B. (2013). Sustaining attention to simple tasks: a meta-analytic review of the neural mechanisms of vigilant attention. Psychological Bulletin, 139(4), 870–900.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lanza, S., Flaherty, B. P., & Collins, L. M. (2003). Latent class and latent transition analysis (Vol. 2). New York: Wiley. Lanza, S., Flaherty, B. P., & Collins, L. M. (2003). Latent class and latent transition analysis (Vol. 2). New York: Wiley.
go back to reference Lidstone, J. S. M., Fernyhough, C., Meins, E., & Whitehouse, A. J. O. (2009). Brief report: inner speech impairment in children with autism is associated with greater nonverbal than verbal skills. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(8), 1222–1225.PubMedCrossRef Lidstone, J. S. M., Fernyhough, C., Meins, E., & Whitehouse, A. J. O. (2009). Brief report: inner speech impairment in children with autism is associated with greater nonverbal than verbal skills. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(8), 1222–1225.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lo, Y., Mendell, N. R., & Rubin, D. B. (2001). Testing the number of components in a normal mixture. Biometrika, 88, 767–778.CrossRef Lo, Y., Mendell, N. R., & Rubin, D. B. (2001). Testing the number of components in a normal mixture. Biometrika, 88, 767–778.CrossRef
go back to reference Lombardi, W. J., Andreason, P. J., Sirocco, K. Y., Rio, D. E., Gross, R. E., Umhau, J. C., et al. (1999). Wisconsin card sorting test performance following head injury: dorsolateral fronto-striatal circuit activity predicts perseveration. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 21(1), 2–16.PubMedCrossRef Lombardi, W. J., Andreason, P. J., Sirocco, K. Y., Rio, D. E., Gross, R. E., Umhau, J. C., et al. (1999). Wisconsin card sorting test performance following head injury: dorsolateral fronto-striatal circuit activity predicts perseveration. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 21(1), 2–16.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Loring, D. W., Meador, K. J., & Lee, G. P. (1989). Differential-handed response to verbal and visual spatial stimuli: evidence of specialized hemispheric processing following callosotomy. Neuropsychologia, 27(6), 811–827.PubMedCrossRef Loring, D. W., Meador, K. J., & Lee, G. P. (1989). Differential-handed response to verbal and visual spatial stimuli: evidence of specialized hemispheric processing following callosotomy. Neuropsychologia, 27(6), 811–827.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lorist, M. M., Klein, M., Nieuwenhuis, S., De Jong, R., Mulder, G., & Meijman, T. F. (2000). Mental fatigue and task control: planning and preparation. Psychophysiology, 37(5), 614–625.PubMedCrossRef Lorist, M. M., Klein, M., Nieuwenhuis, S., De Jong, R., Mulder, G., & Meijman, T. F. (2000). Mental fatigue and task control: planning and preparation. Psychophysiology, 37(5), 614–625.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lubke, G., & Neale, M. C. (2006). Distinguishing between latent classes and continuous factors: resolution by maximum likelihood? Multivariate Behavioral Research, 41, 499–532.PubMedCrossRef Lubke, G., & Neale, M. C. (2006). Distinguishing between latent classes and continuous factors: resolution by maximum likelihood? Multivariate Behavioral Research, 41, 499–532.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Luria, A. R., & Tsvetkova, L. S. (1967). Toward the mechanisms of ‘dynamic aphasia’. Acta Neurologica et Psychiatrica Belgica, 67(11), 1045–1057.PubMed Luria, A. R., & Tsvetkova, L. S. (1967). Toward the mechanisms of ‘dynamic aphasia’. Acta Neurologica et Psychiatrica Belgica, 67(11), 1045–1057.PubMed
go back to reference MacDonald, A. W, 3rd, Cohen, J. D., Stenger, V. A., & Carter, C. S. (2000). Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. Science, 288(5472), 1835–1838.PubMedCrossRef MacDonald, A. W, 3rd, Cohen, J. D., Stenger, V. A., & Carter, C. S. (2000). Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. Science, 288(5472), 1835–1838.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mah, L., Arnold, M. C., & Grafman, J. (2004). Impairment of social perception associated with lesions of the prefrontal cortex. American Journal of Psychiatry, 161(7), 1247–1255.PubMedCrossRef Mah, L., Arnold, M. C., & Grafman, J. (2004). Impairment of social perception associated with lesions of the prefrontal cortex. American Journal of Psychiatry, 161(7), 1247–1255.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Marsh, J. E., Pilgrim, L. K., & Sorqvist, P. (2013). Hemispheric specialization in selective attention and short-term memory: a fine course model of left- and right-ear disadvantages. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(976), 1–6. Marsh, J. E., Pilgrim, L. K., & Sorqvist, P. (2013). Hemispheric specialization in selective attention and short-term memory: a fine course model of left- and right-ear disadvantages. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(976), 1–6.
go back to reference Martin, A. (1999). Automatic activation of the medial temporal lobe during encoding: lateralized influences of meaning and novelty. Hippocampus, 9(1), 62–70.PubMedCrossRef Martin, A. (1999). Automatic activation of the medial temporal lobe during encoding: lateralized influences of meaning and novelty. Hippocampus, 9(1), 62–70.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mathia, J. L., Beall, J. A., & Bigler, E. D. (2004). Neuropsychological and information processing deficits following mild traumatic brain injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 10(2), 286–297. Mathia, J. L., Beall, J. A., & Bigler, E. D. (2004). Neuropsychological and information processing deficits following mild traumatic brain injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 10(2), 286–297.
go back to reference McGuire, P. K., Silersweig, D. A., Murray, R. M., David, A. S., Frackowiak, R. S., & Frith, C. D. (1996). Functional anatomy of inner speech and auditory verbal imagery. Psychological Medicine, 26(1), 29–38.PubMedCrossRef McGuire, P. K., Silersweig, D. A., Murray, R. M., David, A. S., Frackowiak, R. S., & Frith, C. D. (1996). Functional anatomy of inner speech and auditory verbal imagery. Psychological Medicine, 26(1), 29–38.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mecklinger, A. D., von Cramon, D. Y., Springer, A., & Matthes-von Cramon, G. (1999). Executive control functions in task switching: evidence from brain injured patients. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 21(5), 606–619.PubMedCrossRef Mecklinger, A. D., von Cramon, D. Y., Springer, A., & Matthes-von Cramon, G. (1999). Executive control functions in task switching: evidence from brain injured patients. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 21(5), 606–619.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mega, M. S., & Cummings, J. L. (2001). Frontal subcortical circuits: anatomy and function. In S. P. Salloway, P. F. Malloy, & J. D. Duffy (Eds.), The frontal lobes and neuropsychiatric illness (pp. 15–32). Washington D.C.: American Psychiatric Publishing Inc. Mega, M. S., & Cummings, J. L. (2001). Frontal subcortical circuits: anatomy and function. In S. P. Salloway, P. F. Malloy, & J. D. Duffy (Eds.), The frontal lobes and neuropsychiatric illness (pp. 15–32). Washington D.C.: American Psychiatric Publishing Inc.
go back to reference Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24, 167–202.PubMedCrossRef Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24, 167–202.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Miyake, A., Emerson, M. J., Padilla, F., & Ahn, J. C. (2004). Inner speech as a retrieval aid for task goals: the effects of cue type and articulatory suppression in the random task cuing paradigm. Acta Psychologia (Amst.), 115(2–3), 123–142.CrossRef Miyake, A., Emerson, M. J., Padilla, F., & Ahn, J. C. (2004). Inner speech as a retrieval aid for task goals: the effects of cue type and articulatory suppression in the random task cuing paradigm. Acta Psychologia (Amst.), 115(2–3), 123–142.CrossRef
go back to reference Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(1), 8–14.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(1), 8–14.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49–100.PubMedCrossRef Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49–100.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Monsell, S., Yeung, N., & Azuma, R. (2000). Reconfiguration of task-set: is it easier to switch to the weaker task? Psychological Research, 63(3–4), 250–264.PubMedCrossRef Monsell, S., Yeung, N., & Azuma, R. (2000). Reconfiguration of task-set: is it easier to switch to the weaker task? Psychological Research, 63(3–4), 250–264.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Morin, A., & Michaud, J. (2007). Self-awareness and the left inferior frontal gyrus: inner speech use during self-related processing. Brain Research Bulletin, 74(6), 387–396.PubMedCrossRef Morin, A., & Michaud, J. (2007). Self-awareness and the left inferior frontal gyrus: inner speech use during self-related processing. Brain Research Bulletin, 74(6), 387–396.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Muthen, L. K., & Muthen, B. O. (2009). Mplus user’s guide (5th ed.). Los Angeles. Muthen, L. K., & Muthen, B. O. (2009). Mplus user’s guide (5th ed.). Los Angeles.
go back to reference Nagahama, Y., Okina, T., Suzuki, N., Matsuzaki, S., Yamauchi, H., Nabatame, H., et al. (2003). Factor structure of a modified version of the wisconsin card sorting test: an analysis of executive deficit in Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 16(2), 103–112.PubMedCrossRef Nagahama, Y., Okina, T., Suzuki, N., Matsuzaki, S., Yamauchi, H., Nabatame, H., et al. (2003). Factor structure of a modified version of the wisconsin card sorting test: an analysis of executive deficit in Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 16(2), 103–112.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Nagel, B. J., Herting, M. M., Maxwell, E. C., Bruno, R., & Fair, D. (2013). Hemispheric lateralization of verbal and spatial working memory during adolescence. Brain and Cognition, 82, 58–68.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Nagel, B. J., Herting, M. M., Maxwell, E. C., Bruno, R., & Fair, D. (2013). Hemispheric lateralization of verbal and spatial working memory during adolescence. Brain and Cognition, 82, 58–68.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Nagin, D. S. (1999). Analyzing developmental trajectories: a semi-parametric group-based approach. Psychological Methods, 4, 139–157.CrossRef Nagin, D. S. (1999). Analyzing developmental trajectories: a semi-parametric group-based approach. Psychological Methods, 4, 139–157.CrossRef
go back to reference Nathaniel-James, D. A., & Frith, C. D. (2002). The role of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex: evidence from the effects of contextual constraint in a sentence completion task. Neuroimage, 16(4), 1094–1102.PubMedCrossRef Nathaniel-James, D. A., & Frith, C. D. (2002). The role of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex: evidence from the effects of contextual constraint in a sentence completion task. Neuroimage, 16(4), 1094–1102.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Newman, S. D., Carpenter, P. A., Varma, S., & Just, M. A. (2003). Frontal and parietal participation in problem solving in the Tower of London: fMRI and computational modeling of planning and high-level perception. Neuropsychologia, 41(12), 1668–1682.PubMedCrossRef Newman, S. D., Carpenter, P. A., Varma, S., & Just, M. A. (2003). Frontal and parietal participation in problem solving in the Tower of London: fMRI and computational modeling of planning and high-level perception. Neuropsychologia, 41(12), 1668–1682.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Newman, J. P., Patterson, C. M., & Kosson, D. S. (1987). Response perseveration in psychopaths. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 96(2), 145–148.PubMedCrossRef Newman, J. P., Patterson, C. M., & Kosson, D. S. (1987). Response perseveration in psychopaths. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 96(2), 145–148.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Newman, J. P., Schmitt, W. A., & Voss, W. D. (1997). The impact of motivationally neutral cues on psychopathic individuals: assessing the generality of the response modulation hypothesis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106(4), 563–575.PubMedCrossRef Newman, J. P., Schmitt, W. A., & Voss, W. D. (1997). The impact of motivationally neutral cues on psychopathic individuals: assessing the generality of the response modulation hypothesis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106(4), 563–575.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Nylund, K. L., Asparouhov, T., & Muthen, B. O. (2007). Deciding on the number of classes in latent class analysis and growth mixture modeling: a Monte Carlo simulation study. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 14, 535–569.CrossRef Nylund, K. L., Asparouhov, T., & Muthen, B. O. (2007). Deciding on the number of classes in latent class analysis and growth mixture modeling: a Monte Carlo simulation study. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 14, 535–569.CrossRef
go back to reference Oberlin, B. G., Alford, J. L., & Marrocco, R. T. (2005). Normal attention orienting but abnormal stimulus alerting and conflict effect in combined subtype of ADHD. Behavioural Brain Research, 165(1), 1–11.PubMedCrossRef Oberlin, B. G., Alford, J. L., & Marrocco, R. T. (2005). Normal attention orienting but abnormal stimulus alerting and conflict effect in combined subtype of ADHD. Behavioural Brain Research, 165(1), 1–11.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ocklenburg, S., Gunturkun, O., & Beste, C. (2011). Lateralized neural mechanisms underlying the modulation of response inhibition processes. NeuroImage, 55, 1771–1778.PubMedCrossRef Ocklenburg, S., Gunturkun, O., & Beste, C. (2011). Lateralized neural mechanisms underlying the modulation of response inhibition processes. NeuroImage, 55, 1771–1778.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ocklenburg, S., Gunturkun, O., & Beste, C. (2012). Hemispheric asymmetries and cognitive flexibility: an ERP and sLORETA study. Brain and Cognition, 78, 148–155.PubMedCrossRef Ocklenburg, S., Gunturkun, O., & Beste, C. (2012). Hemispheric asymmetries and cognitive flexibility: an ERP and sLORETA study. Brain and Cognition, 78, 148–155.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97–113.PubMedCrossRef Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97–113.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Osmon, D. C. (1996). Understanding symptoms of medial frontal lobe disorder: a clinical case study. Journal of Clinical Psychology in Medical Settings, 3(1), 23–39.PubMedCrossRef Osmon, D. C. (1996). Understanding symptoms of medial frontal lobe disorder: a clinical case study. Journal of Clinical Psychology in Medical Settings, 3(1), 23–39.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Owen, A. M., Stern, C. E., Look, R. B., Tracey, I., Rosen, B. R., & Petrides, M. (1998). Functional organization of spatial and nonspatial working memory processing within the human lateral frontal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 95(13), 7721–7726.CrossRef Owen, A. M., Stern, C. E., Look, R. B., Tracey, I., Rosen, B. R., & Petrides, M. (1998). Functional organization of spatial and nonspatial working memory processing within the human lateral frontal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 95(13), 7721–7726.CrossRef
go back to reference Pare, N., Rabin, L. A., Fogel, J., & Pepin, M. (2009). Mild traumatic brain injury and its sequelae: characterisation of divided attention deficits. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation: An International Journal, 19(1), 110–137.CrossRef Pare, N., Rabin, L. A., Fogel, J., & Pepin, M. (2009). Mild traumatic brain injury and its sequelae: characterisation of divided attention deficits. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation: An International Journal, 19(1), 110–137.CrossRef
go back to reference Paus, T., Collins, D. L., Evans, A. C., Leonard, G., Pike, B., & Zijdenbos, A. (2001). Maturation of white matter in the human brain: a review of magnetic resonance studies. Brain Research Bulletin, 54(3), 255–266.PubMedCrossRef Paus, T., Collins, D. L., Evans, A. C., Leonard, G., Pike, B., & Zijdenbos, A. (2001). Maturation of white matter in the human brain: a review of magnetic resonance studies. Brain Research Bulletin, 54(3), 255–266.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Phan, K. L., Taylor, S. F., Welsh, R. C., Decker, L. R., Noll, D. C., Nichols, T. E., et al. (2003). Activation of the medial prefrontal cortex and extended amygdala by individual ratings of emotional arousal: a fMRI study. Biological Psychiatry, 53(3), 211–215.PubMedCrossRef Phan, K. L., Taylor, S. F., Welsh, R. C., Decker, L. R., Noll, D. C., Nichols, T. E., et al. (2003). Activation of the medial prefrontal cortex and extended amygdala by individual ratings of emotional arousal: a fMRI study. Biological Psychiatry, 53(3), 211–215.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Picton, T. W., Stuss, D. T., Shallice, T., Alexander, M. P., & Gillingham, S. (2006). Keeping time: effects of focal frontal lesions. Neuropsychologia, 44(7), 1195–1209.PubMedCrossRef Picton, T. W., Stuss, D. T., Shallice, T., Alexander, M. P., & Gillingham, S. (2006). Keeping time: effects of focal frontal lesions. Neuropsychologia, 44(7), 1195–1209.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Piguet, O., Grayson, D. A., Tate, R. L., Bennett, H. P., Lye, T. C., Creasey, H., et al. (2005). A model of executive functions in very old community dwellers: evidence from The Sydney Older Persons Study. Cortex, 41(1), 27–37.PubMedCrossRef Piguet, O., Grayson, D. A., Tate, R. L., Bennett, H. P., Lye, T. C., Creasey, H., et al. (2005). A model of executive functions in very old community dwellers: evidence from The Sydney Older Persons Study. Cortex, 41(1), 27–37.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pochon, J. B., Levy, R., Poline, J. B., Crozier, S., Lehericy, S., Pillon, B., et al. (2001). The role of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in the preparation of forthcoming actions: an fMRI study. Cerebral Cortex, 11(3), 260–266.PubMedCrossRef Pochon, J. B., Levy, R., Poline, J. B., Crozier, S., Lehericy, S., Pillon, B., et al. (2001). The role of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in the preparation of forthcoming actions: an fMRI study. Cerebral Cortex, 11(3), 260–266.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Posner, M. I., Snyder, C. R., & Davidson, B. J. (1980). Attention and the detection of signals. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 109(2), 160–174.PubMedCrossRef Posner, M. I., Snyder, C. R., & Davidson, B. J. (1980). Attention and the detection of signals. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 109(2), 160–174.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Prabhakaran, V., Narayanan, K., Zhao, Z., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2000). Integration of diverse information in working memory within the frontal lobe. Nature Neuroscience, 3(1), 85–90.PubMedCrossRef Prabhakaran, V., Narayanan, K., Zhao, Z., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2000). Integration of diverse information in working memory within the frontal lobe. Nature Neuroscience, 3(1), 85–90.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pujol, J., Vendrell, P., Deus, J., Junque, C., Bello, J., Marti-Vilalta, J. L., et al. (2001). The effect of medial frontal and posterior parietal demyelinating lesions on stroop interference. Neuroimage, 13(1), 68–75.PubMedCrossRef Pujol, J., Vendrell, P., Deus, J., Junque, C., Bello, J., Marti-Vilalta, J. L., et al. (2001). The effect of medial frontal and posterior parietal demyelinating lesions on stroop interference. Neuroimage, 13(1), 68–75.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ridderinkhof, K. R., Ullsperger, M., Crone, E. A., & Nieuwenhuis, S. (2004). The role of the medial frontal cortex in cognitive control. Science, 306, 443–447.PubMedCrossRef Ridderinkhof, K. R., Ullsperger, M., Crone, E. A., & Nieuwenhuis, S. (2004). The role of the medial frontal cortex in cognitive control. Science, 306, 443–447.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rogers, R. D., Sahakian, B. J., Hodges, J. R., Polkey, C. E., Kennard, C., & Robbins, T. W. (1998). Dissociating executive mechanisms of task control following frontal lobe damage and Parkinson’s disease. Brain, 121(Pt 5), 815–842.PubMedCrossRef Rogers, R. D., Sahakian, B. J., Hodges, J. R., Polkey, C. E., Kennard, C., & Robbins, T. W. (1998). Dissociating executive mechanisms of task control following frontal lobe damage and Parkinson’s disease. Brain, 121(Pt 5), 815–842.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rueckert, L., & Grafman, J. (1998). Sustained attention deficits in patients with lesions of posterior cortex. Neuropsychologia, 36(7), 653–660.PubMedCrossRef Rueckert, L., & Grafman, J. (1998). Sustained attention deficits in patients with lesions of posterior cortex. Neuropsychologia, 36(7), 653–660.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rushworth, M. F., Walton, M. E., Kennerley, S. W., & Bannerman, D. M. (2004). Action sets and decisions in the medial frontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(9), 410–417.PubMedCrossRef Rushworth, M. F., Walton, M. E., Kennerley, S. W., & Bannerman, D. M. (2004). Action sets and decisions in the medial frontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(9), 410–417.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sackeim, H. A. (1978). Lateral asymmetry in intensity of emotional expression. Neuropsychologia, 16(4), 473–481.PubMedCrossRef Sackeim, H. A. (1978). Lateral asymmetry in intensity of emotional expression. Neuropsychologia, 16(4), 473–481.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Savla, G. N., Twamley, E. W., Delis, D. C., Roesch, S. C., Jeste, D. V., & Palmer, B. W. (2012). Dimensions of executive functioning in schizophrenia and their relationship with processing speed. Schizophria Bulletin, 38(4), 760–768.CrossRef Savla, G. N., Twamley, E. W., Delis, D. C., Roesch, S. C., Jeste, D. V., & Palmer, B. W. (2012). Dimensions of executive functioning in schizophrenia and their relationship with processing speed. Schizophria Bulletin, 38(4), 760–768.CrossRef
go back to reference Scherf, K. S., Sweeney, J. A., & Luna, B. (2006). Brain basis of developmental change in visuospatial working memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(7), 1045–1058.PubMedCrossRef Scherf, K. S., Sweeney, J. A., & Luna, B. (2006). Brain basis of developmental change in visuospatial working memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(7), 1045–1058.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Schretlen, D., Pearlson, G. D., Anthony, J. C., Aylward, E. H., Augustine, A. M., Davis, A., et al. (2000). Elucidating the contributions of processing speed, executive ability, and frontal lobe volume to normal age-related differences in fluid intelligence. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6(1), 52–61.PubMedCrossRef Schretlen, D., Pearlson, G. D., Anthony, J. C., Aylward, E. H., Augustine, A. M., Davis, A., et al. (2000). Elucidating the contributions of processing speed, executive ability, and frontal lobe volume to normal age-related differences in fluid intelligence. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6(1), 52–61.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sclove, L. S. (1987). Application of model-selection criteria to some problems in multivariate analysis. Psychometrika, 52, 333–343.CrossRef Sclove, L. S. (1987). Application of model-selection criteria to some problems in multivariate analysis. Psychometrika, 52, 333–343.CrossRef
go back to reference Shimamura, A. P. (2000). The role of the prefrontal cortex in dynamic filtering. Psychobiology, 28(2), 207–218. Shimamura, A. P. (2000). The role of the prefrontal cortex in dynamic filtering. Psychobiology, 28(2), 207–218.
go back to reference Smith, E. E., & Jonides, J. (1997). Working memory: a view from neuroimaging. Cognitive Psychology, 33, 5–42.PubMedCrossRef Smith, E. E., & Jonides, J. (1997). Working memory: a view from neuroimaging. Cognitive Psychology, 33, 5–42.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Smith, E. E., Jonides, J., Koeppe, R. A., Awh, E., Schumacher, E. H., & Minoshima, S. (1995). Spatial versus object working memory: PET investigations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7(3), 337–356.PubMedCrossRef Smith, E. E., Jonides, J., Koeppe, R. A., Awh, E., Schumacher, E. H., & Minoshima, S. (1995). Spatial versus object working memory: PET investigations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7(3), 337–356.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Smithson, M. (2003). Confidence intervals. Sage University Papers Series on Quantitative Applications in the Social Sciences, 07–140. Thousand Oaks: Sage. Smithson, M. (2003). Confidence intervals. Sage University Papers Series on Quantitative Applications in the Social Sciences, 07–140. Thousand Oaks: Sage.
go back to reference Stefanatos, G. A., & Wasserstein, J. (2001). Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder as a right hemisphere syndrome. Selective literature review and detailed neuropsychological case studies. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 931, 172–195.PubMedCrossRef Stefanatos, G. A., & Wasserstein, J. (2001). Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder as a right hemisphere syndrome. Selective literature review and detailed neuropsychological case studies. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 931, 172–195.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Stelzel, C., Kraft, A., Brandt, S. A., & Schubert, T. (2008). Dissociable neural effects on task order control and task set maintenance during dual-task processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(4), 613–628.PubMedCrossRef Stelzel, C., Kraft, A., Brandt, S. A., & Schubert, T. (2008). Dissociable neural effects on task order control and task set maintenance during dual-task processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(4), 613–628.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Stiles, J., Moses, P., Passarotti, A., Dick, F. K., & Buxton, R. (2003). Exploring developmental change in the neural bases of higher cognitive functions: the promise of functional magnetic resonance imaging. Developmental Neuropsychology, 24(2–3), 641–668.PubMedCrossRef Stiles, J., Moses, P., Passarotti, A., Dick, F. K., & Buxton, R. (2003). Exploring developmental change in the neural bases of higher cognitive functions: the promise of functional magnetic resonance imaging. Developmental Neuropsychology, 24(2–3), 641–668.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Stock, A.-K., & Beste, C. (2014). Lateralization of spatial information processing in response monitoring. Frontiers in Psychology, 5(22), 1–8. Stock, A.-K., & Beste, C. (2014). Lateralization of spatial information processing in response monitoring. Frontiers in Psychology, 5(22), 1–8.
go back to reference Stock, A.-K., Wascher, E., & Beste, C. (2013). Differential effects of motor efference copies and proprioceptive information on response evaluation processes. PLoS ONE, 8(4), e62335.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Stock, A.-K., Wascher, E., & Beste, C. (2013). Differential effects of motor efference copies and proprioceptive information on response evaluation processes. PLoS ONE, 8(4), e62335.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Stout, J. C., Ready, R. E., Grace, J., Malloy, P. F., & Paulsen, J. S. (2003). Factor analysis of the frontal systems behavior scale (FrSBe). Assessment, 10(1), 79–85.PubMedCrossRef Stout, J. C., Ready, R. E., Grace, J., Malloy, P. F., & Paulsen, J. S. (2003). Factor analysis of the frontal systems behavior scale (FrSBe). Assessment, 10(1), 79–85.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sturm, W., & Willmes, K. (2001). On the functional neuroanatomy of intrinsic and phasic alertness. Neuroimage, 14(1 Pt 2), S76–S84.PubMedCrossRef Sturm, W., & Willmes, K. (2001). On the functional neuroanatomy of intrinsic and phasic alertness. Neuroimage, 14(1 Pt 2), S76–S84.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Stuss, D. T., Alexander, M. P., Floden, D., Binns, M. A., Levine, B., McIntosh, A. R., et al. (2002). Fractionation and localization of distinct frontal lobe processes: evidence from focal lesions in humans. In D. T. Stuss & R. T. Knight (Eds.), Principles of frontal lobe function (pp. 392–407). London: Oxford University Press.CrossRef Stuss, D. T., Alexander, M. P., Floden, D., Binns, M. A., Levine, B., McIntosh, A. R., et al. (2002). Fractionation and localization of distinct frontal lobe processes: evidence from focal lesions in humans. In D. T. Stuss & R. T. Knight (Eds.), Principles of frontal lobe function (pp. 392–407). London: Oxford University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Stuss, D. T., Floden, D., Alexander, M. P., Levine, B., & Katz, D. (2001). Stroop performance in focal lesion patients: dissociation of processes and frontal lobe lesion location. Neuropsychologia, 39, 771–786.PubMedCrossRef Stuss, D. T., Floden, D., Alexander, M. P., Levine, B., & Katz, D. (2001). Stroop performance in focal lesion patients: dissociation of processes and frontal lobe lesion location. Neuropsychologia, 39, 771–786.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Suchy, Y. (2009). Executive functioning: overview, assessment, and research issues for non-neuropsychologists. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 37(2), 106–116.PubMedCrossRef Suchy, Y. (2009). Executive functioning: overview, assessment, and research issues for non-neuropsychologists. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 37(2), 106–116.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Suchy, Y., Gold, A., Biechler, R., & Osmon, D. C. (2003). Set maintenance and switching differences in college students with inattentive and impulsive features. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 18, 765–766. Suchy, Y., Gold, A., Biechler, R., & Osmon, D. C. (2003). Set maintenance and switching differences in college students with inattentive and impulsive features. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 18, 765–766.
go back to reference Suchy, Y., & Kosson, D. S. (2006). Forming, switching, and maintaining mental sets among psychopathic offenders during verbal and nonverbal tasks: another look at the left-hemisphere activation hypothesis. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 12(4), 538–548.PubMedCrossRef Suchy, Y., & Kosson, D. S. (2006). Forming, switching, and maintaining mental sets among psychopathic offenders during verbal and nonverbal tasks: another look at the left-hemisphere activation hypothesis. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 12(4), 538–548.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sylvester, C. Y., Wager, T. D., Lacey, S. C., Hernandez, L., Nichols, T. E., Smith, E. E., et al. (2003). Switching attention and resolving interference: fMRI measures of executive functions. Neuropsychologia, 41(3), 357–370.PubMedCrossRef Sylvester, C. Y., Wager, T. D., Lacey, S. C., Hernandez, L., Nichols, T. E., Smith, E. E., et al. (2003). Switching attention and resolving interference: fMRI measures of executive functions. Neuropsychologia, 41(3), 357–370.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Takio, F., Koivisto, M., & Hamalainen, H. (2014). The influence of executive functions on spatial biases varies during the lifespan. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 170–180.PubMedCrossRef Takio, F., Koivisto, M., & Hamalainen, H. (2014). The influence of executive functions on spatial biases varies during the lifespan. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 170–180.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Takio, F., Koivisto, M., Jokiranta, L., Rashind, F., Kallio, J., Tuominen, T., … Hamalainen, H. (2009). The effect of age on attentional modulation in dichotic listening. Developmental Neuropsychology, 34(3), 225–239.PubMedCrossRef Takio, F., Koivisto, M., Jokiranta, L., Rashind, F., Kallio, J., Tuominen, T., … Hamalainen, H. (2009). The effect of age on attentional modulation in dichotic listening. Developmental Neuropsychology, 34(3), 225–239.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Thomason, M. E., Race, E., Burrows, B., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., Glove, H. G., & Babrieli, J. D. E. (2009). Development of spatial and verbal working memory capacity in the human brain. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(2), 316–332.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Thomason, M. E., Race, E., Burrows, B., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., Glove, H. G., & Babrieli, J. D. E. (2009). Development of spatial and verbal working memory capacity in the human brain. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(2), 316–332.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Tofighi, D., & Enders, C. K. (2007). Identifying the correct number of classes in growth mixture models. In G. R. Hancock & K. M. Samuelsen (Eds.), Advances in latent variable mixture models (pp. 317–341). Greewich: Information Age. Tofighi, D., & Enders, C. K. (2007). Identifying the correct number of classes in growth mixture models. In G. R. Hancock & K. M. Samuelsen (Eds.), Advances in latent variable mixture models (pp. 317–341). Greewich: Information Age.
go back to reference Tomarken, A. J., & Keener, A. D. (1998). Frontal brain asymmetry and depression: a self-regulatory perspective. Cognition and Emotion, 12(3), 387–420.CrossRef Tomarken, A. J., & Keener, A. D. (1998). Frontal brain asymmetry and depression: a self-regulatory perspective. Cognition and Emotion, 12(3), 387–420.CrossRef
go back to reference Tucker, D. M., & Williamson, P. A. (1984). Asymmetric neural control systems in human self-regulation. Psychological Review, 91(2), 185–215.PubMedCrossRef Tucker, D. M., & Williamson, P. A. (1984). Asymmetric neural control systems in human self-regulation. Psychological Review, 91(2), 185–215.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Vaughan, L., & Giovanello, K. (2010). Executive function in daily life: age-related influences of executive processes on instrumental activities of daily living. Psychology and Aging, 25(2), 343–355.PubMedCrossRef Vaughan, L., & Giovanello, K. (2010). Executive function in daily life: age-related influences of executive processes on instrumental activities of daily living. Psychology and Aging, 25(2), 343–355.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Vendrell, P., Junque, C., Pujol, J., Jurado, M. A., Molet, J., & Grafman, J. (1995). The role of prefrontal regions in the Stroop task. Neuropsychologia, 33(3), 341–352.PubMedCrossRef Vendrell, P., Junque, C., Pujol, J., Jurado, M. A., Molet, J., & Grafman, J. (1995). The role of prefrontal regions in the Stroop task. Neuropsychologia, 33(3), 341–352.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Vigneau, M., Beaucousin, V., Herve, P. Y., Duffau, H., Crivello, F., Houde, O., … Tzourio-Mazoyer, N. (2006). Meta-analyzing left hemisphere language areas: phonology, semantics, and sentence processing. NeuroImage, 30(4), 1414–1432.PubMedCrossRef Vigneau, M., Beaucousin, V., Herve, P. Y., Duffau, H., Crivello, F., Houde, O., … Tzourio-Mazoyer, N. (2006). Meta-analyzing left hemisphere language areas: phonology, semantics, and sentence processing. NeuroImage, 30(4), 1414–1432.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Vuong, Q. H. (1989). Likelihood ratio tests for model selection and non-tested hypotheses. Econometrics, 57, 307–333.CrossRef Vuong, Q. H. (1989). Likelihood ratio tests for model selection and non-tested hypotheses. Econometrics, 57, 307–333.CrossRef
go back to reference Wallace, G. L., Silvers, J. A., Martin, A., & Kenworthy, L. E. (2009). Brief report: further evidence for inner speech deficits in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 1735–1739.PubMedCrossRef Wallace, G. L., Silvers, J. A., Martin, A., & Kenworthy, L. E. (2009). Brief report: further evidence for inner speech deficits in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 1735–1739.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wechsler, D. (2008). Wechsler adult intelligence scale (4th ed.). San Antonio: Pearson. Wechsler, D. (2008). Wechsler adult intelligence scale (4th ed.). San Antonio: Pearson.
go back to reference Whitmer, A. J., & Banich, M. T. (2007). Inhibition versus switching deficits in different forms of rumination. Psychological Science, 18(6), 546–553.PubMedCrossRef Whitmer, A. J., & Banich, M. T. (2007). Inhibition versus switching deficits in different forms of rumination. Psychological Science, 18(6), 546–553.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wilk, H. A., Ezekiel, F., & Morton, J. B. (2012). Brain regions associated with moment-to-moment adjustments in control and stable task-set maintenance. Neuroimage, 59(2), 1960–1967.PubMedCrossRef Wilk, H. A., Ezekiel, F., & Morton, J. B. (2012). Brain regions associated with moment-to-moment adjustments in control and stable task-set maintenance. Neuroimage, 59(2), 1960–1967.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wilkins, A., & Stewart, A. (1974). The time course of lateral asymmetries in visual perception of letters. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 102(5), 905–908.PubMedCrossRef Wilkins, A., & Stewart, A. (1974). The time course of lateral asymmetries in visual perception of letters. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 102(5), 905–908.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Williams, P. G., Suchy, Y., & Rau, H. K. (2009). Individual differences in executive functioning: implications for stress regulation. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 37(2), 126–140.PubMedCrossRef Williams, P. G., Suchy, Y., & Rau, H. K. (2009). Individual differences in executive functioning: implications for stress regulation. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 37(2), 126–140.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Willis, S. G., Wheatley, G. H., & Mitchell, O. R. (1979). Cerebral processing of spatial and verbal analytic tasks: an EEG study. Neuropsychologia, 17, 473–484.PubMedCrossRef Willis, S. G., Wheatley, G. H., & Mitchell, O. R. (1979). Cerebral processing of spatial and verbal analytic tasks: an EEG study. Neuropsychologia, 17, 473–484.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Willner, P., Bailey, R., Parry, R., & Dymond, S. (2010). Evaluation of executive functioning in people with intellectual disabilities. Journal of Intellectual and Disability Research, 54(4), 366–379.CrossRef Willner, P., Bailey, R., Parry, R., & Dymond, S. (2010). Evaluation of executive functioning in people with intellectual disabilities. Journal of Intellectual and Disability Research, 54(4), 366–379.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Latent profiles of executive functioning in healthy young adults: evidence of individual differences in hemispheric asymmetry
Auteurs
Holly K. Rau
Yana Suchy
Jonathan E. Butner
Paula G. Williams
Publicatiedatum
26-09-2015
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 6/2016
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-015-0706-5

Andere artikelen Uitgave 6/2016

Psychological Research 6/2016 Naar de uitgave