Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 5/2016

11-08-2015 | Original Article

Action induction by visual perception of rotational motion

Auteurs: Claudia Classen, Armin Kibele

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 5/2016

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

A basic process in the planning of everyday actions involves the integration of visually perceived movement characteristics. Such processes of information integration often occur automatically. The aim of the present study was to examine whether the visual perception of spatial characteristics of a rotational motion (rotation direction) can induce a spatially compatible action. Four reaction time experiments were conducted to analyze the effect of perceiving task irrelevant rotational motions of simple geometric figures as well as of gymnasts on a horizontal bar while responding to color changes in these objects. The results show that the participants react faster when the directional information of a rotational motion is compatible with the spatial characteristics of an intended action. The degree of complexity of the perceived event does not play a role in this effect. The spatial features of the used biological motion were salient enough to elicit a motion based Simon effect. However, in the cognitive processing of the visual stimulus, the critical criterion is not the direction of rotation, but rather the relative direction of motion (direction of motion above or below the center of rotation). Nevertheless, this conclusion is tainted with reservations since it is only fully supported by the response behavior of female participants.
Voetnoten
1
Different colors have different effects on human behavior which is an object of interest in color psychology. There exist findings about wavelength effects on simple reaction time (e.g. Weingarten, 1972; Jones & Wilkinson, 1975; Nissen & Pokorny, 1977). In an experiment of our own conducted in our lab, we tested choice reaction times with different colors on different backgrounds and found that there was no difference between the color red (RGB 255, 0, 0) and blue (RGB 51, 56, 255) on a grey background (RGB 128, 128, 128) (Hofmann, 2012).
 
2
In the subsequent experiments of this study (except for Experiment 1b because of the undersized sample) we look for sex differences in the statistical analysis, but will not directly address this issue, because this side effect does not belong to the general purpose of the study.
 
3
Both participants wore contact lenses.
 
4
Participants made on average 1.3 fixations per trial.
 
5
Pillai‘s trace.
 
6
Mean difference corresponds to a viewing angle of 0.09° which is smaller than the eye tracking system’s accuracy. We assume that measurement error to be random, so that the mathematical expectation approximates zero with increasing measures. This must be taken into account for the interpretation of this result.
 
7
We used only two SOA intervals here because we obtained the largest and most consistent dorbSEs with these intervals in the previous experiments (see Table 1).
 
8
A t test showed both dorbSE’s to be significant: lower half, t(9) = 2.64, p = 0.03; upper half, t(9) = 2.28, p = 0.05.
 
9
The SOAs of 200 and 400 ms were adequate intervals to elicit an action selection conflict in the previous experiments. Here we surrogated the 200 ms interval by a 600 ms interval due to the higher complexity of the stimulus in Experiment 2.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Bauer, D. W., & Miller, J. (1982). Stimulus-response compatibility and the motor system. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology, 34(3), 367–380. doi:10.1080/14640748208400849.CrossRef Bauer, D. W., & Miller, J. (1982). Stimulus-response compatibility and the motor system. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology, 34(3), 367–380. doi:10.​1080/​1464074820840084​9.CrossRef
go back to reference Bosbach, S., Prinz, W., & Kerzel, D. (2005). Is direction position? Position- and direction-based correspondence effects in tasks with moving stimuli. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 58(3), 467–506. doi:10.1080/02724980443000016.CrossRef Bosbach, S., Prinz, W., & Kerzel, D. (2005). Is direction position? Position- and direction-based correspondence effects in tasks with moving stimuli. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 58(3), 467–506. doi:10.​1080/​0272498044300001​6.CrossRef
go back to reference Box, G. E. P. (1954). Some theorems on quadratic forms applied in the study of analysis of variance problems, II. Effects of inequality of variance and of correlation between errors in the two-way classification. The Annals of Mathematical Statistics, 25(3), 484–498. doi:10.1214/aoms/1177728717.CrossRef Box, G. E. P. (1954). Some theorems on quadratic forms applied in the study of analysis of variance problems, II. Effects of inequality of variance and of correlation between errors in the two-way classification. The Annals of Mathematical Statistics, 25(3), 484–498. doi:10.​1214/​aoms/​1177728717.CrossRef
go back to reference Cho, Y. S., & Proctor, R. W. (2005). Representing response position relative to display location: influence on orthogonal stimulus–response compatibility. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 58(5), 839–864. doi:10.1080/02724980443000359.CrossRef Cho, Y. S., & Proctor, R. W. (2005). Representing response position relative to display location: influence on orthogonal stimulus–response compatibility. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 58(5), 839–864. doi:10.​1080/​0272498044300035​9.CrossRef
go back to reference Geisser, S., & Greenhouse, S. W. (1958). An extension of Box’s results on the use of the F-distribution in multivariate analysis. The Annals of Mathematical Statistics, 29(3), 885–891. doi:10.1214/aoms/1177706545.CrossRef Geisser, S., & Greenhouse, S. W. (1958). An extension of Box’s results on the use of the F-distribution in multivariate analysis. The Annals of Mathematical Statistics, 29(3), 885–891. doi:10.​1214/​aoms/​1177706545.CrossRef
go back to reference Grossman, E., Donnelly, M., Price, R., Pickens, D., Morgan, V., Neighbor, G., & Blake, R. (2000). Brain Areas Involved in Perception of Biological Motion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(5), 711–720. doi:10.1162/089892900562417.CrossRefPubMed Grossman, E., Donnelly, M., Price, R., Pickens, D., Morgan, V., Neighbor, G., & Blake, R. (2000). Brain Areas Involved in Perception of Biological Motion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(5), 711–720. doi:10.​1162/​089892900562417.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hays, W. L. (1994). Statistics (5th ed.). Fort Worth: Harcourt Brace College Publishers. Hays, W. L. (1994). Statistics (5th ed.). Fort Worth: Harcourt Brace College Publishers.
go back to reference Hofmann, J. (2012). Der Einfluss von Farbe auf die Reaktionszeit im Wettkampfsport: eine empirische Grundlagenuntersuchung zur Frage danach, ob der Vorteil bestimmter Kleiderfarben im Wettkampfsport auf farbabhängige Reaktionszeiten zurückgeführt werden kann [Impact of color on reaction time in competitive sports: A basic examination about the question whether the advantage of special colors of sportswear is due to color dependent reaction times]. (Unpublished master‘s thesis). University of Kassel, Kassel. Hofmann, J. (2012). Der Einfluss von Farbe auf die Reaktionszeit im Wettkampfsport: eine empirische Grundlagenuntersuchung zur Frage danach, ob der Vorteil bestimmter Kleiderfarben im Wettkampfsport auf farbabhängige Reaktionszeiten zurückgeführt werden kann [Impact of color on reaction time in competitive sports: A basic examination about the question whether the advantage of special colors of sportswear is due to color dependent reaction times]. (Unpublished master‘s thesis). University of Kassel, Kassel.
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1993a). The relationship between stimulus processing and response selection in the Simon task: evidence for a temporal overlap. Psychological Research, 55, 280–290. doi:10.1007/BF00419688.CrossRef Hommel, B. (1993a). The relationship between stimulus processing and response selection in the Simon task: evidence for a temporal overlap. Psychological Research, 55, 280–290. doi:10.​1007/​BF00419688.CrossRef
go back to reference Lippa, Y., & Adam, J. (2001). An explanation of orthogonal S-R compatibility effects that vary with hand or response position: the end-state comfort hypothesis. Perception and Psychophysics, 63(1), 156–174. doi:10.3758/BF03200510.CrossRefPubMed Lippa, Y., & Adam, J. (2001). An explanation of orthogonal S-R compatibility effects that vary with hand or response position: the end-state comfort hypothesis. Perception and Psychophysics, 63(1), 156–174. doi:10.​3758/​BF03200510.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Michaels, C. F. (1988). S-R compatibility between response position and destination of apparent motion: evidence of the detection of affordances. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14(2), 231–240. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.14.2.231.PubMed Michaels, C. F. (1988). S-R compatibility between response position and destination of apparent motion: evidence of the detection of affordances. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14(2), 231–240. doi:10.​1037/​0096-1523.​14.​2.​231.PubMed
go back to reference Prinz, W. (1990). A common coding approach to perception and action. In O. Newmann, W. Prinz, & P. Bieri (Eds.), Relationships between perception and action. Current approaches. Berlin, New York: Springer-Verlag. Prinz, W. (1990). A common coding approach to perception and action. In O. Newmann, W. Prinz, & P. Bieri (Eds.), Relationships between perception and action. Current approaches. Berlin, New York: Springer-Verlag.
go back to reference Proctor, R. W., Van Zandt, T., Lu, C.-H., & Weeks, D. J. (1993). Stimulus-response compatibility for moving stimuli: perception of affordances or directional coding? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19(1), 81–91. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.19.1.81.PubMed Proctor, R. W., Van Zandt, T., Lu, C.-H., & Weeks, D. J. (1993). Stimulus-response compatibility for moving stimuli: perception of affordances or directional coding? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19(1), 81–91. doi:10.​1037/​0096-1523.​19.​1.​81.PubMed
go back to reference Proctor, R. W., Wang, D.-Y. D., & Pick, D. F. (2004). Stimulus–response compatibility with wheel-rotation responses: will an incompatible response coding be used when a compatible coding is possible? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11(5), 841–847. doi:10.3758/BF03196710 .CrossRef Proctor, R. W., Wang, D.-Y. D., & Pick, D. F. (2004). Stimulus–response compatibility with wheel-rotation responses: will an incompatible response coding be used when a compatible coding is possible? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11(5), 841–847. doi:10.​3758/​BF03196710 .CrossRef
go back to reference Senior, C., Barnes, J., Giampietroc, V., Simmons, A., Bullmore, E. T., Brammer, M., & David, A. S. (2000). The functional neuroanatomy of implicit-motion perception or ‘representational momentum’. Current Biology, 10(1), 16–22. doi:10.1016/S0960-9822(99)00259-6 .CrossRefPubMed Senior, C., Barnes, J., Giampietroc, V., Simmons, A., Bullmore, E. T., Brammer, M., & David, A. S. (2000). The functional neuroanatomy of implicit-motion perception or ‘representational momentum’. Current Biology, 10(1), 16–22. doi:10.​1016/​S0960-9822(99)00259-6 .CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Simon, J. R., & Craft, J. L. (1970). Effects of altered display-control relationships on information processing from a visual display. Journal of Applied Psychology, 54(3), 253–257. doi:10.1037/h0029083 .CrossRef Simon, J. R., & Craft, J. L. (1970). Effects of altered display-control relationships on information processing from a visual display. Journal of Applied Psychology, 54(3), 253–257. doi:10.​1037/​h0029083 .CrossRef
go back to reference Simon, J. R., & Rudell, A. P. (1967). Auditory S-R compatibility: the effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. Journal of Applied Psychology, 51(3), 300–304. doi:10.1037/h0020586 .CrossRefPubMed Simon, J. R., & Rudell, A. P. (1967). Auditory S-R compatibility: the effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. Journal of Applied Psychology, 51(3), 300–304. doi:10.​1037/​h0020586 .CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Steinman, R. M., Cushman, W. B., & Martins, A. J. (1982). The precision of gaze. A review. Human neurobiology, 1(2), 97–109.PubMed Steinman, R. M., Cushman, W. B., & Martins, A. J. (1982). The precision of gaze. A review. Human neurobiology, 1(2), 97–109.PubMed
go back to reference Stoffer, T. H., & Umiltà, C. (1997). Spatial stimulus coding and the focus of attention in S-R compatibility and the Simon effect. In Bernhard Hommel and Wolfgang Prinz (Ed.), Advances in Psychology : Theoretical Issues in Stimulus-Response Compatibility (pp. 181–208). North-Holland. Stoffer, T. H., & Umiltà, C. (1997). Spatial stimulus coding and the focus of attention in S-R compatibility and the Simon effect. In Bernhard Hommel and Wolfgang Prinz (Ed.), Advances in Psychology : Theoretical Issues in Stimulus-Response Compatibility (pp. 181–208). North-Holland.
go back to reference Weigelt, M., Bosbach, S., Schack, T., & Kunde, W. (2006). Wenn wir anderen Menschen beim Handeln zusehen, interpretieren wir gleichzeitig auch deren Intention [If we watch other’s acting we simultaneously interpret their intention]. In M. Raab, A. Arnold, & K. Gärtner (Eds.), Schriftenreihe Human Performance and Sport (Vol. 2, pp. 49–52)., Zukunft der Sportspiele. fördern, fordern, forschen Flensburg: University Press. Weigelt, M., Bosbach, S., Schack, T., & Kunde, W. (2006). Wenn wir anderen Menschen beim Handeln zusehen, interpretieren wir gleichzeitig auch deren Intention [If we watch other’s acting we simultaneously interpret their intention]. In M. Raab, A. Arnold, & K. Gärtner (Eds.), Schriftenreihe Human Performance and Sport (Vol. 2, pp. 49–52)., Zukunft der Sportspiele. fördern, fordern, forschen Flensburg: University Press.
Metagegevens
Titel
Action induction by visual perception of rotational motion
Auteurs
Claudia Classen
Armin Kibele
Publicatiedatum
11-08-2015
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 5/2016
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-015-0693-6

Andere artikelen Uitgave 5/2016

Psychological Research 5/2016 Naar de uitgave