Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 4/2016

09-05-2015 | Original Article

Does the anticipation of compatible partner reactions facilitate action planning in joint tasks?

Auteur: Romy Müller

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 4/2016

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Observing another human’s actions influences action planning, but what about merely anticipating them? In joint action settings where a partner’s subsequent actions are a consequence of one’s own actions, such contingent partner reactions can be regarded as action effects. Therefore, just like automatic effects they might facilitate those of a person’s actions that overlap with them in relevant features. In Experiments 1 and 2, the spatial compatibility of contingent partner reactions was manipulated and compared with the influence of automatic effects. Experiment 1 used a simplistic scenario in which lateral keypress actions by the subject were responded to by mouse movements of a partner producing spatially compatible or incompatible visual effects. Experiment 2 transferred the paradigm to a more complex task in which subjects manually relocated virtual objects on a multi-touch display, and these or other objects were subsequently manipulated by the partner. In Experiment 1, compatible partner reactions speeded up subjects’ preceding actions, whereas in Experiment 2 the influence was not statistically reliable. To test whether influences of partner reaction compatibility could be found in such naturalistic settings at all, Experiment 3 also used a multi-touch setting but varied temporal instead of spatial compatibility, which has several methodological advantages. This time, a compatibility effect emerged in subjects’ movement initiation times, whereas contrast effects were found for movement durations. These findings indicate that the principles of ideomotor action control can be extended to joint action settings. At the same time, they also emphasize the importance of task features in determining whether our own behaviour is influenced by anticipations of another person’s reactions.
Voetnoten
1
The relative spatial positioning of the participants differed between the experiments (next to each other vs. opposite sides). This resulted from the different affordances posed by the technologies (standard computer monitor vs. multi-touch table) and thus was mainly due to practical considerations. However, note that whereas the position of the participants’ bodies differed, the position and movement direction of their spatial indicator (i.e. mouse cursor in Experiment 1 and index finger in Experiment 2) were identical.
 
2
Note that in the present multi-touch setup, the virtual objects lacked any haptic boundaries and thus did not pose clear restrictions on subjects’ movement amplitudes. In consequence, subjects did not slow down when approaching the end of the object, but extended their swipe gesture way beyond its boundary. Therefore, any speed changes within the measured area were fairly monotonic.
 
3
A previous study examining the temporal compatibility of action effects (Kunde, 2003) analysed the data in a different way, by computing the interaction of action duration and effect duration. This procedure was not adopted in Experiment 3 to make the analysis procedure consistent with that used in Experiments 1 and 2. However, to facilitate the comparison with Kunde’s study, the data were re-analysed accordingly: initiation times from compatible and incompatible blocks were collapsed across the two choice conditions and a repeated measures ANOVA entered with the factors action duration and CPR duration. There was a main effect of action duration, F(1,23) = 163.576, p < 0.001, η p 2  = 0.877, and an interaction with CPR duration, F(1,23) = 6.753, p = 0.016, η p 2  = 0.227. Fast actions were initiated more quickly than slow actions, and compatible mappings (i.e. fast actions being followed by fast CPR and slow movements by slow CPR) led to shorter initiation times than incompatible mappings. However, the main effect of CPR duration was absent, F < 1, indicating that initiation times for actions that triggered fast effects were not generally shorter than those triggering slow effects. The latter result diverges from previous findings (Kunde, 2003).
 
4
Analogous to the initiation times, movement durations were re-analysed with the factors action duration and CPR duration. There were main effects of action duration, F(1,23) = 149.705, p < 0.001, η p 2  = 0.867, CPR duration, F(1,23) = 7.520, p = 0.012, η p 2  = 0.246, and an interaction, F(1,23) = 6.062, p = 0.022, η p 2  = 0.209. Fast movements were performed faster than slow movements, and movements with slow CPR were performed faster than movements with fast CPR. Moreover, with a compatible mapping movements were performed faster than with an incompatible mapping. The presence of an interaction for the movement durations is in contrast with the findings of Kunde (2003).
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Bertenthal, B. I., Longo, M. R., & Kosobud, A. (2006). Imitative response tendencies following observation of intransitive actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32(2), 210–225. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.32.2.210.PubMed Bertenthal, B. I., Longo, M. R., & Kosobud, A. (2006). Imitative response tendencies following observation of intransitive actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32(2), 210–225. doi:10.​1037/​0096-1523.​32.​2.​210.PubMed
go back to reference Blakemore, S. J., Goodbody, S. J., & Wolpert, D. M. (1998). Predicting the consequences of our own actions: the role of sensorimotor context estimation. Journal of Neuroscience, 18(18), 7511–7518.PubMed Blakemore, S. J., Goodbody, S. J., & Wolpert, D. M. (1998). Predicting the consequences of our own actions: the role of sensorimotor context estimation. Journal of Neuroscience, 18(18), 7511–7518.PubMed
go back to reference Catmur, C., & Heyes, C. (2013). Is it what you do, or when you do it? The roles of contingency and similarity in pro-social effects of imitation. Cognitive Science, 37(8), 1541–1552. doi:10.1111/cogs.12071.CrossRefPubMed Catmur, C., & Heyes, C. (2013). Is it what you do, or when you do it? The roles of contingency and similarity in pro-social effects of imitation. Cognitive Science, 37(8), 1541–1552. doi:10.​1111/​cogs.​12071.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Csibra, G. (2007). Action mirroring and action understanding: an alternative account. In P. Haggard, Y. Rosetti, & M. Kawato (Eds.), Attention and performance (Vol. 22, pp. 435–459)., Sensorimotor foundations of higher cognition Oxford: Oxford University Press. Csibra, G. (2007). Action mirroring and action understanding: an alternative account. In P. Haggard, Y. Rosetti, & M. Kawato (Eds.), Attention and performance (Vol. 22, pp. 435–459)., Sensorimotor foundations of higher cognition Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference De Coster, L., Verschuere, B., Goubert, L., Tsakiris, M., & Brass, M. (2013). I suffer more from your pain when you act like me: being imitated enhances affective responses to seeing someone else in pain. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 13(3), 519–532. doi:10.3758/s13415-013-0168-4.CrossRef De Coster, L., Verschuere, B., Goubert, L., Tsakiris, M., & Brass, M. (2013). I suffer more from your pain when you act like me: being imitated enhances affective responses to seeing someone else in pain. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 13(3), 519–532. doi:10.​3758/​s13415-013-0168-4.CrossRef
go back to reference Dolk, T., Hommel, B., Prinz, W., & Liepelt, R. (2013). The (not so) social Simon effect: a referential coding account. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39(5), 1248–1260. doi:10.1037/a0031031.PubMed Dolk, T., Hommel, B., Prinz, W., & Liepelt, R. (2013). The (not so) social Simon effect: a referential coding account. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39(5), 1248–1260. doi:10.​1037/​a0031031.PubMed
go back to reference Herwig, A., Prinz, W., & Waszak, F. (2007). Two modes of sensorimotor integration in intention-based and stimulus-based actions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60(11), 1540–1554. doi:10.1080/17470210601119134.CrossRef Herwig, A., Prinz, W., & Waszak, F. (2007). Two modes of sensorimotor integration in intention-based and stimulus-based actions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60(11), 1540–1554. doi:10.​1080/​1747021060111913​4.CrossRef
go back to reference Heyes, C. (2013). Imitation - associative and context-dependent. In W. Prinz, M. Beisert, & A. Herwig (Eds.), Action science: Foundations of an emerging discipline. Cambridge: MIT Press. Heyes, C. (2013). Imitation - associative and context-dependent. In W. Prinz, M. Beisert, & A. Herwig (Eds.), Action science: Foundations of an emerging discipline. Cambridge: MIT Press.
go back to reference Hoffmann, J., Lenhard, A., Sebald, A., & Pfister, R. (2009). Movements or targets: what makes an action in action–effect learning? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(12), 2433–2449. doi:10.1080/17470210902922079.CrossRef Hoffmann, J., Lenhard, A., Sebald, A., & Pfister, R. (2009). Movements or targets: what makes an action in action–effect learning? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(12), 2433–2449. doi:10.​1080/​1747021090292207​9.CrossRef
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1993). Inverting the Simon effect by intention: determinants of direction and extent of effects of irrelevant spatial information. Psychological Research/Psychologische Forschung, 55(4), 270–279. doi:10.1007/BF00419687.CrossRef Hommel, B. (1993). Inverting the Simon effect by intention: determinants of direction and extent of effects of irrelevant spatial information. Psychological Research/Psychologische Forschung, 55(4), 270–279. doi:10.​1007/​BF00419687.CrossRef
go back to reference Hommel, B. (2013). Ideomotor action control: On the perceptual grounding of voluntary actions and agents. In W. Prinz, M. Beisert, & A. Herwig (Eds.), Action science: Foundations of an emerging discipline (pp. 113–136). Cambridge: MIT Press. Hommel, B. (2013). Ideomotor action control: On the perceptual grounding of voluntary actions and agents. In W. Prinz, M. Beisert, & A. Herwig (Eds.), Action science: Foundations of an emerging discipline (pp. 113–136). Cambridge: MIT Press.
go back to reference Janczyk, M., Pfister, R., Crognale, M. A., & Kunde, W. (2012). Effective rotations: action effects determine the interplay of mental and manual rotations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141(3), 489–501. doi:10.1037/a0026997.CrossRef Janczyk, M., Pfister, R., Crognale, M. A., & Kunde, W. (2012). Effective rotations: action effects determine the interplay of mental and manual rotations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141(3), 489–501. doi:10.​1037/​a0026997.CrossRef
go back to reference Janczyk, M., Yamaguchi, M., Proctor, R. W., & Pfister, R. (2015). Response–effect compatibility with complex actions: the case of wheel rotations. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 77(3), 930–940. doi:10.3758/s13414-014-0828-7 CrossRef Janczyk, M., Yamaguchi, M., Proctor, R. W., & Pfister, R. (2015). Response–effect compatibility with complex actions: the case of wheel rotations. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 77(3), 930–940. doi:10.​3758/​s13414-014-0828-7 CrossRef
go back to reference Klapp, S. T. (1995). Motor response programming during simple choice reaction time: the role of practice. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(5), 1015–1027. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.21.5.1015. Klapp, S. T. (1995). Motor response programming during simple choice reaction time: the role of practice. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(5), 1015–1027. doi:10.​1037/​0096-1523.​21.​5.​1015.
go back to reference Kühn, S., Müller, B. C. N., van Baaren, R. B., Wietzker, A., Dijksterhuis, A., & Brass, M. (2010). Why do I like you when you behave like me? Neural mechanisms mediating positive consequences of observing someone being imitated. Social Neuroscience, 5(4), 384–392. doi:10.1080/17470911003633750.CrossRefPubMed Kühn, S., Müller, B. C. N., van Baaren, R. B., Wietzker, A., Dijksterhuis, A., & Brass, M. (2010). Why do I like you when you behave like me? Neural mechanisms mediating positive consequences of observing someone being imitated. Social Neuroscience, 5(4), 384–392. doi:10.​1080/​1747091100363375​0.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kunde, W., Koch, I., & Hoffmann, J. (2004). Anticipated action effects affect the selection, initiation and execution of actions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57A(1), 87–106. doi:10.1080/02724980343000143.CrossRef Kunde, W., Koch, I., & Hoffmann, J. (2004). Anticipated action effects affect the selection, initiation and execution of actions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57A(1), 87–106. doi:10.​1080/​0272498034300014​3.CrossRef
go back to reference Loehr, J. D., Sebanz, N., & Knoblich, G. (2013). Joint action: From perception-action links to shared representations. In W. Prinz, M. Beisert, & A. Herwig (Eds.), Action science: Foundations of an emerging discipline (pp. 333–356). Cambridge: MIT Press.CrossRef Loehr, J. D., Sebanz, N., & Knoblich, G. (2013). Joint action: From perception-action links to shared representations. In W. Prinz, M. Beisert, & A. Herwig (Eds.), Action science: Foundations of an emerging discipline (pp. 333–356). Cambridge: MIT Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Newman-Norlund, R. D., van Schie, H. T., van Zuijlen, A. M. J., & Bekkering, H. (2007). The mirror neuron system is more active during complementary compared with imitative action. Nature Neuroscience, 10(7), 817–818. doi:10.1038/nn1911.CrossRefPubMed Newman-Norlund, R. D., van Schie, H. T., van Zuijlen, A. M. J., & Bekkering, H. (2007). The mirror neuron system is more active during complementary compared with imitative action. Nature Neuroscience, 10(7), 817–818. doi:10.​1038/​nn1911.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Raftery, A. E. (1995). Bayesian model selection in social research. In P. V. Marsden (Ed.), Sociological methodology (pp. 111–196). Cambridge: Blackwell. Raftery, A. E. (1995). Bayesian model selection in social research. In P. V. Marsden (Ed.), Sociological methodology (pp. 111–196). Cambridge: Blackwell.
go back to reference Stürmer, B., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2000). Correspondence effects with manual gestures and postures: a study of imitation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26(6), 1746–1759. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.26.6.1746.PubMed Stürmer, B., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2000). Correspondence effects with manual gestures and postures: a study of imitation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26(6), 1746–1759. doi:10.​1037/​0096-1523.​26.​6.​1746.PubMed
go back to reference Theeuwes, J., Kramer, A. F., Hahn, S., & Irwin, D. E. (1998). Our eyes do not always go where we want them to go: capture of the eyes by new objects. Psychological Science, 9(5), 379–385. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00071.CrossRef Theeuwes, J., Kramer, A. F., Hahn, S., & Irwin, D. E. (1998). Our eyes do not always go where we want them to go: capture of the eyes by new objects. Psychological Science, 9(5), 379–385. doi:10.​1111/​1467-9280.​00071.CrossRef
go back to reference Wolpert, D. M., Diedrichsen, J., & Flanagan, J. R. (2011). Principles of sensorimotor learning. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 12(12), 739–751. doi:10.1038/nrn3112.PubMed Wolpert, D. M., Diedrichsen, J., & Flanagan, J. R. (2011). Principles of sensorimotor learning. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 12(12), 739–751. doi:10.​1038/​nrn3112.PubMed
go back to reference Wolpert, D. M., Doya, K., & Kawato, M. (2003). A unifying computational framework for motor control and social interaction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, B, 358(1431), 593–602. doi:10.1098/rstb.2002.1238.CrossRef Wolpert, D. M., Doya, K., & Kawato, M. (2003). A unifying computational framework for motor control and social interaction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, B, 358(1431), 593–602. doi:10.​1098/​rstb.​2002.​1238.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Does the anticipation of compatible partner reactions facilitate action planning in joint tasks?
Auteur
Romy Müller
Publicatiedatum
09-05-2015
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 4/2016
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-015-0670-0

Andere artikelen Uitgave 4/2016

Psychological Research 4/2016 Naar de uitgave