Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 2/2015

01-03-2015 | Original Article

Preferred, but not objective temperature predicts working memory depletion

Auteurs: Roberta Sellaro, Bernhard Hommel, Meriem Manaï, Lorenza S. Colzato

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 2/2015

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

The present study investigated the relationship between objective temperature and subjective temperature preferences in predicting performance in simple and complex cognitive tasks. We assessed the impact of room temperature (warm and cold) on the ability to “update” (and monitor) working memory (WM) representations in two groups of participants, who differed in their subjective temperature preferences (warm-preferred vs. cold-preferred). Participants performed an N-back task in which conditions (1-back and 2-back) differ in their WM load and cognitive demands. Results showed that the preferred, but not the objective temperature predicts WM performance in the more resource-demanding (the 2-back) condition. We propose that subjective preferences are more reliable predictors of performance than objective temperature and that performing under the preferred temperature may counteract “ego-depletion” (i.e., reduced self-control after an exhausting cognitive task) when substantial cognitive control is required. Our findings do not only favor a cognitive approach over the environmental/physical approaches dominating the research on cognition–environment interactions, but they also have important, straightforward practical implications for the design of workplaces.
Literatuur
go back to reference Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1252–1265.CrossRefPubMed Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1252–1265.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Cheema, A., & Patrick, V. M. (2012). Influence of warm versus cool temperatures on consumer choice: A resource depletion account. Journal of Marketing Research, 49, 984–995.CrossRef Cheema, A., & Patrick, V. M. (2012). Influence of warm versus cool temperatures on consumer choice: A resource depletion account. Journal of Marketing Research, 49, 984–995.CrossRef
go back to reference Colzato, L. S., van den Wildenberg, W. P. M., Zmigrod, S., & Hommel, B. (2013b). Action video gaming and cognitive control: Playing first person shooter games is associated with improvement in working memory but not action inhibition. Psychological Research, 77, 234–239.CrossRefPubMed Colzato, L. S., van den Wildenberg, W. P. M., Zmigrod, S., & Hommel, B. (2013b). Action video gaming and cognitive control: Playing first person shooter games is associated with improvement in working memory but not action inhibition. Psychological Research, 77, 234–239.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hammel, H. T. (1968). Regulation of internal body temperature. Annual Review of Physiology, 30, 641–710.CrossRefPubMed Hammel, H. T. (1968). Regulation of internal body temperature. Annual Review of Physiology, 30, 641–710.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hancock, P. A. (1986). The effect of skill on performance under an environmental stressor. Aviation, Space and Environmental Medicine, 57, 59–64. Hancock, P. A. (1986). The effect of skill on performance under an environmental stressor. Aviation, Space and Environmental Medicine, 57, 59–64.
go back to reference Hancock, P. A., Ross, J., & Szalma, J. (2007). A meta-analysis of performance response under thermal stressors. Human Factors, 49, 851–877.CrossRefPubMed Hancock, P. A., Ross, J., & Szalma, J. (2007). A meta-analysis of performance response under thermal stressors. Human Factors, 49, 851–877.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hancock, P. A., & Vasmatzidis, I. (2003). Effects of heat stress on cognitive performance: The current state of knowledge. International Journal of Hyperthermia, 19, 355–372.CrossRefPubMed Hancock, P. A., & Vasmatzidis, I. (2003). Effects of heat stress on cognitive performance: The current state of knowledge. International Journal of Hyperthermia, 19, 355–372.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference IJzerman, H., & Semin, G. R. (2009). The thermometer of social relations: Mapping social proximity on temperature. Psychological Science, 20, 1214–1220.CrossRefPubMed IJzerman, H., & Semin, G. R. (2009). The thermometer of social relations: Mapping social proximity on temperature. Psychological Science, 20, 1214–1220.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kane, M. J., Conway, A. R. A., Miura, T. K., & Colflesh, G. J. H. (2007). Working memory, attention control, and the N-back task: A question of construct validity. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 615–622.CrossRefPubMed Kane, M. J., Conway, A. R. A., Miura, T. K., & Colflesh, G. J. H. (2007). Working memory, attention control, and the N-back task: A question of construct validity. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 615–622.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kang, Y., Williams, L. E., Clark, M. S., Gray, J. R., & Bargh, J. A. (2011). Physical temperature effects on trust behavior: The role of insula. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 6, 507–515.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMed Kang, Y., Williams, L. E., Clark, M. S., Gray, J. R., & Bargh, J. A. (2011). Physical temperature effects on trust behavior: The role of insula. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 6, 507–515.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMed
go back to reference Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100.CrossRefPubMed Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Pilcher, J., Nadler, E., & Busch, C. (2002). Effects of hot and cold temperature exposure on performance: A meta-analytic review. Ergonomics, 45, 682–698.CrossRefPubMed Pilcher, J., Nadler, E., & Busch, C. (2002). Effects of hot and cold temperature exposure on performance: A meta-analytic review. Ergonomics, 45, 682–698.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Russell, J. A., Weis, A., & Mendelsohn, G. A. (1989). Affect grid: A single-item scale of pleasure and arousal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 493–502.CrossRef Russell, J. A., Weis, A., & Mendelsohn, G. A. (1989). Affect grid: A single-item scale of pleasure and arousal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 493–502.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Preferred, but not objective temperature predicts working memory depletion
Auteurs
Roberta Sellaro
Bernhard Hommel
Meriem Manaï
Lorenza S. Colzato
Publicatiedatum
01-03-2015
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 2/2015
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-014-0558-4

Andere artikelen Uitgave 2/2015

Psychological Research 2/2015 Naar de uitgave