Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 3/2014

01-05-2014 | Original Article

Analyzing distributional properties of interference effects across modalities: chances and challenges

Auteurs: Kerstin Dittrich, David Kellen, Christoph Stahl

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 3/2014

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

In research investigating Stroop or Simon effects, data are typically analyzed at the level of mean response time (RT), with results showing faster responses for compatible than for incompatible trials. However, this analysis provides only limited information as it glosses over the shape of the RT distributions and how they may differ across tasks and experimental conditions. These limitations have encouraged the analysis of RT distributions using delta plots. In the present review, we aim to bring together research on distributional properties of auditory and visual interference effects. Extending previous reviews on distributional properties of the Simon effect, we additionally review studies reporting distributional analyses of Stroop effects. We show that distributional analyses of sequential effects (i.e., taking into account congruency of the previous trial) capture important similarities and differences of interference effects across tasks (Simon, Stroop) as well as across sensory modalities, despite some challenges associated to this approach.
Voetnoten
1
In the original task, the words “man” and “girl” were used. Here, we used the monosyllabic word “Frau” (woman) instead of “Mädchen” (girl) to maximize comparability with the word “Mann” (man). Participants first practiced the respective task with which they started during a block consisting of 40 trials. Subsequently, participants performed three experimental blocks of this task with 40 trials each. After completion of the first task, participants practiced the second task during one block of 40 trials and then performed three experimental blocks of the second task with 40 trials each. Twenty-four University of Freiburg students (13 women) participated for course credit or as paid volunteers; mean age was 26 years, ranging from 18 to 46 years.
 
2
The RT distribution was divided into nine bins; slopes of the individual delta plots were estimated via ordinary least-squares regression (see also Pratte et al., 2010).
 
3
We thank Mike Pratte for pointing out this interrelation.
 
4
Key mapping was counterbalanced across participants. Participants practiced the task in a block consisting of 24 trials. Subsequently, they performed two experimental blocks of 96 trials each. Twenty-four students from University of Freiburg (17 women) participated for course credit or as paid volunteers; mean age was 22 years, ranging from 19 to 34 years.
 
5
Note that a subtle modification such as using letters (A and B) as target stimuli in the visual Simon task might in fact represent a fundamental change to the nature of the task: One may ask whether the visual Simon task used by Wascher et al. (2001) in fact reflected a spatial Stroop task, because the letters A and B might be associated with left and right responses, respectively.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Ashby, F. G., & Townsend, J. T. (1980). Decomposing the reaction time distribution: pure insertion and selective influence revisited. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 21, 93–123.CrossRef Ashby, F. G., & Townsend, J. T. (1980). Decomposing the reaction time distribution: pure insertion and selective influence revisited. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 21, 93–123.CrossRef
go back to reference Burle, B., Spieser, L., Servant, M., & Hasbroucq, T. (2013). Distributional reaction time properties in the Eriksen task: Marked differences or hidden similarities with the Simon task?. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review. doi:10.3758/s13423-013-0561-6. Burle, B., Spieser, L., Servant, M., & Hasbroucq, T. (2013). Distributional reaction time properties in the Eriksen task: Marked differences or hidden similarities with the Simon task?. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review. doi:10.​3758/​s13423-013-0561-6.
go back to reference Burle, B., van den Wildenberg, W. P. M., & Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2005). Dynamics of facilitation and interference in cue-priming and Simon tasks. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 17, 619–641. doi:10.1080/09541440540000121.CrossRef Burle, B., van den Wildenberg, W. P. M., & Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2005). Dynamics of facilitation and interference in cue-priming and Simon tasks. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 17, 619–641. doi:10.​1080/​0954144054000012​1.CrossRef
go back to reference Davranche, K., Hall, B., & McMorris, T. (2009). Effect of acute exercise on cognitive control required during an Eriksen flanker task. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 31, 628–639.PubMed Davranche, K., Hall, B., & McMorris, T. (2009). Effect of acute exercise on cognitive control required during an Eriksen flanker task. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 31, 628–639.PubMed
go back to reference De Jong, R., Liang, C–. C., & Lauber, E. (1994). Conditional and unconditional automaticity: a dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus–response correspondence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 731–750. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.20.4.731.PubMed De Jong, R., Liang, C–. C., & Lauber, E. (1994). Conditional and unconditional automaticity: a dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus–response correspondence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 731–750. doi:10.​1037/​0096-1523.​20.​4.​731.PubMed
go back to reference Dittrich, K., Rothe, A., & Klauer, K. C. (2012). Increased spatial salience in the social Simon task: a response-coding account of spatial compatibility effects. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 74, 911–929. doi:10.3758/s13414-012-0304-1.CrossRef Dittrich, K., Rothe, A., & Klauer, K. C. (2012). Increased spatial salience in the social Simon task: a response-coding account of spatial compatibility effects. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 74, 911–929. doi:10.​3758/​s13414-012-0304-1.CrossRef
go back to reference Dittrich, K., & Stahl, C. (2012). Selective impairment of auditory selective attention under concurrent cognitive load. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 618–627. doi:10.1037/a0024978.PubMed Dittrich, K., & Stahl, C. (2012). Selective impairment of auditory selective attention under concurrent cognitive load. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 618–627. doi:10.​1037/​a0024978.PubMed
go back to reference Dolk, T., Hommel, B., Prinz, W., & Liepelt, R. (2013). The (not so) social Simon effect: a referential coding account. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39, 1248–1260. doi:10.1037/a0031031.PubMed Dolk, T., Hommel, B., Prinz, W., & Liepelt, R. (2013). The (not so) social Simon effect: a referential coding account. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39, 1248–1260. doi:10.​1037/​a0031031.PubMed
go back to reference Elliott, E. M., Morey, C. C., Morey, R. D., Eaves, S. D., Shelton, J. T., & Lutfi-Proctor, D. A. (2014). The role of modality: auditory and visual distractors in Stroop interference. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 26, 15–26. doi:10.1080/20445911.2013.859133.CrossRef Elliott, E. M., Morey, C. C., Morey, R. D., Eaves, S. D., Shelton, J. T., & Lutfi-Proctor, D. A. (2014). The role of modality: auditory and visual distractors in Stroop interference. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 26, 15–26. doi:10.​1080/​20445911.​2013.​859133.CrossRef
go back to reference Forstmann, B. U., van den Wildenberg, W. P. M., & Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2008). Neural mechanisms, temporal dynamics, and individual differences in interference control. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 1854–1865. doi:10.1162/jocn.2008.20122.PubMedCrossRef Forstmann, B. U., van den Wildenberg, W. P. M., & Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2008). Neural mechanisms, temporal dynamics, and individual differences in interference control. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 1854–1865. doi:10.​1162/​jocn.​2008.​20122.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hazeltine, E., Akçay, C., & Mordkoff, J. T. (2011). Keeping Simon simple: examining the relationship between sequential modulations and feature repetitions with two stimuli, two locations and two responses. Acta Psychologica, 136, 245–252. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.07.011.PubMedCrossRef Hazeltine, E., Akçay, C., & Mordkoff, J. T. (2011). Keeping Simon simple: examining the relationship between sequential modulations and feature repetitions with two stimuli, two locations and two responses. Acta Psychologica, 136, 245–252. doi:10.​1016/​j.​actpsy.​2010.​07.​011.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Jiménez, L., & Méndez, A. (2013). It is not what you expect: dissociating conflict adaptation from expectancies in a Stroop task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39, 271–284. doi:10.1037/a0027734.PubMed Jiménez, L., & Méndez, A. (2013). It is not what you expect: dissociating conflict adaptation from expectancies in a Stroop task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39, 271–284. doi:10.​1037/​a0027734.PubMed
go back to reference Kahneman, D., & Chajczyk, D. (1983). Tests of the automaticity of reading: dilution of Stroop effects by color-irrelevant stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9, 497–509. doi:10.1037//0096-1523.9.4.497.PubMed Kahneman, D., & Chajczyk, D. (1983). Tests of the automaticity of reading: dilution of Stroop effects by color-irrelevant stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9, 497–509. doi:10.​1037/​/​0096-1523.​9.​4.​497.PubMed
go back to reference Lu, C.-H., & Proctor, R. W. (1995). The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: a review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 2, 174–207. doi:10.3758/BF03210959.PubMedCrossRef Lu, C.-H., & Proctor, R. W. (1995). The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: a review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 2, 174–207. doi:10.​3758/​BF03210959.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Matzke, D., Dolan, C. V., Logan, G. D., Brown, S. D., & Wagenmakers, E.-J. (2013). Bayesian parametric estimation of stop-signal reaction time distributions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 1047–1073. doi:10.1037/a0030543.CrossRef Matzke, D., Dolan, C. V., Logan, G. D., Brown, S. D., & Wagenmakers, E.-J. (2013). Bayesian parametric estimation of stop-signal reaction time distributions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 1047–1073. doi:10.​1037/​a0030543.CrossRef
go back to reference Mayr, U., Awh, E., & Laurey, P. (2003). Conflict adaptation effects in the absence of executive control. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 450–452. doi:10.1038/nn1051.PubMed Mayr, U., Awh, E., & Laurey, P. (2003). Conflict adaptation effects in the absence of executive control. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 450–452. doi:10.​1038/​nn1051.PubMed
go back to reference Notebaert, W., Gevers, W., Verbruggen, F., & Liefooghe, B. (2006). Top-down and bottom-up sequential modulations of congruency effects. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 13, 112–117. doi:10.3758/BF03193821.PubMedCrossRef Notebaert, W., Gevers, W., Verbruggen, F., & Liefooghe, B. (2006). Top-down and bottom-up sequential modulations of congruency effects. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 13, 112–117. doi:10.​3758/​BF03193821.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pratte, M. S., Rouder, J. N., Morey, R. D., & Feng, C. (2010). Exploring the differences in distributional properties between Stroop and Simon effects using delta plots. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 72, 2013–2025. doi:10.3758/APP.72.7.2013.CrossRef Pratte, M. S., Rouder, J. N., Morey, R. D., & Feng, C. (2010). Exploring the differences in distributional properties between Stroop and Simon effects using delta plots. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 72, 2013–2025. doi:10.​3758/​APP.​72.​7.​2013.CrossRef
go back to reference Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2002a). Activation and suppression in conflict tasks: Empirical clarification through distributional analyses. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Common mechanisms in perception and action: attention and performance, XIX (pp. 494–519). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2002a). Activation and suppression in conflict tasks: Empirical clarification through distributional analyses. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Common mechanisms in perception and action: attention and performance, XIX (pp. 494–519). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Ridderinkhof, K. R., Scheres, A., Oosterlaan, J., & Sergeant, J. A. (2005). Delta plots in the study of individual differences: new tools reveal response inhibition deficits in AD/HD that are eliminated by methylphenidate treatment. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 197–215. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.114.2.197.PubMedCrossRef Ridderinkhof, K. R., Scheres, A., Oosterlaan, J., & Sergeant, J. A. (2005). Delta plots in the study of individual differences: new tools reveal response inhibition deficits in AD/HD that are eliminated by methylphenidate treatment. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 197–215. doi:10.​1037/​0021-843X.​114.​2.​197.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Roberts, K. L., & Hall, D. A. (2008). Examining a supramodal network for conflict processing: a systematic review and novel functional magnetic resonance imaging data for related visual and auditory Stroop tasks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 1063–1078. doi:10.1162/jocn.2008.20074.PubMedCrossRef Roberts, K. L., & Hall, D. A. (2008). Examining a supramodal network for conflict processing: a systematic review and novel functional magnetic resonance imaging data for related visual and auditory Stroop tasks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 1063–1078. doi:10.​1162/​jocn.​2008.​20074.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sharma, D., Booth, R., Brown, R., & Huguet, P. (2010). Exploring the temporal dynamics of social facilitation in the Stroop task. Psychonomic Bulletin amd Review, 17, 52–58. doi:10.3758/PBR.17.1.52.CrossRef Sharma, D., Booth, R., Brown, R., & Huguet, P. (2010). Exploring the temporal dynamics of social facilitation in the Stroop task. Psychonomic Bulletin amd Review, 17, 52–58. doi:10.​3758/​PBR.​17.​1.​52.CrossRef
go back to reference Shor, R. E. (1975). An auditory analog of Stroop test. Journal of General Psychology, 93, 281–288.PubMed Shor, R. E. (1975). An auditory analog of Stroop test. Journal of General Psychology, 93, 281–288.PubMed
go back to reference Simon, J. R. (1990). The effects of an irrelevant directional cue on human information processing. In R. W. Proctor & T. G. Reeve (Eds.), Stimulus–response compatibility: An integrated perspective (pp. 31–86). Amsterdam: North-Holland. Simon, J. R. (1990). The effects of an irrelevant directional cue on human information processing. In R. W. Proctor & T. G. Reeve (Eds.), Stimulus–response compatibility: An integrated perspective (pp. 31–86). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
go back to reference Soutschek, A., Schwarzkopf, W., Finke, K., Hennig-Fast, K., Müller, H. J., Riedel, M., et al. (2013). Interference control in adult ADHD: no evidence for interference control deficits if response speed is controlled by delta plots. Acta Psychologica, 143, 71–78. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.02.013.PubMedCrossRef Soutschek, A., Schwarzkopf, W., Finke, K., Hennig-Fast, K., Müller, H. J., Riedel, M., et al. (2013). Interference control in adult ADHD: no evidence for interference control deficits if response speed is controlled by delta plots. Acta Psychologica, 143, 71–78. doi:10.​1016/​j.​actpsy.​2013.​02.​013.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Stahl, C., Voss, A., Schmitz, F., Nuszbaum, M., Tüscher, O., Lieb, K., et al. (2013). Behavioral components of impulsivity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. doi:10.1037/a0033981. Stahl, C., Voss, A., Schmitz, F., Nuszbaum, M., Tüscher, O., Lieb, K., et al. (2013). Behavioral components of impulsivity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. doi:10.​1037/​a0033981.
go back to reference Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662.CrossRef Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662.CrossRef
go back to reference Stürmer, B., Leuthold, H., Soetens, E., Schröter, H., & Sommer, W. (2002). Control over location-based response activation in the Simon task: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28, 1345–1363. doi:10.1037//0096-1523.28.6.1345.PubMed Stürmer, B., Leuthold, H., Soetens, E., Schröter, H., & Sommer, W. (2002). Control over location-based response activation in the Simon task: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28, 1345–1363. doi:10.​1037/​/​0096-1523.​28.​6.​1345.PubMed
go back to reference van den Wildenberg, W. P. M., Wylie, S. A., Forstmann, B. U., Burle, B., Hasbroucq, T., & Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2010). To head or to heed? Beyond the surface of selective action inhibition: a review. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4, 1–13. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2010.00222. van den Wildenberg, W. P. M., Wylie, S. A., Forstmann, B. U., Burle, B., Hasbroucq, T., & Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2010). To head or to heed? Beyond the surface of selective action inhibition: a review. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4, 1–13. doi:10.​3389/​fnhum.​2010.​00222.
go back to reference Voss, A., Rothermund, K., Gast, A., & Wentura, D. (2013b). Cognitive processes in associative and categorical priming: a diffusion model analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 536–559. doi:10.1037/a0029459.CrossRef Voss, A., Rothermund, K., Gast, A., & Wentura, D. (2013b). Cognitive processes in associative and categorical priming: a diffusion model analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 536–559. doi:10.​1037/​a0029459.CrossRef
go back to reference Wascher, E., Schatz, U., Kuder, T., & Verleger, R. (2001). Validity and boundary conditions of automatic response activation in the Simon task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 731–751. doi:10.1037//0096-1523.27.3.731.PubMed Wascher, E., Schatz, U., Kuder, T., & Verleger, R. (2001). Validity and boundary conditions of automatic response activation in the Simon task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 731–751. doi:10.​1037/​/​0096-1523.​27.​3.​731.PubMed
go back to reference Wiegand, K., & Wascher, E. (2005). Dynamic aspects of stimulus–response correspondence: evidence for two mechanisms involved in the Simon effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 453–464. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.31.3.453.PubMed Wiegand, K., & Wascher, E. (2005). Dynamic aspects of stimulus–response correspondence: evidence for two mechanisms involved in the Simon effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 453–464. doi:10.​1037/​0096-1523.​31.​3.​453.PubMed
go back to reference Wylie, S. A., Ridderinkhof, K. R., Bashore, T. R., & van den Wildenberg, W. P. M. (2010). The effect of Parkinson’s disease on the dynamics of on-line and proactive cognitive control during action selection. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 2058–2073. doi:10.1162/jocn.2009.21326.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef Wylie, S. A., Ridderinkhof, K. R., Bashore, T. R., & van den Wildenberg, W. P. M. (2010). The effect of Parkinson’s disease on the dynamics of on-line and proactive cognitive control during action selection. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 2058–2073. doi:10.​1162/​jocn.​2009.​21326.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Zhang, J., & Kornblum, S. (1997). Distributional analysis and De Jong, Liang, and Lauber’s (1994) dual-process model of the Simon effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 1543–1551. doi:10.1037//0096-1523.23.5.1543. Zhang, J., & Kornblum, S. (1997). Distributional analysis and De Jong, Liang, and Lauber’s (1994) dual-process model of the Simon effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 1543–1551. doi:10.​1037/​/​0096-1523.​23.​5.​1543.
Metagegevens
Titel
Analyzing distributional properties of interference effects across modalities: chances and challenges
Auteurs
Kerstin Dittrich
David Kellen
Christoph Stahl
Publicatiedatum
01-05-2014
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 3/2014
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-014-0551-y

Andere artikelen Uitgave 3/2014

Psychological Research 3/2014 Naar de uitgave