Skip to main content
Log in

A test of the embodied simulation theory of object perception: potentiation of responses to artifacts and animals

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Theories of embodied object representation predict a tight association between sensorimotor processes and visual processing of manipulable objects. Previous research has shown that object handles can ‘potentiate’ a manual response (i.e., button press) to a congruent location. This potentiation effect is taken as evidence that objects automatically evoke sensorimotor simulations in response to the visual presentation of manipulable objects. In the present series of experiments, we investigated a critical prediction of the theory of embodied object representations that potentiation effects should be observed with manipulable artifacts but not non-manipulable animals. In four experiments we show that (a) potentiation effects are observed with animals and artifacts; (b) potentiation effects depend on the absolute size of the objects and (c) task context influences the presence/absence of potentiation effects. We conclude that potentiation effects do not provide evidence for embodied object representations, but are suggestive of a more general stimulus–response compatibility effect that may depend on the distribution of attention to different object features.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Currently, there is no unified theory of embodied cognition, and many accounts have been provided. For instance, see Wilson (2002) for a general overview of the issues associated with embodied cognition; see the perceptual symbols theory of Barsalou (2003); and see Lakoff and Johnson (1999) for a general theoretical and philosophical framework. A similar approach has been described in the literature on common-coding (e.g., Hommel, Müsseler, Aschersleben, & Prinz, 2001) and regarding semantic representation (Allport, 1985). Additionally, the idea that artifacts ‘afford’ actions was first developed by Gibson (1979). For our purposes, Shapiro (2011) has identified at least three distinct aspects of the embodied cognition hypothesis; in the present experiment, we are specifically referring to the ‘conceptualization’ hypothesis. This hypothesis suggests that simulations and reactivations in modality-specific systems form the basis of object identification. According to the conceptualization hypothesis, we must simulate (or reactivate) our visual, auditory and motor experiences with a hammer to know an object is hammer.

  2. This effect has also been referred to as the orientation effect (see Vainio, Ellis, & Tucker, 2007), the visuo-motor priming effect (Craighero, Fadiga, Umiltà & Rizzolatti, 1996) or simply the correspondence or compatibility effect (see Phillips & Ward, 2002; Cho & Proctor, 2010). However, we feel that ‘potentiation’ is a more descriptive and less ambiguous term.

  3. Consistent patterns of results have been reported using similar paradigms. For instance, Tucker and Ellis (1998, 2004) have shown that participants are faster at making precision grasps (e.g., pinching with the index finger and thumb) when shown an object that affords a precisions grasp (e.g., a clothes peg) than when shown an object that does not (e.g., a wine bottle); conversely, power grasps (e.g., grasping with the whole hand) were primed by objects that afford power grasps (e.g., a wine bottle) but not other objects (e.g., a clothes peg). While these results are consistent with theories of embodied cognition, it could be argued that they arise because of a general ‘dimensional overlap’ between stimuli and responses (Kornblum et al., 1990).

  4. Note, the effect size we report is the generalized eta squared. The generalized eta is designed to reduce the influence of the number of factors in an experiment. However, the standard interpretations of the small, medium, and large effect sizes still apply (see Olejnik & Algina, 2003).

  5. This conclusion is based on the interpretation of the pattern of data across Experiment 1 and 2. To ensure the reliability of this interpretation, we combined data from the two experiments and included Experiment as a between subject factor. The three-way interaction between experiment, compatibility, and category was marginally significant in the accuracy data, F (1, 48) = 2.9, p < 0.09, η 2G  = 0.009, and significant in the logRT data, F (1, 48) = 6.47, p = 0.001, η 2G  = 0.002. These interactions support our conclusions.

  6. We recognize that the direction of the potentiation effect depends on our definition of ‘compatibility’; therefore, in this way the effect’s direction is arbitrary. For simplicity, we retain our definition throughout this report and discuss the consequences in the “General Discussion”. Importantly, whether we consider the potentiation effect as ‘reversed’ or not has no consequences for the main conclusion we draw here.

  7. This argument is based on findings that a participant’s own hand will bias attention in the visual field. We thank one reviewer for suggesting that it is unknown whether a hand image (that is not the participant’s own hand) will affect attention similarly. Future research should explore whether own vs. other hand images influence attention in similar ways.

References

  • Allport, D. A. (1985). Distributed memory, modular subsystems and dysphasia. In S. D. Newman & R. Epstein (Eds.), Current perspectives in dysphasia (pp. 207–244). New York: Churchill Livingstone.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, S. J., Yamagishi, N., & Karavia, V. (2002). Attentional processes link perception and action. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, 269(1497), 1225–1232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baayen, R. H. (2008). Analyzing linguistic data: a practical introduction to statistics using R. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, L. (2003). Abstraction in perceptual symbol systems. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 358, 1177–1187.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, L. (2008). Grounded cognition. The Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617–645.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bates, D., Maechler, M., & Bolker, B. (2012). lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes. R package version 0.999999-0. http://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/lme4/index.html. Accessed 1 Aug 2012.

  • Bub, D. N., & Masson, M. E. J. (2010). Grasping beer mugs: on the dynamics of alignment effects induced by handled objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36(2), 341–358.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bub, D. N., Masson, M. E. J., & Cree, G. S. (2008). Evocation of functional and volumetric gestural knowledge by objects and words. Cognition, 106, 27–58.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buccino, G., Sato, M., Cattaneo, L., Rodà, F., & Riggio, L. (2009). Broken affordances, broken objects: a TMS study. Neuropsychologia, 47, 3074–3078.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cate, A., Goodale, M., & Köhler, S. (2011). The role of apparent size in building- and object-specific regions of ventral visual cortex. Brain Research, 4, 09–122.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chao, L., & Martin, A. (2000). Representation of manipulable man-made objects in the dorsal stream. NeuroImage, 12, 478–484.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cho, D., & Proctor, R. W. (2010). The object-based Simon-effect: grasping affordance or relative location of the graspable part? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36(4), 853–861.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cho, D., & Proctor, R. W. (2011). Correspondence effects for objects with opposing left and right protrusions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37(3), 737–749.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cho, D., & Proctor, R. W. (2012). Object-based correspondence effects for action-relevant and surface-property judgments with keypress responses: evidence for a basis in spatial coding. Psychological Research,. doi:10.1007/s00426-012-0458-4.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Craighero, L., Fadiga, L., Umiltà, C., & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Evidence for visuomotor priming effect. NeuroReport, 8, 347–349.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V., & Sinigaglia, C. (2011). What is so special about embodied simulation? Trends in Cognitive Science, 15(11), 512–519.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gerlach, C., Law, I., & Paulson, O. (2002). When action turns into words: activation of motor-based knowledge during categorization of manipulable objects. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(8), 1230–1239.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Helbig, H., Graf, M., & Keifer, M. (2006). The role of action representations in visual object recognition. Experimental Brain Research, 174, 221–228.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Helbig, H., Steinwender, J., Graf, M., & Kiefer, M. (2010). Action observation can prime visual object recognition. Experimental Brain Research, 200, 251–258.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hillyard, S. A., Vogel, E. K., & Luck, S. J. (1998). Sensory gain control (amplification) as a mechanism of selective attention: electrophysiological and neuroimaging evidence. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 353(1373), 1257–1270.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B. (1993). Inverting the Simon effect by intention. Psychological Research, 55, 270–279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–937.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kirchner, H., & Thorpe, S. J. (2006). Ultra-rapid object detection with saccadic eye movements: visual processing speed revisited. Vision Research, 46(11), 1762–1776.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility--a model and taxonomy. Psychological review, 97(2), 253–270.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kovic, V., Plunkett, K., & Westermann, G. (2009a). Eye-tracking study of animate objects. Psihologija, 42(3), 307–327.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kovic, V., Plunkett, K., & Westermann, G. (2009b). Eye-tracking study of inanimate objects. Psihologija, 42(4), 417–436.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the flesh: the embodied mind and its challenge to western thought. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lawrence, MA. (2012). ez: Easy analysis and visualization of factorial experiments. R package version 4.1-1. http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=ez. Accessed 1 Aug 2012.

  • Masson, M. E. J., Bub, D. N., & Breuer, A. T. (2011). Priming of reach and grasp actions by handled objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37(5), 1470–1484.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McMullen, P., & Jolicoeur, P. (1990). The spatial frame of reference in object naming and discrimination of left-right reflections. Memory and Cognition, 18(1), 99–115.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mounoud, P., Dushcherer, K., Moy, G., & Perraudin, S. (2007). The influence of action perception on object recognition: a developmental study. Developmental Science, 10(6), 836–852.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Murata, A., Gallese, V., Luppino, G., Kaseda, M., & Sakata, H. (2000). Selectivity for the shape, size, and orientation of objects for grasping in neurons of the monkey parietal area AIP. Journal of Neurophysiology, 83, 2580–2601.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Newman, A. J., Tremblay, A., Nichols, E. S., Neville, H. J., & Ullman, M. T. (2012). The influence of language proficiency on brain activation in native and late learners of English: an ERP study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24(5), 1205–1223.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Olejnik, S., & Algina, J. (2003). Generalized Eta and omega squared statistics: measures of effect size for some common research designs. Psychological Methods, 8(4), 434–447.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pellicano, A., Iani, C., Borghi, A. M., Rubichi, S., & Nicoletti, R. (2010). Simon-like and functional affordance effects with tools: the effects of object perceptual discrimination and object action state. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63(11), 2190–2201.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Phillips, J. C., & Ward, R. (2002). S-R correspondence effects of irrelevant visual affordance: time course and specificity of response activation. Visual Cognition, 9, 540–558.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pulvermüller, F., Hauk, O., Nikulin, V. V., & Ilmoniemi, R. J. (2005). Functional links between motor and language systems. European Journal of Neuroscience, 21(3), 793–797.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ratcliff, R. (1993). Methods for dealing with reaction time outliers. Psychological Bulletin, 114(3), 510–532.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reed, C. L., Grubb, J. D., & Steele, C. (2006). Hands up: attentional prioritization of space near the hand. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32(1), 166–177.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D. L., & Buckner, R. L. (1998). Priming and the brain. Neuron, 20, 185–195.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shapiro, L. (2011). Embodied Cognition. New York, NY: Taylor & Francis.

  • Simon, J. R. (1969). Reactions towards the source of stimulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81, 174–176.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Symes, E., Ellis, R., & Tucker, M. (2007). Visual object affordances: object orientation. Acta Psychologia, 124, 238–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • R Core Team (2012). R: a language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. ISBN 3-900051-07-0, URL http://www.R-project.org/. Accessed 1 Aug 2012.

  • Tipper, S. P., Paul, M. A., & Hayes, A. E. (2006). Vision-for-action: the effects of object property discrimination and action state on affordance compatibility effects. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 3(3), 493–498.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tremblay, R., & Ransijn, J. (2011). LMERConvenienceFunctions: a suite of functions to back-fit fixed effects and forward-fit random effects, as well as other miscellaneous functions. R Package Version, 1(6), 7.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tucker, M., & Ellis, R. (1998). On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 830–846.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tucker, M., & Ellis, R. (2004). Action priming by briefly presented objects. Acta Psychologica, 116, 185–203.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vainio, L., Ellis, R., & Tucker, M. (2007). The role of visual attention in action priming. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60(2), 241–261.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vu, K. P. (2007). Influences on the Simon effect of prior practice with spatially incompatible mappings: transfer within and between horizontal and vertical dimensions. Memory and Cognition, 35(6), 1463–1471.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Whelan, R. (2008). Effective analysis of reaction time data. The Psychological Record, 58, 475–482.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, M. (2002). Six views of embodied cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9(4), 625–636.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Witt, J., Kemmerer, D., Linkenauger, S., & Culham, J. (2010). A functional role for motor simulation in identifying tools. Psychological Science, 21(9), 1215–1219.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yang, S.-J., & Beilock, S. L. (2011). Seeing and doing: ability to act moderates orientation effects in object perception. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(4), 639–648.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Conflict of interest

The authors report no conflicts of interest in the preparation of the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Heath E. Matheson.

Appendices

Appendix 1

In addition to the main analysis of variance (ANOVA), logRT measures were analyzed using linear mixed-effects (LME) modeling as it is implemented by the lmer() function of the lme4 library in R (Bates, Maechler, & Bolker, 2012). Although linear mixed-effects modeling is a relatively new advancement in statistical modeling, this approach has been used by a number of authors in the analysis of continuous data (e.g., see Baayen, 2008; see Newman, Tremblay, Nichols, Neville, & Ullman, 2012, and Lawrence & Klein, in press, for similar implementations as the one used here), and offers a number of advantages over the more traditional repeated-measures ANOVA. First, LME models permit both fixed-effects parameters (those corresponding to the controlled manipulated variables in the experiment, including the category of object and its orientation—compatible vs. incompatible) and random effects (the effects of a variable that are randomly chosen from a larger population such as the experimental stimuli used or the participants sampled). Modeling random effects allows the analysis to better meet the assumption of uncorrelated randomly distributed residuals, an assumption that is typically violated in analyses of repeated-measures RT data. Further, modeling random effects allows for greater generalization of the results beyond the participants and items used in the experiment (Baayen, 2008). This is an important advantage of the current study over previous research that has used single stimuli (e.g., Anderson, Yamagishi, & Karavia, 2002); in these cases, there is no confidence that the effects will extend to other stimulus sets. By accounting for variability due to items we are able to derive a more precise estimate of the effects within and between our two object categories (see Baayen, 2008 discussion comparing LME with quasi-F comparisons in item and subject analyses). Second, linear mixed-effects models account for non-sphericity and missing data points, which is often an issue when analyzing only a subset of RTs (i.e., from correct trials only), without the use of common corrections (e.g., using the Greenhouse-Geisser or Huynh–Feldt corrections). Third, mixed models do not aggregate the data, meaning that each data point is used in building the statistical model, increasing its power (see Baayen, 2008 for a thorough discussion of these advantages and more).

The statistical models were computed using the lme() and LMERConvenienceFunctions() packages (Tremblay & Ransijn, 2011). Models are fitted with the following method:

  1. 1.

    An initial model was computed containing all the theoretically relevant fixed effects (in this case object category and compatibility), but it only included a single random effect—one for subjects. Conceptually, modeling subjects as random is similar to performing a repeated-measures ANOVA, allowing for by-subject adjustments to the reaction time data, accounting for the fact that some subjects are faster than others overall.

  2. 2.

    The residuals of the initial model were inspected for outliers and any violations of normality. Though we analyzed logRTs, outliers more than 2.5 standard deviations were removed from the data. This is recommended prior to fitting optimal models (see Baayen, 2008), and facilitates comparing results between the LME reported here and the analysis in the main text. After trimming, the initial model was refit.

  3. 3.

    Next, random effects were ‘forward-fitted’. With this process, different random effects structures were added and compared to the initial model to investigate whether they were warranted for inclusion. In all our analyses we looked for the same collection of random effects structures, including a by-item intercept, and by-subject intercepts and slopes for both compatibility and category. By-item intercepts allow the model to account for the fact that some items are responded to more quickly than others. Thus, this random effect, if included, accounts for the fact that stimuli used in this experiment are only a random subset of all possible stimuli, and therefore, make the optimal model more generalizable. At this stage, only random effects that provide sufficient explanatory power over and above the by-subject random effect adjustments were retained.

  4. 4.

    Next, the fixed effects were ‘back-fitted’. With the random effects structure in place, the importance of each fixed effect was re-evaluated (this is done to determine whether the inclusion of any random effect structures altered the predictive power of the fixed effects). To do so, a full model, which includes each fixed effect and their interactions (and the random effect structure), was fitted. Any non-significant terms were removed with each interaction until all of the highest-order interactions were significant. In this way, only the fixed effects that provide explanatory power are included. This allows us to consider the final model ‘optimal’.

  5. 5.

    Finally, though there is no agreed upon way of determining statistical significance of each component of the model individually (because there is no consensus on which degrees of freedom to use), we provide an ANOVA table using the upper bound and lower bound degrees of freedom. This allows us to readily compare the results to the more restricted ANOVA.

Results

Experiment 1

Our initial model included category and compatibility as fixed-effects factors and by-subjects random intercepts. The optimal model included only an effect of object category and no interaction terms, as well as by-subject random intercepts (i.e., a by-subject adjustment to the mean), and by-item random intercepts (i.e., a by-item adjustment to the mean). The results of the model are shown in Table 3. Overall, the mean RTs in response to animals were faster than to artifacts; difference score = 0.102 in natural log units).

Table 3 ANOVA table of significant factors in the LME model of RT data

Experiment 2

We performed LME modeling on the natural logarithm of reaction times. Our initial model included category and compatibility as fixed-effects factors, as well as by-subjects random intercepts. The optimal model included the category and compatibility factors, and the interaction between category and compatibility. The model also included by-subject random intercepts (i.e., a by-subject adjustment to the mean) and by-item random intercepts. Additionally, there was a by-subject adjustment to the slope for the category effect, allowing us to model variance in the size of the category effect for each subject. The results of the model are shown in Table 4. Overall, the mean RTs in response to animals was faster than the response to artifacts (difference score = 0.084 in natural log units). The Category × Compatibility interaction was due to faster responses on compatible trials relative to incompatible trials for artifacts (difference score = 0.019), while the opposite pattern was observed for animals (difference = −0.037).

Table 4 ANOVA table on significant factors in LME model of RT data

Experiment 3

We performed LME modeling on the natural logarithm of reaction times. Our initial model included category, compatibility, and hand prime as fixed effects, as well as by-subject random intercepts. The optimal model included the category, compatibility and hand prime factors, as well as a two-way interaction between category and compatibility. The model also included by-subject random intercepts (i.e., a by-subject adjustment to the mean), and a by-item random intercepts, in addition to a by-subject random slope for the compatibility factor. Consistent with the results of the ANOVA, the interaction was driven by different compatibility effects for the animals (different score = −0.018) and the artifacts (difference score = 0.007). Importantly, this pattern replicates the pattern of Experiment 2. The results of the model are shown in Table 5.

Table 5 ANOVA table on significant factors in LME model of RT data

Experiment 4

We performed LME modeling on the natural logarithm of reaction times. Our initial model included category and compatibility as fixed-effects factors, as well as by-subjects random intercepts. The optimal model included the category and compatibility factors with no interaction term. This model also included by-subject random intercepts and a by-subject slopes and intercepts for category. The results of the model are shown in Table 6. The effect of compatibility was due to faster responding on incompatible than compatible trials (difference score = 0.016 in natural log units).

Table 6 ANOVA table on significant factors in LME model of RT data

Discussion

The results of the optimal linear mixed-effects models are consistent with the analysis of variance (ANOVA) in each experiment reported in the main text. This provides confidence in the general patterns of data reported here and the interpretations of them. Providing converging statistical analyses in this way has a number of advantages in the present study (see Baayen, 2008). First, by using linear mixed-effects models we were able to account for variability due to the specific stimuli in our experiment and have shown effects of object category over and above the effects of stimulus. Specifically, of particular importance is the fact that the linear mixed models predicted much smaller category effects after accounting for stimulus differences. Thus, using this approach, we were able to provide a much more precise prediction of differences due to object category (the size of the main effects). Further, unlike the ANOVA, the linear mixed model allows us to be confident in our generalization of our effects to stimuli beyond those used in our experiments. Finally, using linear mixed models have allowed us to show interactions between category and compatibility even after accounting for by-subject differences in the effects of these variables (in the cases where there was large variance in by-subject effects). Thus, this powerful technique allows us to make more precise predictions of the effects of our factors and their generalization beyond our study.

Appendix 2

Because our use of logRT in the main analysis may make it difficult to compare our results with previous studies that analyzed raw reaction times (RTs), we provide a complete analysis with raw RTs here. Note, the pattern of results both within and between experiments does not change, and the final values for each effect are comparable to those reported in the main text.

Experiment 1: raw RT

In line with the accuracy findings, the ANOVA revealed a significant effect of object category, F (1, 25) = 148.81, p < 0.001, η G = 0.11, due to faster responses to the animals (raw RT = 558.34, SD = 78.44) than the artifacts (raw RT = 617.05, SD = 90.99). No other effects were significant, ps > 0.05.

Experiment 2: raw RT

Consistent with the accuracy results, ANOVA on the raw RTs revealed a significant effect of category, F (1, 23) = 60.24, p < 0.001, η 2G  = 0.08, due to faster responses to animals (raw RT = 534.53, SD = 84.67) than to artifacts (raw RT = 590.64, SD = 100.81). Additionally, there was a significant Category × Compatibility interaction, F (1, 23) = 17.68, p < 0.001, η 2G  = 0.009, indicating that the compatibility effect was different for the animals and the artifacts. For animals, responses were faster on incompatible trials (raw RT = 522.79, SD = 78.79) than compatible trials (raw RT = 546.73, SD = 94.05). For artifacts, the opposite pattern was shown, with faster responses on compatible trials (raw RT = 585.49, SD = 101.34) than incompatible trials (raw RT = 596.14, SD = 102.55). (Fisher’s least significant difference showed that the compatibility effect for the animals was significant though it was not in the artifacts. However, an a priori directional paired samples t test in artifacts was significant, p = 0.04).

Experiment 3: raw RT

ANOVA revealed a significant effect of category, F (1, 32) = 234.76, p < 0.001, η 2G  = 0.08. Participants responded faster to animals (raw RT = 517.92, SD = 75.37) than to artifacts (raw RT = 563.50, SD = 80.49). Additionally, there was a significant effect of hand prime size, F (1, 32) = 14.61, p < 0.001, η 2G  = 0.004; responses were faster to images presented after the large hand prime (raw RT = 535.87, SD = 77.06) than the small hand prime (raw RT = 545.55, SD = 78.61). Again, there was a significant Category × Compatibility interaction, F (1, 32) = 11.79, p = 0.002, η 2G  = 0.002, indicating that the compatibility effect was different for animals and artifacts, replicating the pattern of Experiment 2. For animals, responses were faster on incompatible trials (raw RT = 513.39, SD = 78.23) than compatible trials (raw RT = 522.44, SD = 73.62). In the artifacts, the opposite pattern was shown, with fastest responding on the compatible trials (raw RT = 561.11, SD = 78.48) than the incompatible trials (raw RT = 565.89, SD = 83.86). (Fisher’s least significant difference tests showed that, while the compatibility effect for the animals was significant, it did not quite reach significance in the artifacts despite the hypothesized direction). Importantly, there were no interactions with hand prime, ps > 0.05.

Experiment 4: raw RT

The ANOVA revealed a significant effect of category, F (1, 29) = 15.49, p < 0.001, η 2G  = 0.022. As in the previous experiments, participants responded faster to animals (raw RT = 444.77, SD = 46.86) than to artifacts (raw RT = 458.77, SD = 47.94). There was a significant main effect of compatibility, F (1, 29) = 9.49, p = 0.004, η 2G  = 0.004, due to faster responding on incompatible trials (raw RT = 448.91, SD = 47.91) than on compatible trials (raw RT = 454.63, SD = 45.39). The Compatibility × Category interaction was not significant, p > 0.05.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Matheson, H.E., White, N.C. & McMullen, P.A. A test of the embodied simulation theory of object perception: potentiation of responses to artifacts and animals. Psychological Research 78, 465–482 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-013-0502-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-013-0502-z

Keywords

Navigation