Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 2/2014

01-03-2014 | Original Article

Contingent capture in cueing: the role of color search templates and cue-target color relations

Auteurs: Ulrich Ansorge, Stefanie I. Becker

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 2/2014

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Visual search studies have shown that attention can be top-down biased to a specific target color, so that only items with this color or a similar color can capture attention. According to some theories of attention, colors from different categories (i.e., red, green, blue, yellow) are represented independently. However, other accounts have proposed that these are related—either because color is filtered through broad overlapping channels (4-channel view), or because colors are represented in one continuous feature space (e.g., CIE space) and search is governed by specific principles (e.g., linear separability between colors, or top-down tuning to relative colors). The present study tested these different views using a cueing experiment in which observers had to select one target color (e.g., red) and ignore two or four differently colored distractors that were presented prior to the target (cues). The results showed clear evidence for top-down contingent capture by colors, as a target-colored cue captured attention more strongly than differently colored cues. However, the results failed to support any of the proposed views that different color categories are related to one another by overlapping channels, linear separability, or relational guidance (N = 96).
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Literatuur
go back to reference Ansorge, U., & Becker, S. I. (2012). Automatic priming of attentional control by relevant colors. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74, 83–104. Ansorge, U., & Becker, S. I. (2012). Automatic priming of attentional control by relevant colors. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74, 83–104.
go back to reference Anderson, B. A., & Folk, C. L. (2010). Variations in the magnitude of attentional capture: Testing a two-process model. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72, 342–352.CrossRef Anderson, B. A., & Folk, C. L. (2010). Variations in the magnitude of attentional capture: Testing a two-process model. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72, 342–352.CrossRef
go back to reference Ansorge, U., & Heumann, M. (2003). Top-down contingencies in peripheral cuing: The roles of color and location. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 937–948.PubMed Ansorge, U., & Heumann, M. (2003). Top-down contingencies in peripheral cuing: The roles of color and location. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 937–948.PubMed
go back to reference Ansorge, U., & Heumann, M. (2004). Peripheral cuing by abrupt-onset cues: the influence of color in S-R corresponding conditions. Acta Psychologica, 116(2), 115–143.PubMedCrossRef Ansorge, U., & Heumann, M. (2004). Peripheral cuing by abrupt-onset cues: the influence of color in S-R corresponding conditions. Acta Psychologica, 116(2), 115–143.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ansorge, U., & Horstmann, G. (2007). Preemptive control of attentional capture by color: Evidence from trial-by-trial analysis and ordering of onsets of capture effects in RT distributions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 952–975.CrossRef Ansorge, U., & Horstmann, G. (2007). Preemptive control of attentional capture by color: Evidence from trial-by-trial analysis and ordering of onsets of capture effects in RT distributions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 952–975.CrossRef
go back to reference Ansorge, U., Kiss, M., & Eimer, M. (2009). Goal-driven attentional capture by invisible colors: Evidence from event-related potentials. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 648–653.CrossRef Ansorge, U., Kiss, M., & Eimer, M. (2009). Goal-driven attentional capture by invisible colors: Evidence from event-related potentials. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 648–653.CrossRef
go back to reference Awh, E., Belopolsky, A. V., & Theeuwes, J. (2012). Top-down versus bottom-up attentional control: A failed dichotomy. Trends in Cognitive Science, 16, 437–443.CrossRef Awh, E., Belopolsky, A. V., & Theeuwes, J. (2012). Top-down versus bottom-up attentional control: A failed dichotomy. Trends in Cognitive Science, 16, 437–443.CrossRef
go back to reference Bauer, B. G., Jolicoeur, P., & Cowan, W. (1998). The linear separability effect in color visual search: Ruling out the additive color hypothesis. Perception & Psychophysics, 60, 1083–1093.CrossRef Bauer, B. G., Jolicoeur, P., & Cowan, W. (1998). The linear separability effect in color visual search: Ruling out the additive color hypothesis. Perception & Psychophysics, 60, 1083–1093.CrossRef
go back to reference Becker, S. I. (2007). Irrelevant singletons in pop-out search: Attentional capture or filtering costs? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33, 764–787.PubMed Becker, S. I. (2007). Irrelevant singletons in pop-out search: Attentional capture or filtering costs? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33, 764–787.PubMed
go back to reference Becker, S. I. (2010a). The role of target–distractor relationships in guiding attention and the eyes in visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139, 247–265.CrossRef Becker, S. I. (2010a). The role of target–distractor relationships in guiding attention and the eyes in visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139, 247–265.CrossRef
go back to reference Becker, S. I. (2010b). Oculomotor capture by irrelevant colour singletons depends on intertrial priming. Vision Research, 50, 2116–2126.PubMedCrossRef Becker, S. I. (2010b). Oculomotor capture by irrelevant colour singletons depends on intertrial priming. Vision Research, 50, 2116–2126.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Becker, S. I., Ansorge, U., & Horstmann, G. (2009). Can intertrial priming account for the similarity effect in visual search? Vision Research, 49, 1738–1756.PubMedCrossRef Becker, S. I., Ansorge, U., & Horstmann, G. (2009). Can intertrial priming account for the similarity effect in visual search? Vision Research, 49, 1738–1756.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Becker, S. I., Folk, C. L., & Remington, R. W. (2010). The role of relational information in contingent capture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 1460–1476.PubMed Becker, S. I., Folk, C. L., & Remington, R. W. (2010). The role of relational information in contingent capture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 1460–1476.PubMed
go back to reference Eckstein, M., Drescher, B., & Shimozaki, S. S. (2006). Attentional cues in real scenes, saccadic targeting, and Bayesian priors. Psychological Science, 17, 973–980.PubMedCrossRef Eckstein, M., Drescher, B., & Shimozaki, S. S. (2006). Attentional cues in real scenes, saccadic targeting, and Bayesian priors. Psychological Science, 17, 973–980.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Eimer, M., & Kiss, M. (2008). Involuntary attentional capture is determined by task set: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 1423–1433.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef Eimer, M., & Kiss, M. (2008). Involuntary attentional capture is determined by task set: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 1423–1433.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Einhäuser, W., Spain, M., & Perona, P. (2008). Objects predict fixations better than early salience. Journal of Vision, 8, 1–26. Einhäuser, W., Spain, M., & Perona, P. (2008). Objects predict fixations better than early salience. Journal of Vision, 8, 1–26.
go back to reference Fecteau, J. (2007). Priming of pop-out depends on the current goals of the observers. Journal of Vision, 7, 1–11.PubMedCrossRef Fecteau, J. (2007). Priming of pop-out depends on the current goals of the observers. Journal of Vision, 7, 1–11.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Folk, C. L., & Remington, R. W. (1998). Selectivity in distraction by irrelevant featural singletons: Evidence for two forms of attentional capture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 847–858.PubMed Folk, C. L., & Remington, R. W. (1998). Selectivity in distraction by irrelevant featural singletons: Evidence for two forms of attentional capture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 847–858.PubMed
go back to reference Folk, C. L., & Remington, R. W. (2008). Bottom-up priming of top-down attentional control settings. Visual Cognition, 16, 215–231.CrossRef Folk, C. L., & Remington, R. W. (2008). Bottom-up priming of top-down attentional control settings. Visual Cognition, 16, 215–231.CrossRef
go back to reference Folk, C. L., Remington, R. W., & Johnston, J. C. (1992). Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 1030–1044.PubMed Folk, C. L., Remington, R. W., & Johnston, J. C. (1992). Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 1030–1044.PubMed
go back to reference Fortier-Gauthier, U., Dell’Acqua, R., & Jolicœur, P. (2013). The “red-alert” effect in visual search: Evidence from human electrophysiology. Psychophysiology (in press). Fortier-Gauthier, U., Dell’Acqua, R., & Jolicœur, P. (2013). The “red-alert” effect in visual search: Evidence from human electrophysiology. Psychophysiology (in press).
go back to reference Geyer, T., Krummenacher, J., & Müller, H. J. (2008). Expectancies modulate attentional capture by salient color singletons. Vision Research, 48, 1315–1326.PubMedCrossRef Geyer, T., Krummenacher, J., & Müller, H. J. (2008). Expectancies modulate attentional capture by salient color singletons. Vision Research, 48, 1315–1326.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gilbert, A. L., Regier, T., Kay, P., & Ivry, R. B. (2006). Whorf hypothesis is supported in the right visual field but not in the left. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103, 489–494.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef Gilbert, A. L., Regier, T., Kay, P., & Ivry, R. B. (2006). Whorf hypothesis is supported in the right visual field but not in the left. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103, 489–494.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Grubert, A., & Eimer, M. (2013). Qualitative differences in the guidance of attention during single-colour and multiple-colour visual search: Behavioural and electrophysiological evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance (in press). Grubert, A., & Eimer, M. (2013). Qualitative differences in the guidance of attention during single-colour and multiple-colour visual search: Behavioural and electrophysiological evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance (in press).
go back to reference Harris, A., Remington, R. W., & Becker, S. I. (2013). Feature-specificity in attentional capture by size and colour. Journal of Vision (in press). Harris, A., Remington, R. W., & Becker, S. I. (2013). Feature-specificity in attentional capture by size and colour. Journal of Vision (in press).
go back to reference Hodsoll, J. P., & Humphreys, G. W. (2005). The effect of target foreknowledge on visual search for categorically separable orientation targets. Vision Research, 45, 2346–2351.PubMedCrossRef Hodsoll, J. P., & Humphreys, G. W. (2005). The effect of target foreknowledge on visual search for categorically separable orientation targets. Vision Research, 45, 2346–2351.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Irons, J. L., Folk, C. L., & Remington, R. W. (2012). All set! Evidence of simultaneous attentional control settings for multiple target colors. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 758–775.PubMed Irons, J. L., Folk, C. L., & Remington, R. W. (2012). All set! Evidence of simultaneous attentional control settings for multiple target colors. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 758–775.PubMed
go back to reference Itti, L., & Koch, C. (2001). Computational modelling of visual attention. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 4–11.CrossRef Itti, L., & Koch, C. (2001). Computational modelling of visual attention. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 4–11.CrossRef
go back to reference Kim, M.-S., & Cave, K. (1999). Top-down and bottom-up attentional control: On the nature of interference from a salient distractor. Perception & Psychophysics, 61, 1009–1013.CrossRef Kim, M.-S., & Cave, K. (1999). Top-down and bottom-up attentional control: On the nature of interference from a salient distractor. Perception & Psychophysics, 61, 1009–1013.CrossRef
go back to reference Maljkovic, V., & Nakayama, K. (1994). Priming of pop-out: I. Role of features. Memory & Cognition, 22, 657–672.CrossRef Maljkovic, V., & Nakayama, K. (1994). Priming of pop-out: I. Role of features. Memory & Cognition, 22, 657–672.CrossRef
go back to reference Müller, H. J., Geyer, T., Zehetleitner, M., & Krummenacher, J. (2009). Attentional capture by salient color singleton distractors is modulated by top-down dimensional set. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 1–16.PubMed Müller, H. J., Geyer, T., Zehetleitner, M., & Krummenacher, J. (2009). Attentional capture by salient color singleton distractors is modulated by top-down dimensional set. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 1–16.PubMed
go back to reference Navalpakkam, V., & Itti, L. (2007). Search goal tunes visual features optimally. Neuron, 53, 605–617.PubMedCrossRef Navalpakkam, V., & Itti, L. (2007). Search goal tunes visual features optimally. Neuron, 53, 605–617.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Theeuwes, J. (1992). Perceptual selectivity for color and form. Perception & Psychophysics, 51, 599–606.CrossRef Theeuwes, J. (1992). Perceptual selectivity for color and form. Perception & Psychophysics, 51, 599–606.CrossRef
go back to reference Theeuwes, J., Atchley, P., & Kramer, A. F. (2000). On the time course of top-down and bottom-up control of visual attention. In S. Monsell & J. Driver (Eds.), Attention and performance XVIII (pp. 105–125). Cambridge: MIT Press. Theeuwes, J., Atchley, P., & Kramer, A. F. (2000). On the time course of top-down and bottom-up control of visual attention. In S. Monsell & J. Driver (Eds.), Attention and performance XVIII (pp. 105–125). Cambridge: MIT Press.
go back to reference Töllner, T., Müller, H., & Zehetleitner, M. (2012). Top-down dimensional weight set determines the capture of visual attention: Evidence from the PCN component. Cerebral Cortex, 22, 1554–1563.PubMedCrossRef Töllner, T., Müller, H., & Zehetleitner, M. (2012). Top-down dimensional weight set determines the capture of visual attention: Evidence from the PCN component. Cerebral Cortex, 22, 1554–1563.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Torralba, A., Oliva, A., Castelhano, M. S., & Henderson, J. M. (2006). Contextual guidance of eye movements and attention in real-world scenes: The role of global features in object search. Psychological Review, 113, 766–786.PubMedCrossRef Torralba, A., Oliva, A., Castelhano, M. S., & Henderson, J. M. (2006). Contextual guidance of eye movements and attention in real-world scenes: The role of global features in object search. Psychological Review, 113, 766–786.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Witzel, C., & Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2011). Is there a lateralized category effect for color? Journal of Vision, 11(12), 16.PubMedCrossRef Witzel, C., & Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2011). Is there a lateralized category effect for color? Journal of Vision, 11(12), 16.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wolfe, J. M. (1994). Guided search 2.0: A revised model of visual search. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1, 202–238.CrossRef Wolfe, J. M. (1994). Guided search 2.0: A revised model of visual search. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1, 202–238.CrossRef
go back to reference Worschech, F., & Ansorge, U. (2012). Top-down search for color prevents voluntary directing of attention to informative singleton cues. Experimental Psychology, 59, 153–162.PubMedCrossRef Worschech, F., & Ansorge, U. (2012). Top-down search for color prevents voluntary directing of attention to informative singleton cues. Experimental Psychology, 59, 153–162.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Yantis, S., & Hillstrom, A. P. (1994). Stimulus-driven attentional capture: Evidence from equiluminant visual objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 601–621. Yantis, S., & Hillstrom, A. P. (1994). Stimulus-driven attentional capture: Evidence from equiluminant visual objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 601–621.
Metagegevens
Titel
Contingent capture in cueing: the role of color search templates and cue-target color relations
Auteurs
Ulrich Ansorge
Stefanie I. Becker
Publicatiedatum
01-03-2014
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 2/2014
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-013-0497-5

Andere artikelen Uitgave 2/2014

Psychological Research 2/2014 Naar de uitgave