Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 1/2014

01-01-2014 | Original Article

Tool characteristics in imagery of tool actions

Auteurs: Martina Rieger, Cristina Massen

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 1/2014

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Are tool characteristics represented in imagined tool actions? In two experiments participants imagined and executed coloring rectangles with a thick and a thin pen. In Experiment 2, an additional execution condition without visual feedback of coloring allowed us to dissociate between the relevance of kinesthetic and visual feedback. Pen thickness influenced coloring durations in all conditions, indicating that characteristics of a simple tool are represented during imagery. Imagination was shorter than execution, indicating that imagination may be less detailed than execution. Execution without visual feedback was even shorter than imagination, indicating that vision is more important than kinesthesis for differences between imagination and execution, and that either imagining the movement, inhibiting movement execution or imagining the progress of the action is effortful during imagery. In conclusion, characteristics of simple tools are represented in imagined tool actions but the representation of tools’ effects may not always be adequate.
Voetnoten
1
We also analyzed (here and in “Experiment 2”) the accuracy with which participants colored the rectangles (% colored area) in the execution condition and correlated it with the execution—imagination difference in coloring duration. Correlations seemed to indicate that participants who colored lesser area of the rectangle showed lower execution—imagination differences. However, most correlations were low and not significant. This was probably due to low variability in the variable percentage of colored area, as most participants colored the rectangles nearly completely. We therefore decided not to report those results.
 
2
Participants also performed another task: after coloring each rectangle they were presented the same rectangle and were asked to draw the first stroke which they had just drawn. Because this task did not yield any interesting findings we will not report the results.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Anderson, N. H., & Weiss, D. J. (1971). Test of a multiplying model for estimated area of rectangles. American Journal of Psychology, 84, 543–548.PubMedCrossRef Anderson, N. H., & Weiss, D. J. (1971). Test of a multiplying model for estimated area of rectangles. American Journal of Psychology, 84, 543–548.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Beilock, S. L., Carr, T. H., MacMahon, C., & Starkes, J. L. (2002). When paying attention becomes counterproductive: impact of divided versus skill-focused attention on novice and experienced performance of sensorimotor skills. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 8, 6–16.PubMed Beilock, S. L., Carr, T. H., MacMahon, C., & Starkes, J. L. (2002). When paying attention becomes counterproductive: impact of divided versus skill-focused attention on novice and experienced performance of sensorimotor skills. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 8, 6–16.PubMed
go back to reference Campos, J. L., Siegle, J. H., Mohler, B. J., Bülthoff, H. H., & Loomis, J. M. (2009). Imagined self-motion differs from perceived self-motion: evidence from a novel continuous pointing method. PLoS ONE, 4(11), e7793.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef Campos, J. L., Siegle, J. H., Mohler, B. J., Bülthoff, H. H., & Loomis, J. M. (2009). Imagined self-motion differs from perceived self-motion: evidence from a novel continuous pointing method. PLoS ONE, 4(11), e7793.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Courtine, G., Papaxanthis, C., Gentili, R., & Pozzo, T. (2004). Gait-dependent motor memory facilitation in covert movement execution. Cognitive Brain Research, 22, 67–75.PubMedCrossRef Courtine, G., Papaxanthis, C., Gentili, R., & Pozzo, T. (2004). Gait-dependent motor memory facilitation in covert movement execution. Cognitive Brain Research, 22, 67–75.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Creem-Regehr, S. H., & Lee, J. N. (2005). Neural representations of graspable objects: are tools special? Cognitive Brain Research, 22, 457–469.PubMedCrossRef Creem-Regehr, S. H., & Lee, J. N. (2005). Neural representations of graspable objects: are tools special? Cognitive Brain Research, 22, 457–469.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Davidson, P. R., & Wolpert, D. M. (2005). Widespread access to predictive models in the motor system: a short review. Journal of Neural Engineering, 2, 313–319.CrossRef Davidson, P. R., & Wolpert, D. M. (2005). Widespread access to predictive models in the motor system: a short review. Journal of Neural Engineering, 2, 313–319.CrossRef
go back to reference Davoli, C. C., Brockmole, J. R., & Witt, J. K. (2011). Compressing perceived distance with remote tool-use: real, imagined, and remembered. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. Advance online publication. Davoli, C. C., Brockmole, J. R., & Witt, J. K. (2011). Compressing perceived distance with remote tool-use: real, imagined, and remembered. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. Advance online publication.
go back to reference Decety, J. (1991). Motor information may be important for updating the cognitive processes involved in mental imagery of movement. European Bulletin of Cognitive Psychology, 4, 415–426. Decety, J. (1991). Motor information may be important for updating the cognitive processes involved in mental imagery of movement. European Bulletin of Cognitive Psychology, 4, 415–426.
go back to reference Decety, J. (1996). The neurophysiological basis of motor imagery. Behavioural Brain Research, 77, 45–52.PubMedCrossRef Decety, J. (1996). The neurophysiological basis of motor imagery. Behavioural Brain Research, 77, 45–52.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Decety, J., & Jeannerod, M. (1996). Mentally simulated movements in virtual reality: does Fitts’s law hold in motor imagery? Behavioural Brain Research, 72, 127–134.CrossRef Decety, J., & Jeannerod, M. (1996). Mentally simulated movements in virtual reality: does Fitts’s law hold in motor imagery? Behavioural Brain Research, 72, 127–134.CrossRef
go back to reference Decety, J., Jeannerod, M., & Prablanc, C. (1989). The timing of mentally represented actions. Behavioural Brain Research, 34, 35–42.PubMedCrossRef Decety, J., Jeannerod, M., & Prablanc, C. (1989). The timing of mentally represented actions. Behavioural Brain Research, 34, 35–42.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Decety, J., & Michel, F. (1989). Comparative analysis of actual and mental movement times in two graphic tasks. Brain and Cognition, 11, 87–97.PubMedCrossRef Decety, J., & Michel, F. (1989). Comparative analysis of actual and mental movement times in two graphic tasks. Brain and Cognition, 11, 87–97.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fourkas, A. D., Bonavolontà, V., Avenanti, A., & Aglioti, S. M. (2008). Kinaesthetic imagery and tool-specific modulation of corticospinal representations in expert tennis players. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 2382–2390.PubMedCrossRef Fourkas, A. D., Bonavolontà, V., Avenanti, A., & Aglioti, S. M. (2008). Kinaesthetic imagery and tool-specific modulation of corticospinal representations in expert tennis players. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 2382–2390.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Grealy, M. A., & Shearer, G. F. (2008). Timing processes in motor imagery. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 20(5), 867–892.CrossRef Grealy, M. A., & Shearer, G. F. (2008). Timing processes in motor imagery. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 20(5), 867–892.CrossRef
go back to reference Grush, R. (2004). The emulation theory of representation: motor control, imagery, and perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27(3), 377–396.PubMed Grush, R. (2004). The emulation theory of representation: motor control, imagery, and perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27(3), 377–396.PubMed
go back to reference Guillot, A., & Collet, C. (2005). Duration of mentally simulated movement: a review. Journal of Motor Behavior, 37, 10–20.PubMedCrossRef Guillot, A., & Collet, C. (2005). Duration of mentally simulated movement: a review. Journal of Motor Behavior, 37, 10–20.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Guillot, A., Debarnot, U., Louis, M., Hoyek, N., & Collet, C. (2010). Motor imagery and motor performance: evidence from the sport science literature. In A. Guillot & C. Collet (Eds.), The neurophysiological foundations of mental and motor imagery (pp. 215–226). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRef Guillot, A., Debarnot, U., Louis, M., Hoyek, N., & Collet, C. (2010). Motor imagery and motor performance: evidence from the sport science literature. In A. Guillot & C. Collet (Eds.), The neurophysiological foundations of mental and motor imagery (pp. 215–226). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Higuchi, S., Imamizu, H., & Kawato, M. (2007). Cerebellar activity evoked by common tool-use execution and imagery tasks: an fMRI study. Cortex, 43, 350–358.PubMedCrossRef Higuchi, S., Imamizu, H., & Kawato, M. (2007). Cerebellar activity evoked by common tool-use execution and imagery tasks: an fMRI study. Cortex, 43, 350–358.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Imazu, S., Sugio, T., Tanaka, S., & Inui, T. (2007). Differences between actual and imagined usage of chopsticks: an fmri study. Cortex, 43, 301–307.PubMedCrossRef Imazu, S., Sugio, T., Tanaka, S., & Inui, T. (2007). Differences between actual and imagined usage of chopsticks: an fmri study. Cortex, 43, 301–307.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Jeannerod, M. (1994). The representing brain: neural correlates of motor intention and imagery. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17, 187–245.CrossRef Jeannerod, M. (1994). The representing brain: neural correlates of motor intention and imagery. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17, 187–245.CrossRef
go back to reference Komesu, Y., Urwitz-Lane, R., Ozel, B., Lukban, J., Kahn, M, Muir, T. Fenner, D., & Rogers, R. (2009). Does mental imagery prior to cytoscopy make a difference? A randomized controlled trial. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology, 201(2), 218e1–218e9. Komesu, Y., Urwitz-Lane, R., Ozel, B., Lukban, J., Kahn, M, Muir, T. Fenner, D., & Rogers, R. (2009). Does mental imagery prior to cytoscopy make a difference? A randomized controlled trial. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology, 201(2), 218e1–218e9.
go back to reference Kunz, B. R., Creem-Regehr, S. H., & Thompson, W. B. (2009). Evidence for motor simulation in imagined locomotion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(5), 1458–1471.PubMed Kunz, B. R., Creem-Regehr, S. H., & Thompson, W. B. (2009). Evidence for motor simulation in imagined locomotion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(5), 1458–1471.PubMed
go back to reference Logan, G. D., & Crump, M. J. C. (2009). The left hand doesn’t know what the right hand is doing. Psychological Science, 20(10), 1296–1300.PubMedCrossRef Logan, G. D., & Crump, M. J. C. (2009). The left hand doesn’t know what the right hand is doing. Psychological Science, 20(10), 1296–1300.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Macuga, K. L., Papailiou, A. P., & Frey, S. H. (2012). Motor imagery of tool use: relationship to actual use and adherence to Fitts’ law across tasks. Experimental Brain Research, 218, 169–179.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef Macuga, K. L., Papailiou, A. P., & Frey, S. H. (2012). Motor imagery of tool use: relationship to actual use and adherence to Fitts’ law across tasks. Experimental Brain Research, 218, 169–179.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef
go back to reference McAvinue, L. P., & Robertson, I. H. (2008). Measuring motor imagery ability: a review. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 20, 232–251.CrossRef McAvinue, L. P., & Robertson, I. H. (2008). Measuring motor imagery ability: a review. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 20, 232–251.CrossRef
go back to reference Munzert, J., Lorey, B., & Zentgraf, K. (2009). Cognitive motor processes: the role of motor imagery in the study of motor representations. Brain Research Reviews, 60, 306–326.PubMedCrossRef Munzert, J., Lorey, B., & Zentgraf, K. (2009). Cognitive motor processes: the role of motor imagery in the study of motor representations. Brain Research Reviews, 60, 306–326.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Noel, R. C. (1980). The effects of visuo-motor behavior rehearsal on tennis performance. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 2, 221–226. Noel, R. C. (1980). The effects of visuo-motor behavior rehearsal on tennis performance. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 2, 221–226.
go back to reference Papaxanthis, C., Schieppati, M., Gentili, R., & Pozzo, T. (2002). Imagined and actual arm movements have similar durations when performed under conditions of direction and mass. Experimental Brain Research, 143, 447–452.PubMedCrossRef Papaxanthis, C., Schieppati, M., Gentili, R., & Pozzo, T. (2002). Imagined and actual arm movements have similar durations when performed under conditions of direction and mass. Experimental Brain Research, 143, 447–452.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pylyshyn, Z. W. (2002). Mental imagery: in search of a theory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 157–238.PubMed Pylyshyn, Z. W. (2002). Mental imagery: in search of a theory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 157–238.PubMed
go back to reference Rieger, M., Martinez, F., & Wenke, D. (2011). Imagery of errors in typing. Cognition, 121, 163–175.PubMedCrossRef Rieger, M., Martinez, F., & Wenke, D. (2011). Imagery of errors in typing. Cognition, 121, 163–175.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Schwartz, D. L., & Holton, D. L. (2000). Tool use and the effect of action on the imagination. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 1655–1665.PubMedCrossRef Schwartz, D. L., & Holton, D. L. (2000). Tool use and the effect of action on the imagination. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 1655–1665.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Taylor, J. A., & Shaw, D. F. (2002). The effects of outcome imagery on golf-putting performance. Journal of Sports Sciences, 20(8), 607–613.PubMedCrossRef Taylor, J. A., & Shaw, D. F. (2002). The effects of outcome imagery on golf-putting performance. Journal of Sports Sciences, 20(8), 607–613.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Witt, J. K., & Proffitt, D. R. (2008). Action-specific Influences on Distance Perception: a Role for Motor Simulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 1479–1492.PubMedCentralPubMed Witt, J. K., & Proffitt, D. R. (2008). Action-specific Influences on Distance Perception: a Role for Motor Simulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 1479–1492.PubMedCentralPubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Tool characteristics in imagery of tool actions
Auteurs
Martina Rieger
Cristina Massen
Publicatiedatum
01-01-2014
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 1/2014
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-013-0481-0

Andere artikelen Uitgave 1/2014

Psychological Research 1/2014 Naar de uitgave