Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 1/2013

01-01-2013 | Editorial

Vision, action and language unified through embodiment

Auteurs: Daniele Caligiore, Martin H. Fischer

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 1/2013

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Excerpt

Increasing evidence shows that vision, action and language should not be regarded as a set of disembodied processes. Instead, they form a closely integrated and highly dynamic system that is attuned to the constraints of its bodily implementation as well as to the constraints coming from the world with which this body interacts. One consequence of such embodiment of cognition is that seeing an object, even when there is no intention to handle it, activates plans for actions directed toward it (e.g., Tucker & Ellis, 1998, 2001; Fischer & Dahl, 2007). Using object names induces similar action planning effects as seeing the objects themselves (Tucker & Ellis, 2004; Borghi, Glenberg & Kaschak, 2004). Depending on linguistic context, different object features can be activated for action planning, as indicated by facilitated manual responses or “affordance effects” (e.g., Borghi, 2004; Glenberg & Robertson, 2000; Zwaan, 2004). Similarly, different action intentions direct attention differently to object features for processing (e.g., Bekkering & Neggers, 2002; Fischer & Hoellen, 2004; Symes, Tucker, Ellis, Vainio, & Ottoboni, 2008). Eye movements during visually guided actions shed further light on the close relationship between vision, action and language (Land & Furneaux, 1997; Johansson, Westling, Bäckström, & Flanagan, 2001). For example when humans interact with objects, their eyes move ahead of their hands to support the on-line control of grasping (e.g., Bekkering & Neggers, 2002). …
Literatuur
go back to reference Arbib, M. A., & Lee, J. Y. (2007). Vision and action in the language-ready brain: From mirror neurons to SemRep. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 4729, 104.CrossRef Arbib, M. A., & Lee, J. Y. (2007). Vision and action in the language-ready brain: From mirror neurons to SemRep. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 4729, 104.CrossRef
go back to reference Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 577–660.PubMed Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 577–660.PubMed
go back to reference Bekkering, H., & Neggers, S. F. W. (2002). Visual search is modulated by action intentions. Psychological Science, 13, 370–374.PubMedCrossRef Bekkering, H., & Neggers, S. F. W. (2002). Visual search is modulated by action intentions. Psychological Science, 13, 370–374.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bonaiuto, J., & Arbib, M. A. (2010). Extending the mirror neuron system model, II: What did I just do? A new role for mirror neurons. Biological Cybernetics, 102, 341–259. Bonaiuto, J., & Arbib, M. A. (2010). Extending the mirror neuron system model, II: What did I just do? A new role for mirror neurons. Biological Cybernetics, 102, 341–259.
go back to reference Bonaiuto, J., Rosta, E., & Arbib, M. A. (2007). Extending the mirror neuron system model, I. Biological Cybernetics, 96, 9–38.PubMedCrossRef Bonaiuto, J., Rosta, E., & Arbib, M. A. (2007). Extending the mirror neuron system model, I. Biological Cybernetics, 96, 9–38.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Borghi, A. M. (2004). Object concepts and action: Extracting affordances from objects parts. Acta Psychologica, 115(1), 69–96.PubMedCrossRef Borghi, A. M. (2004). Object concepts and action: Extracting affordances from objects parts. Acta Psychologica, 115(1), 69–96.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Borghi, A. M., & Cimatti, F. (2009). Words as tools and the problem of abstract words meanings. In N. Taatgen & H. van Rijn (Eds.), Proceedings of the 31st Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 2304–2309). Amsterdam: Cognitive Science Society. Borghi, A. M., & Cimatti, F. (2009). Words as tools and the problem of abstract words meanings. In N. Taatgen & H. van Rijn (Eds.), Proceedings of the 31st Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 2304–2309). Amsterdam: Cognitive Science Society.
go back to reference Borghi, A. M., Glenberg, A. M., & Kaschak, M. (2004). Putting words in perspective. Memory & Cognition, 32, 863–873.CrossRef Borghi, A. M., Glenberg, A. M., & Kaschak, M. (2004). Putting words in perspective. Memory & Cognition, 32, 863–873.CrossRef
go back to reference Buccino, G., Binkofski, F., Fink, G. R., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., et al. (2001). Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMRI study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 13, 400–404.PubMed Buccino, G., Binkofski, F., Fink, G. R., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., et al. (2001). Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMRI study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 13, 400–404.PubMed
go back to reference Buccino, G., Binkofski, F., & Riggio, L. (2004). The mirror neuron system and action recognition. Brain and Language, 89, 370–376.PubMedCrossRef Buccino, G., Binkofski, F., & Riggio, L. (2004). The mirror neuron system and action recognition. Brain and Language, 89, 370–376.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Caligiore, D., Borghi, A. M., Parisi, D., & Baldassarre, G. (2010). TRoPICALS: A computational embodied neuroscience model of compatibility effects. Psychological Review, 117, 1188–1228.PubMedCrossRef Caligiore, D., Borghi, A. M., Parisi, D., & Baldassarre, G. (2010). TRoPICALS: A computational embodied neuroscience model of compatibility effects. Psychological Review, 117, 1188–1228.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Caligiore, D., Borghi, A. M., Parisi, D., Ellis, R., Cangelosi, A., & Baldassarre, G. (2012). How affordances associated with a distractor object affect compatibility effects: A study with the computational model TRoPICALS. Psychological Research (this issue). Caligiore, D., Borghi, A. M., Parisi, D., Ellis, R., Cangelosi, A., & Baldassarre, G. (2012). How affordances associated with a distractor object affect compatibility effects: A study with the computational model TRoPICALS. Psychological Research (this issue).
go back to reference Cangelosi, A., Hourdakis, E., & Tikhanoff, V. (2006). Language acquisition and symbol grounding transfer with neural networks and cognitive robots. The 2006 IEEE International Joint Conference on Neural Network Proceedings (pp. 1576–1582). Cangelosi, A., Hourdakis, E., & Tikhanoff, V. (2006). Language acquisition and symbol grounding transfer with neural networks and cognitive robots. The 2006 IEEE International Joint Conference on Neural Network Proceedings (pp. 1576–1582).
go back to reference Cangelosi, A., & Parisi, D. (2002). Simulating the evolution of language. In A. Cangelosi & D. Parisi (Eds.), Simulating the Evolution of Language (p. xii + 356). Berlin: Springer. Cangelosi, A., & Parisi, D. (2002). Simulating the evolution of language. In A. Cangelosi & D. Parisi (Eds.), Simulating the Evolution of Language (p. xii + 356). Berlin: Springer.
go back to reference Cangelosi, A., & Riga, T. (2006). An embodied model for sensorimotor grounding and grounding transfer: experiments with robots. Cognitive Science, 30, 673–689.PubMedCrossRef Cangelosi, A., & Riga, T. (2006). An embodied model for sensorimotor grounding and grounding transfer: experiments with robots. Cognitive Science, 30, 673–689.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference De Vega, M., Moreno, V., & Castillo, D. (2012). The comprehension of action-related sentences may cause interference rather than facilitation on matching actions. Psychological Research (this issue). De Vega, M., Moreno, V., & Castillo, D. (2012). The comprehension of action-related sentences may cause interference rather than facilitation on matching actions. Psychological Research (this issue).
go back to reference Ellis, R., Swabey, D., Bridgeman, J., May, B., Tucker, M., & Hyne, A. (2012). Bodies and other visual objects: The dialectics of reaching toward objects. Psychological Research (this issue). Ellis, R., Swabey, D., Bridgeman, J., May, B., Tucker, M., & Hyne, A. (2012). Bodies and other visual objects: The dialectics of reaching toward objects. Psychological Research (this issue).
go back to reference Fadiga, L., Craighero, L., Fabbri Destro, M., Finos, L., Cotillon Williams, N., Smith, A. T., et al. (2006). Language in shadow. Social Neuroscience, 1, 77–89.PubMedCrossRef Fadiga, L., Craighero, L., Fabbri Destro, M., Finos, L., Cotillon Williams, N., Smith, A. T., et al. (2006). Language in shadow. Social Neuroscience, 1, 77–89.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (2000). Visuomotor neurons: Ambiguity of the discharge or ‘motor’ perception? International Journal of Psychophysiology, 35, 165–177.PubMedCrossRef Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (2000). Visuomotor neurons: Ambiguity of the discharge or ‘motor’ perception? International Journal of Psychophysiology, 35, 165–177.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fagg, A. H., & Arbib, M. A. (1998). Modeling parietal-premotor interaction in primate control of grasping. Neural Networks, 11, 1277–1303.PubMedCrossRef Fagg, A. H., & Arbib, M. A. (1998). Modeling parietal-premotor interaction in primate control of grasping. Neural Networks, 11, 1277–1303.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fischer, M. H., & Dahl, C. (2007). The time course of visuo-motor affordances. Experimental Brain Research, 176, 519–524.CrossRef Fischer, M. H., & Dahl, C. (2007). The time course of visuo-motor affordances. Experimental Brain Research, 176, 519–524.CrossRef
go back to reference Fischer, M. H., & Hoellen, N. (2004). Space- and object-based attention depend on motor intention. The Journal of General Psychology, 13, 365–377. Fischer, M. H., & Hoellen, N. (2004). Space- and object-based attention depend on motor intention. The Journal of General Psychology, 13, 365–377.
go back to reference Fleischer, J. G., & Edelman, G. M. (2009). Brain-based devices: An embodied approach to linking nervous system structure and function to behavior. IEEE Robotics & Automation Magazine, 16, 33–41.CrossRef Fleischer, J. G., & Edelman, G. M. (2009). Brain-based devices: An embodied approach to linking nervous system structure and function to behavior. IEEE Robotics & Automation Magazine, 16, 33–41.CrossRef
go back to reference Fogassi, L., Ferrari, P. F., Gesierich, B., Rozzi, S., Chersi, F., & Rizzolatti, G. (2005). Parietal lobe: From action organization to intention understanding. Science, 308, 662–667.PubMedCrossRef Fogassi, L., Ferrari, P. F., Gesierich, B., Rozzi, S., Chersi, F., & Rizzolatti, G. (2005). Parietal lobe: From action organization to intention understanding. Science, 308, 662–667.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Friston, K. (2009). Causal modelling and brain connectivity in functional magnetic resonance imaging. PLoS Biol, 7(2), e33.PubMedCrossRef Friston, K. (2009). Causal modelling and brain connectivity in functional magnetic resonance imaging. PLoS Biol, 7(2), e33.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gallese, V. (2008). Mirror neurons and the social nature of language: The neural exploitation hypothesis. Social Neuroscience, 3, 317–333.PubMedCrossRef Gallese, V. (2008). Mirror neurons and the social nature of language: The neural exploitation hypothesis. Social Neuroscience, 3, 317–333.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Garagnani, M., Wennekers, T., & Pulvermüller, F. (2008). A neuroanatomically grounded Hebbian-learning model of attention-language interactions in the human brain. European Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 492–513.PubMedCrossRef Garagnani, M., Wennekers, T., & Pulvermüller, F. (2008). A neuroanatomically grounded Hebbian-learning model of attention-language interactions in the human brain. European Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 492–513.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gentilucci, M. (2003). Grasp observation influences speech production. European Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 179–184.PubMedCrossRef Gentilucci, M. (2003). Grasp observation influences speech production. European Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 179–184.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gentilucci, M., Benuzzi, F., Gangitano, M., & Grimaldi, S. (2001). Grasp with hand and mouth: A kinematic study on healthy subjects. Journal of Neurophysiology, 86, 1685–1699.PubMed Gentilucci, M., Benuzzi, F., Gangitano, M., & Grimaldi, S. (2001). Grasp with hand and mouth: A kinematic study on healthy subjects. Journal of Neurophysiology, 86, 1685–1699.PubMed
go back to reference Gianelli, C., Scorolli, C., & Borghi, A. M. (2012). Acting in perspective: The role of body and language as social tools. Psychological Research (this issue). Gianelli, C., Scorolli, C., & Borghi, A. M. (2012). Acting in perspective: The role of body and language as social tools. Psychological Research (this issue).
go back to reference Glenberg, A. M. (2010). Embodiment as a unifying perspective for psychology. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 1, 586–596. Glenberg, A. M. (2010). Embodiment as a unifying perspective for psychology. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 1, 586–596.
go back to reference Glenberg, A. M., & Kaschak, M. P. (2002). Grounding language in action. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 558–565.CrossRef Glenberg, A. M., & Kaschak, M. P. (2002). Grounding language in action. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 558–565.CrossRef
go back to reference Glenberg, A. M., & Robertson, D. A. (1999). Indexical understanding of instructions. Discourse Processes, 28, 1–26.CrossRef Glenberg, A. M., & Robertson, D. A. (1999). Indexical understanding of instructions. Discourse Processes, 28, 1–26.CrossRef
go back to reference Glenberg, A. M., & Robertson, D. A. (2000). Symbol grounding and meaning: A comparison of high-dimensional and embodied theories of meaning. Journal of Memory and Language, 43, 379–401.CrossRef Glenberg, A. M., & Robertson, D. A. (2000). Symbol grounding and meaning: A comparison of high-dimensional and embodied theories of meaning. Journal of Memory and Language, 43, 379–401.CrossRef
go back to reference Grèzes, J., Tucker, M., Armony, J., Ellis, R., & Passingham, R. E. (2003). Objects automatically potentiate action: An fMRI study of implicit processing. European Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 2735–2740.PubMedCrossRef Grèzes, J., Tucker, M., Armony, J., Ellis, R., & Passingham, R. E. (2003). Objects automatically potentiate action: An fMRI study of implicit processing. European Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 2735–2740.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hanlon, R. E., Brown, J. W., & Gerstman, L. J. (1990). Enhancement of naming in nonfluent aphasia through gesture. Brain and Language, 38, 298–314.PubMedCrossRef Hanlon, R. E., Brown, J. W., & Gerstman, L. J. (1990). Enhancement of naming in nonfluent aphasia through gesture. Brain and Language, 38, 298–314.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hauk, O., Johnsrude, I., & Pulvermüller, F. (2004). Somatotopic representation of action words in human motor and premotor cortex. Neuron, 41, 301–307.PubMedCrossRef Hauk, O., Johnsrude, I., & Pulvermüller, F. (2004). Somatotopic representation of action words in human motor and premotor cortex. Neuron, 41, 301–307.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hommel, B., & Colzato, L. S. (2010). Games with(out) Frontiers: Towards an integrated science of human cognition. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 2.PubMedCrossRef Hommel, B., & Colzato, L. S. (2010). Games with(out) Frontiers: Towards an integrated science of human cognition. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 2.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Iizuka, H., Marocco, D., Ando, H., & Maeda, T. (2012). An experimental study on co-evolution of categorical perception and communication system in humans. Psychological Research (this issue). Iizuka, H., Marocco, D., Ando, H., & Maeda, T. (2012). An experimental study on co-evolution of categorical perception and communication system in humans. Psychological Research (this issue).
go back to reference Jeannerod, M. (2007). Motor cognition: What actions tell the self. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Jeannerod, M. (2007). Motor cognition: What actions tell the self. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Johansson, R. S., Westling, G., Bäckström, A., & Flanagan, J. R. (2001). Eye-hand coordination in object manipulation. Journal of Neuroscience, 21, 6917–6932.PubMed Johansson, R. S., Westling, G., Bäckström, A., & Flanagan, J. R. (2001). Eye-hand coordination in object manipulation. Journal of Neuroscience, 21, 6917–6932.PubMed
go back to reference Johnson-Frey, S. H., Maloof, F. R., Newman-Norlund, R., Farrer, C., Inati, S., & Grafton, S. T. (2003). Actions or hand-objects interactions? Human inferior frontal cortex and action observation. Neuron, 39, 1053–1058.PubMedCrossRef Johnson-Frey, S. H., Maloof, F. R., Newman-Norlund, R., Farrer, C., Inati, S., & Grafton, S. T. (2003). Actions or hand-objects interactions? Human inferior frontal cortex and action observation. Neuron, 39, 1053–1058.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, fire, and dangerous things: What categories reveal about the mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, fire, and dangerous things: What categories reveal about the mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
go back to reference Land, M. F., & Furneaux, S. (1997). The knowledge base of the oculomotor system. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London—Series B: Biological Sciences, 352(1358), 1231–1239.PubMedCrossRef Land, M. F., & Furneaux, S. (1997). The knowledge base of the oculomotor system. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London—Series B: Biological Sciences, 352(1358), 1231–1239.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Li, P., Farkas, I., & MacWhinney, B. (2004). Early lexical development in a self-organizing neural network. Neural Networks, 17, 1345–1362.PubMedCrossRef Li, P., Farkas, I., & MacWhinney, B. (2004). Early lexical development in a self-organizing neural network. Neural Networks, 17, 1345–1362.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference MacWhinney, B. (1998). Models of the emergence of language. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 199–227.PubMedCrossRef MacWhinney, B. (1998). Models of the emergence of language. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 199–227.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mannella, F., Mirolli, M., & Baldassarre, G. (2010). The interplay of Pavlovian and instrumental processes in devaluation experiments: A computational embodied neuroscience model tested with a simulated rat. In C. R. Tosh & G. D. Ruxton (Eds.), Modelling perception using artificial neural networks. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Mannella, F., Mirolli, M., & Baldassarre, G. (2010). The interplay of Pavlovian and instrumental processes in devaluation experiments: A computational embodied neuroscience model tested with a simulated rat. In C. R. Tosh & G. D. Ruxton (Eds.), Modelling perception using artificial neural networks. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Marino, B., Gough, P. M., Gallese, V., Riggio, L., & Buccino, G. (2012). How the motor system handles nouns: A behavioural study. Psychological Research (this issue). Marino, B., Gough, P. M., Gallese, V., Riggio, L., & Buccino, G. (2012). How the motor system handles nouns: A behavioural study. Psychological Research (this issue).
go back to reference Mayor, J., & Plunkett, K. (2010). A neurocomputational account of taxonomic responding and fast mapping in early word learning. Psychological Review, 117, 1–31.PubMedCrossRef Mayor, J., & Plunkett, K. (2010). A neurocomputational account of taxonomic responding and fast mapping in early word learning. Psychological Review, 117, 1–31.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference O’Reilly, R. C. (1998). Six Principles for Biologically-Based Computational Models of Cortical Cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 455–462.PubMedCrossRef O’Reilly, R. C. (1998). Six Principles for Biologically-Based Computational Models of Cortical Cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 455–462.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Oztop, E., & Arbib, M. A. (2002). Schema design and implementation of the grasp-related mirror neuron system. Biological Cybernetics, 87, 116–140.PubMedCrossRef Oztop, E., & Arbib, M. A. (2002). Schema design and implementation of the grasp-related mirror neuron system. Biological Cybernetics, 87, 116–140.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pezzulo, G., Barsalou, L. W., Cangelosi, A., Fischer, M. H., Spivey, M., & McRae, K. (2011). The mechanics of embodiment: A dialogue on embodiment and computational modeling. Frontiers in Psychology,. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00005.PubMed Pezzulo, G., Barsalou, L. W., Cangelosi, A., Fischer, M. H., Spivey, M., & McRae, K. (2011). The mechanics of embodiment: A dialogue on embodiment and computational modeling. Frontiers in Psychology,. doi:10.​3389/​fpsyg.​2011.​00005.PubMed
go back to reference Prescott, T. J., Montes-Gonzalez, F., Gurney, K., Humphries, M. D., & Redgrave, P. (2006). A robot model of the basal ganglia: Behavior and intrinsic processing. Neural Networks, 19, 31–61.PubMedCrossRef Prescott, T. J., Montes-Gonzalez, F., Gurney, K., Humphries, M. D., & Redgrave, P. (2006). A robot model of the basal ganglia: Behavior and intrinsic processing. Neural Networks, 19, 31–61.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pulvermüller, F. (2005). Brain mechanisms linking language and action. Nature Review Neuroscience, 6, 576–582.CrossRef Pulvermüller, F. (2005). Brain mechanisms linking language and action. Nature Review Neuroscience, 6, 576–582.CrossRef
go back to reference Pulvermüller, F., & Fadiga, L. (2010). Active perception: Sensorimotor circuits as a cortical basis for language. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 351–360.PubMedCrossRef Pulvermüller, F., & Fadiga, L. (2010). Active perception: Sensorimotor circuits as a cortical basis for language. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 351–360.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rizzolatti, G., & Arbib, M. A. (1998). Language within our grasp. Trends in Neurosciences, 21, 188–194.PubMedCrossRef Rizzolatti, G., & Arbib, M. A. (1998). Language within our grasp. Trends in Neurosciences, 21, 188–194.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169–192.PubMedCrossRef Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169–192.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rothkopf, C. A., & Ballard, D. H. (2010). Credit assignment in multiple goal embodied visuomotor behavior. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 173.PubMedCrossRef Rothkopf, C. A., & Ballard, D. H. (2010). Credit assignment in multiple goal embodied visuomotor behavior. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 173.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sakata, H., Taira, M., Mine, S., & Murata, A. (1992). Hand-movement-related neurons of the posterior parietal cortex of the monkey: Their role in the visual guidance of hand movements. In R. Caminiti, P. B. Johnson, & Y. Burnod (Eds.), Control of arm movement in space: Neurophysiological and computational approaches (pp. 185–198). Heidelberg: Springer.CrossRef Sakata, H., Taira, M., Mine, S., & Murata, A. (1992). Hand-movement-related neurons of the posterior parietal cortex of the monkey: Their role in the visual guidance of hand movements. In R. Caminiti, P. B. Johnson, & Y. Burnod (Eds.), Control of arm movement in space: Neurophysiological and computational approaches (pp. 185–198). Heidelberg: Springer.CrossRef
go back to reference Steels, L. (2003). Evolving grounded communication for robots. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 308–312. Steels, L. (2003). Evolving grounded communication for robots. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 308–312.
go back to reference Steels, L., & Vogt, P. (1997). Grounding adaptive language games in robotic agents. In I. Harvey & P. Husbands (Eds.), Proceedings of the fourth european conference on artificial life (pp. 474–482). MIT Press. Steels, L., & Vogt, P. (1997). Grounding adaptive language games in robotic agents. In I. Harvey & P. Husbands (Eds.), Proceedings of the fourth european conference on artificial life (pp. 474–482). MIT Press.
go back to reference Strick, P. L., Dum, R. P., & Fiez, J. A. (2009). Cerebellum and non-motor function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 32, 413–434.PubMedCrossRef Strick, P. L., Dum, R. P., & Fiez, J. A. (2009). Cerebellum and non-motor function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 32, 413–434.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Symes, E., Tucker, M., Ellis, R., Vainio, L., & Ottoboni, G. (2008). Grasp preparation improves change detection for congruent objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 854–871.PubMedCrossRef Symes, E., Tucker, M., Ellis, R., Vainio, L., & Ottoboni, G. (2008). Grasp preparation improves change detection for congruent objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 854–871.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Tanenhaus, M. K., Spivey-Knowlton, M. J., Eberhard, K. M., & Sedivy, J. E. (1995). Integration of visual and linguistic information in spoken language comprehension. Science, 268, 1632–1634.PubMedCrossRef Tanenhaus, M. K., Spivey-Knowlton, M. J., Eberhard, K. M., & Sedivy, J. E. (1995). Integration of visual and linguistic information in spoken language comprehension. Science, 268, 1632–1634.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Tettamanti, M., Buccino, G., Saccuman, M. C., Gallese, V., Danna, M., Scifo, P., et al. (2005). Listening to action-related sentences activates fronto-parietal motor circuits. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 273–281.PubMedCrossRef Tettamanti, M., Buccino, G., Saccuman, M. C., Gallese, V., Danna, M., Scifo, P., et al. (2005). Listening to action-related sentences activates fronto-parietal motor circuits. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 273–281.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Thill, S., Caligiore, D., Borghi, A. M., Ziemke, T., & Baldassarre, G. (2012). Integrating theories and computational models of affordance control and mirror neurons: A review and a model design. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews (Submitted). Thill, S., Caligiore, D., Borghi, A. M., Ziemke, T., & Baldassarre, G. (2012). Integrating theories and computational models of affordance control and mirror neurons: A review and a model design. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews (Submitted).
go back to reference Tsiotas, G., Borghi, A. M., & Parisi, D. (2005). Objects and affordances: An Artificial Life simulation. In B. Bara, L. Barsalou, B. Bucciarelli (Eds.), COGSCI2005. XXVII Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 2212–2217). Mahwah, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Tsiotas, G., Borghi, A. M., & Parisi, D. (2005). Objects and affordances: An Artificial Life simulation. In B. Bara, L. Barsalou, B. Bucciarelli (Eds.), COGSCI2005. XXVII Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 2212–2217). Mahwah, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
go back to reference Tucker, M., & Ellis, R. (1998). On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 830–846.PubMedCrossRef Tucker, M., & Ellis, R. (1998). On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 830–846.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Tucker, M., & Ellis, R. (2001). The potentiation of grasp types during visual object categorization. Visual Cognition, 8, 769–800.CrossRef Tucker, M., & Ellis, R. (2001). The potentiation of grasp types during visual object categorization. Visual Cognition, 8, 769–800.CrossRef
go back to reference Tucker, M., & Ellis, R. (2004). Action priming by briefly presented objects. Acta Psychologica, 116, 185–203.PubMedCrossRef Tucker, M., & Ellis, R. (2004). Action priming by briefly presented objects. Acta Psychologica, 116, 185–203.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Watkins, K. E., Strafella, A. P., & Paus, T. (2003). Seeing and hearing speech excites the motor system involved in speech production. Neuropsychologia, 41, 989–994.PubMedCrossRef Watkins, K. E., Strafella, A. P., & Paus, T. (2003). Seeing and hearing speech excites the motor system involved in speech production. Neuropsychologia, 41, 989–994.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Weiner, K. S., & Grill-Spector, K. (2010). Sparsely-distributed organization of face and limb activations in human ventral temporal cortex. Neuroimage, 52, 1559–1573. Weiner, K. S., & Grill-Spector, K. (2010). Sparsely-distributed organization of face and limb activations in human ventral temporal cortex. Neuroimage, 52, 1559–1573.
go back to reference Weiner, K. S., & Grill-Spector, K. (2011). Not one extrastriate body area: Using anatomical landmarks, hMT+, and visual field maps to parcellate limb-selective activations in human lateral occipitotemporal cortex. Neuroimage, 56, 2183–2199. Weiner, K. S., & Grill-Spector, K. (2011). Not one extrastriate body area: Using anatomical landmarks, hMT+, and visual field maps to parcellate limb-selective activations in human lateral occipitotemporal cortex. Neuroimage, 56, 2183–2199.
go back to reference Weiner, K. S., & Grill-Spector, K. (2012). Neural representations of faces and limbs neighbor in human high-level visual cortex: Evidence for a new organization principle. Psychological Research (this issue). Weiner, K. S., & Grill-Spector, K. (2012). Neural representations of faces and limbs neighbor in human high-level visual cortex: Evidence for a new organization principle. Psychological Research (this issue).
go back to reference Willems, R. M., & Hagoort, P. (2007). Neural evidence for the interplay between language, gesture and action: A review. Brain and Language, 101, 278–289.PubMedCrossRef Willems, R. M., & Hagoort, P. (2007). Neural evidence for the interplay between language, gesture and action: A review. Brain and Language, 101, 278–289.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Yoon, E. Y., Heinke, D., & Humphreys, G. (2002). Modelling direct perceptual constraints on action selection: The Naming and Action Model (NAM). Visual Cognition, 9, 615–661. Yoon, E. Y., Heinke, D., & Humphreys, G. (2002). Modelling direct perceptual constraints on action selection: The Naming and Action Model (NAM). Visual Cognition, 9, 615–661.
go back to reference Zwaan, R. A. (2004). The immersed experiencer: Toward an embodied theory of language comprehension. In B. H. Ross (ed.), Psychology of learning and motivation, (vol. 44, pp. 35–62). New York: Academic. Zwaan, R. A. (2004). The immersed experiencer: Toward an embodied theory of language comprehension. In B. H. Ross (ed.), Psychology of learning and motivation, (vol. 44, pp. 35–62). New York: Academic.
Metagegevens
Titel
Vision, action and language unified through embodiment
Auteurs
Daniele Caligiore
Martin H. Fischer
Publicatiedatum
01-01-2013
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 1/2013
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-012-0417-0

Andere artikelen Uitgave 1/2013

Psychological Research 1/2013 Naar de uitgave