Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 6/2011

01-11-2011 | Original Article

Interference effects of stimulus–response modality pairings in dual tasks and their robustness

Auteurs: Christine Stelzel, Torsten Schubert

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 6/2011

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Recent evidence suggests that the degree of interference in dual-task situations depends crucially on the pairings of input- and output modalities of the two component tasks with increased dual-task costs for modality incompatible (i.e., visual–vocal and auditory–manual) compared to modality compatible (i.e., visual–manual and auditory–vocal) dual tasks. These effects of modality pairings in dual tasks have been related to the overlap of non-preferred processing pathways in modality incompatible tasks. Until now, modality compatibility has not yet been related to other sources of interference in a dual-task context, such as stimulus–response (S–R) compatibility or crosstalk. In the present study, we conducted two experiments using the paradigm of the psychological refractory period (PRP) to test the effects of S–R compatibility and crosstalk on the effects of modality compatibility in temporally overlapping task situations. Experiment 1 revealed an overadditive interaction between stimulus onset asynchrony and modality compatibility for tasks with S–R compatible mappings, indicating that modality compatibility effects are present in different task situations, even when S–R mappings are otherwise compatible. In Experiment 2, we aimed at pinpointing the boundaries of the effects of modality compatibility in dual-task situations. We showed that additional sources of dual-task interference in a modality compatible dual task could overwrite the pronounced PRP effect previously shown for modality incompatible tasks. Taken together, these data provide new evidence that the specific types of stimulus–response modality pairings are an additional factor that might interact with other sources of interference in dual-task situations.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Voetnoten
1
Note that the inclusion of the between subject factor stimulus order did not change the pattern of results reported below.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Atchley, P., & Dressel, J. (2004). Conversation limits the functional field of view. Human Factors, 46(4), 664–673.PubMedCrossRef Atchley, P., & Dressel, J. (2004). Conversation limits the functional field of view. Human Factors, 46(4), 664–673.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Atchley, P., Dressel, J., Jones, T., Burson, R., & Marshall, D. (2011). Talking and driving: applications of crossmodal action reveal a special role for spatial language. Psychological Research (this issue). Atchley, P., Dressel, J., Jones, T., Burson, R., & Marshall, D. (2011). Talking and driving: applications of crossmodal action reveal a special role for spatial language. Psychological Research (this issue).
go back to reference Cohen, A., & Feintuch, U. (2002). The dimensional-action system: a distinct visual system. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Common mechanisms in perception and action (Vol. 19, pp. 587–608). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Cohen, A., & Feintuch, U. (2002). The dimensional-action system: a distinct visual system. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Common mechanisms in perception and action (Vol. 19, pp. 587–608). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Fuster, J. M. (2000). Executive frontal functions. Experimental Brain Research, 133(1), 66–70.CrossRef Fuster, J. M. (2000). Executive frontal functions. Experimental Brain Research, 133(1), 66–70.CrossRef
go back to reference Greenwald, A. G. (1972). Evidence of both perceptual filtering and response suppression for rejected messages in selective attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 94(1), 58–67.PubMedCrossRef Greenwald, A. G. (1972). Evidence of both perceptual filtering and response suppression for rejected messages in selective attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 94(1), 58–67.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hazeltine, E., & Ruthruff, E. (2006). Modality pairing effects and the response selection bottleneck. Psychological Research, 70(6), 504–513.PubMedCrossRef Hazeltine, E., & Ruthruff, E. (2006). Modality pairing effects and the response selection bottleneck. Psychological Research, 70(6), 504–513.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hazeltine, E., & Wifall, T. (2011). Searching working memory for the source of dual-task costs. Psychological Research (this issue). Hazeltine, E., & Wifall, T. (2011). Searching working memory for the source of dual-task costs. Psychological Research (this issue).
go back to reference Hazeltine, E., Ruthruff, E., & Remington, R. W. (2006). The role of input and output modality pairings in dual-task performance: Evidence for content-dependent central interference. Cognitive Psychology, 52(4), 291–345.PubMedCrossRef Hazeltine, E., Ruthruff, E., & Remington, R. W. (2006). The role of input and output modality pairings in dual-task performance: Evidence for content-dependent central interference. Cognitive Psychology, 52(4), 291–345.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hazeltine, E., Teague, D., & Ivry, R. B. (2002). Simultaneous dual-task performance reveals parallel response selection after practice. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28(3), 527–545.PubMedCrossRef Hazeltine, E., Teague, D., & Ivry, R. B. (2002). Simultaneous dual-task performance reveals parallel response selection after practice. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28(3), 527–545.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1998). Automatic stimulus–response translation in dual-task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24(5), 1368–1384.PubMedCrossRef Hommel, B. (1998). Automatic stimulus–response translation in dual-task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24(5), 1368–1384.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Huestegge, L., & Koch, I. (2009). Dual-task crosstalk between saccades and manual responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(2), 352–362.PubMedCrossRef Huestegge, L., & Koch, I. (2009). Dual-task crosstalk between saccades and manual responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(2), 352–362.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Huestegge, L., & Koch, I. (2010). Crossmodal action selection: evidence from dual-task compatibility. Memory & Cognition, 38(4), 493–501.CrossRef Huestegge, L., & Koch, I. (2010). Crossmodal action selection: evidence from dual-task compatibility. Memory & Cognition, 38(4), 493–501.CrossRef
go back to reference Israel, M., & Cohen, A. (2011). Involuntary strategy-dependent dual task performance. Psychological Research (this issue). Israel, M., & Cohen, A. (2011). Involuntary strategy-dependent dual task performance. Psychological Research (this issue).
go back to reference Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus–response compatibility. A model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97(2), 253–270. Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus–response compatibility. A model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97(2), 253–270.
go back to reference Lien, M. C., & Proctor, R. W. (2002). Stimulus–response compatibility and psychological refractory period effects: implications for response selection. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(2), 212–238.CrossRef Lien, M. C., & Proctor, R. W. (2002). Stimulus–response compatibility and psychological refractory period effects: implications for response selection. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(2), 212–238.CrossRef
go back to reference Logan, G. D., & Gordon, R. D. (2001). Executive control of visual attention in dual-task situations. Psychological Review, 108(2), 393–434.PubMedCrossRef Logan, G. D., & Gordon, R. D. (2001). Executive control of visual attention in dual-task situations. Psychological Review, 108(2), 393–434.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Logan, G. D., & Schulkind, M. D. (2000). Parallel memory retrieval in dual-task situations: I. Semantic memory. Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance, 26(3), 1072–1090.PubMedCrossRef Logan, G. D., & Schulkind, M. D. (2000). Parallel memory retrieval in dual-task situations: I. Semantic memory. Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance, 26(3), 1072–1090.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Meyer, D. E., & Kieras, D. E. (1997). A computational theory of executive cognitive processes and multiple-task performance: I. Basic mechanisms. Psychological Review, 104(1), 3–65.PubMedCrossRef Meyer, D. E., & Kieras, D. E. (1997). A computational theory of executive cognitive processes and multiple-task performance: I. Basic mechanisms. Psychological Review, 104(1), 3–65.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24, 167–202.PubMedCrossRef Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24, 167–202.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pashler, H. (1994). Dual-task interference in simple tasks: data and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 116(2), 220–244.PubMedCrossRef Pashler, H. (1994). Dual-task interference in simple tasks: data and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 116(2), 220–244.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ruthruff, E., Hazeltine, E., & Remington, R. W. (2006). What causes residual dual-task interference after practice? Psychological Research, 70(6), 494–503.PubMedCrossRef Ruthruff, E., Hazeltine, E., & Remington, R. W. (2006). What causes residual dual-task interference after practice? Psychological Research, 70(6), 494–503.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Schubert, T. (1999). Processing differences between simple and choice reactions affect bottleneck localization in overlapping tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 408–425.CrossRef Schubert, T. (1999). Processing differences between simple and choice reactions affect bottleneck localization in overlapping tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 408–425.CrossRef
go back to reference Schubert, T., Fischer, R., & Stelzel, C. (2008). Response activation in overlapping tasks and the response-selection bottleneck. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34(2), 376–397.PubMedCrossRef Schubert, T., Fischer, R., & Stelzel, C. (2008). Response activation in overlapping tasks and the response-selection bottleneck. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34(2), 376–397.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Schubert, T., & Szameitat, A. J. (2003). Functional neuroanatomy of interference in overlapping dual tasks: An fMRI study. Cognitive Brain Research, 17(3), 733–746.PubMedCrossRef Schubert, T., & Szameitat, A. J. (2003). Functional neuroanatomy of interference in overlapping dual tasks: An fMRI study. Cognitive Brain Research, 17(3), 733–746.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Schuch, S., & Koch, I. (2004). The costs of changing the representation of action: response repetition and response-response compatibility in dual tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30(3), 566–582.PubMedCrossRef Schuch, S., & Koch, I. (2004). The costs of changing the representation of action: response repetition and response-response compatibility in dual tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30(3), 566–582.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Schumacher, E. H., Seymour, T. L., Glass, J. M., Fencsik, D. E., Lauber, E. J., Kieras, D. E., et al. (2001). Virtually perfect time sharing in dual-task performance: uncorking the central cognitive bottleneck. Psychology Science, 12(2), 101–108.CrossRef Schumacher, E. H., Seymour, T. L., Glass, J. M., Fencsik, D. E., Lauber, E. J., Kieras, D. E., et al. (2001). Virtually perfect time sharing in dual-task performance: uncorking the central cognitive bottleneck. Psychology Science, 12(2), 101–108.CrossRef
go back to reference Schumacher, E. H., Schwarb, H., Lightman, E., & Hazeltine, E. (2011). Investigating the modality specificity of response selection using a temporal flanker task. Psychological Research (this issue). Schumacher, E. H., Schwarb, H., Lightman, E., & Hazeltine, E. (2011). Investigating the modality specificity of response selection using a temporal flanker task. Psychological Research (this issue).
go back to reference Sigman, M., & Dehaene, S. (2006). Dynamics of the central bottleneck: dual-task and task uncertainty. PLoS Biology, 4(7), e220.PubMedCrossRef Sigman, M., & Dehaene, S. (2006). Dynamics of the central bottleneck: dual-task and task uncertainty. PLoS Biology, 4(7), e220.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Stelzel, C., Schumacher, E. H., Schubert, T., & D’Esposito, M. (2006). The neural effect of stimulus–response modality compatibility on dual-task performance: an fMRI study. Psychological Research, 70(6), 514–525.PubMedCrossRef Stelzel, C., Schumacher, E. H., Schubert, T., & D’Esposito, M. (2006). The neural effect of stimulus–response modality compatibility on dual-task performance: an fMRI study. Psychological Research, 70(6), 514–525.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Stephan, D. N., & Koch, I. (2010). Central cross-talk in task switching: Evidence from manipulating input–output modality compatibility. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36(4), 1075–1081.PubMedCrossRef Stephan, D. N., & Koch, I. (2010). Central cross-talk in task switching: Evidence from manipulating input–output modality compatibility. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36(4), 1075–1081.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Stephan, D. N., & Koch, I. (2011). The role of input-output modality compatibility in task switching. Psychological Research (this issue). Stephan, D. N., & Koch, I. (2011). The role of input-output modality compatibility in task switching. Psychological Research (this issue).
go back to reference Virzi, R., & Egeth, H. (1985). Toward a translational model of Stroop interference. Memory & Cognition, 13(4), 304–319.CrossRef Virzi, R., & Egeth, H. (1985). Toward a translational model of Stroop interference. Memory & Cognition, 13(4), 304–319.CrossRef
go back to reference Welford, A. (1952). The ‘psychological refractory period’ and the timing of high-speed performance: a review and a theory. British Journal of Psychology, 43, 2–9. Welford, A. (1952). The ‘psychological refractory period’ and the timing of high-speed performance: a review and a theory. British Journal of Psychology, 43, 2–9.
go back to reference Wickens, C. D. (1980). The structure of attentional resources. In R. S. Nickerson (Ed.), Attention & performance VIII (pp. 239–257). Hillsdale: Erlbaum. Wickens, C. D. (1980). The structure of attentional resources. In R. S. Nickerson (Ed.), Attention & performance VIII (pp. 239–257). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Wickens, C., Sandry, D., & Vidulich, M. (1983). Compatibility and resource competition between modalities of input, central processing, and output. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 25(2), 227–248. Wickens, C., Sandry, D., & Vidulich, M. (1983). Compatibility and resource competition between modalities of input, central processing, and output. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 25(2), 227–248.
go back to reference Wylie, G., Sumowski, J. F., & Murray, M. (2011). Are there control processes, and (if so) can they be studied? Psychological Research (this issue). Wylie, G., Sumowski, J. F., & Murray, M. (2011). Are there control processes, and (if so) can they be studied? Psychological Research (this issue).
Metagegevens
Titel
Interference effects of stimulus–response modality pairings in dual tasks and their robustness
Auteurs
Christine Stelzel
Torsten Schubert
Publicatiedatum
01-11-2011
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 6/2011
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-011-0368-x

Andere artikelen Uitgave 6/2011

Psychological Research 6/2011 Naar de uitgave