Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 5/2012

01-09-2012 | Original Article

How sequence learning creates explicit knowledge: the role of response–stimulus interval

Auteur: Dennis Rünger

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 5/2012

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Destrebecqz and Cleeremans (Psychon Bull Rev 8:343–350, 2001; Attention and implicit learning. John Benjamins Publishing Company, Amsterdam, pp 181–213, 2003) reported that increasing the response–stimulus interval (RSI) during incidental sequence learning improved participants’ ability to discriminate old and new sequences in a recognition test. However, the original experimental design confounded RSI effects during training and test. I therefore repeated the experiment with an improved design in which RSI was varied systematically during the training phase and the recognition task. Participants learned a sequence of response locations either incidentally or intentionally. As a result, sequence recognition was not affected by the RSI manipulations in the group of incidental learners. With intentional learning instructions, recognition was unaffected by training RSI, but a long RSI in the test phase improved recognition performance over a short RSI. Response latencies while executing the test sequences indicated no effect of training RSI on sequence learning. However, sequence knowledge was expressed more readily when the RSI in the test phase matched the RSI in the training phase.
Voetnoten
1
In addition to a recognition test each participant performed two versions of a free-generation task which involved manual reproduction of the training sequence. Since we did not include a generation task in our own study, we will focus on recognition test results in the remainder of this report.
 
2
Welch’s (1947) t test was used for the 0-ms training RSI condition because equality of variance could not be assumed.
 
3
A six-point rating scale is commonly used in studies of sequence recognition (e.g., Destrebecqz & Cleeremans, 2001; Norman et al., 2006; Rünger, Nagy, & Frensch, 2009; Shanks & Johnstone, 1999; Shanks & Perruchet, 2002), but there is the valid concern that this scale does not meet the psychometric requirements for parametric analysis. I therefore analyzed recognition judgments with the sensitivity index d′ from signal-detection theory (Macmillan & Creelman, 1991) as the dependent variable. In an ANOVA with the between-subjects variables instruction, training RSI, and recognition RSI, the only significant effect was that of instruction, F(1, 181) = 6.07, MSE = 0.72. In particular, the Instruction × Recognition RSI interaction failed to reach significance, F(1, 181) = 1.91, MSE = 0.72, P = 0.17. Nevertheless, the pattern of mean d′ values was in close agreement with the pattern of mean recognition ratings (cf. Fig. 4). Pooled across recognition test RSIs, mean d′ was numerically lower with the 500-ms training RSI than with the 0-ms RSI for both incidental learners (0.37 vs. 0.53) and intentional learners (0.73 vs. 0.78). Thus, there was no indication that a longer training RSI improved sequence recognition. Pooled across training RSIs, incidental learners performed slightly worse with the 500-ms recognition RSI than with the 0-ms RSI (0.38 vs. 0.51). In contrast, sequence discrimination of intentional learners was better with the long recognition RSI (0.86 vs. 0.65).
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Ashby, F. G., & Ell, S. W. (2002). Single versus multiple systems of learning and memory. In H. Pashler & J. Wixted (Eds.), Stevens’ handbook of experimental psychology (3rd ed.), Vol. 4: Methodology in experimental psychology (pp. 655–691). Hoboken, NJ, US: John Wiley & Sons Inc. Ashby, F. G., & Ell, S. W. (2002). Single versus multiple systems of learning and memory. In H. Pashler & J. Wixted (Eds.), Stevens’ handbook of experimental psychology (3rd ed.), Vol. 4: Methodology in experimental psychology (pp. 655–691). Hoboken, NJ, US: John Wiley & Sons Inc.
go back to reference Atkinson, R. C., & Juola, J. F. (1974). Search and decision processes in recognition memory. In D. H. Krantz, R. C. Atkinson, R. D. Luce, & P. Suppes (Eds.), Contemporary development in mathematical psychology: Learning, memory, and thinking (pp. 243–293). New York: Freeman. Atkinson, R. C., & Juola, J. F. (1974). Search and decision processes in recognition memory. In D. H. Krantz, R. C. Atkinson, R. D. Luce, & P. Suppes (Eds.), Contemporary development in mathematical psychology: Learning, memory, and thinking (pp. 243–293). New York: Freeman.
go back to reference Buchner, A., Steffens, M. C., Erdfelder, E., & Rothkegel, R. (1997). A multinomial model to assess fluency and recollection in a sequence learning task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. A, Human Experimental Psychology, 50, 631–663. doi:10.1080/027249897392053.CrossRef Buchner, A., Steffens, M. C., Erdfelder, E., & Rothkegel, R. (1997). A multinomial model to assess fluency and recollection in a sequence learning task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. A, Human Experimental Psychology, 50, 631–663. doi:10.​1080/​027249897392053.CrossRef
go back to reference Cleeremans, A. (2006). Conscious and unconscious cognition: A graded, dynamic perspective. In Q. Jing, M. R. Rosenzweig, G. d’Ydewalle, H. Zhang, H.-C. Chen, & C. Zhang (Eds.), Progress in psychological science around the world: Vol. 1. Neural, cognitive, and developmental issues (pp. 401–418). Hove, U.K.: Psychology Press. Cleeremans, A. (2006). Conscious and unconscious cognition: A graded, dynamic perspective. In Q. Jing, M. R. Rosenzweig, G. d’Ydewalle, H. Zhang, H.-C. Chen, & C. Zhang (Eds.), Progress in psychological science around the world: Vol. 1. Neural, cognitive, and developmental issues (pp. 401–418). Hove, U.K.: Psychology Press.
go back to reference Cleeremans, A., & Jiménez, L. (2002). Implicit learning and consciousness: A graded, dynamic perspective. In R. M. French & A. Cleeremans (Eds.), Implicit learning and consciousness (pp. 1–40). Hove, UK: Psychology Press. Cleeremans, A., & Jiménez, L. (2002). Implicit learning and consciousness: A graded, dynamic perspective. In R. M. French & A. Cleeremans (Eds.), Implicit learning and consciousness (pp. 1–40). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
go back to reference Curran, T. (1997). Effects of aging on implicit sequence learning: Accounting for sequence structure and explicit knowledge. Psychological Research/Psychologische Forschung, 60, 24–41. doi:10.1007/BF00419678.CrossRef Curran, T. (1997). Effects of aging on implicit sequence learning: Accounting for sequence structure and explicit knowledge. Psychological Research/Psychologische Forschung, 60, 24–41. doi:10.​1007/​BF00419678.CrossRef
go back to reference Destrebecqz, A., & Cleeremans, A. (2001). Can sequence learning be implicit? New evidence with the process dissociation procedure. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 343–350. doi:10.3758/BF03196171.CrossRef Destrebecqz, A., & Cleeremans, A. (2001). Can sequence learning be implicit? New evidence with the process dissociation procedure. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 343–350. doi:10.​3758/​BF03196171.CrossRef
go back to reference Destrebecqz, A., & Cleeremans, A. (2003). Temporal effects in sequence learning. In L. Jiménez (Ed.), Attention and implicit learning (pp. 181–213). Amsterdam, Netherlands: John Benjamins Publishing Company. Destrebecqz, A., & Cleeremans, A. (2003). Temporal effects in sequence learning. In L. Jiménez (Ed.), Attention and implicit learning (pp. 181–213). Amsterdam, Netherlands: John Benjamins Publishing Company.
go back to reference Frensch, P. A., Haider, H., Rünger, D., Neugebauer, U., Voigt, S., & Werg, J. (2003). The route from implicit learning to verbal expression of what has been learned: Verbal report of incidentally experienced environmental regularity. In L. Jiménez (Ed.), Attention and implicit learning (pp. 335–366). Amsterdam, Netherlands: John Benjamins Publishing Company. Frensch, P. A., Haider, H., Rünger, D., Neugebauer, U., Voigt, S., & Werg, J. (2003). The route from implicit learning to verbal expression of what has been learned: Verbal report of incidentally experienced environmental regularity. In L. Jiménez (Ed.), Attention and implicit learning (pp. 335–366). Amsterdam, Netherlands: John Benjamins Publishing Company.
go back to reference Frensch, P. A., Lin, J., & Buchner, A. (1998). Learning versus behavioral expression of the learned: The effects of a secondary tone-counting task on implicit learning in the serial reaction task. Psychological Research/Psychologische Forschung, 61, 83–98. doi:10.1007/s004260050015.CrossRef Frensch, P. A., Lin, J., & Buchner, A. (1998). Learning versus behavioral expression of the learned: The effects of a secondary tone-counting task on implicit learning in the serial reaction task. Psychological Research/Psychologische Forschung, 61, 83–98. doi:10.​1007/​s004260050015.CrossRef
go back to reference Frensch, P. A., & Miner, C. S. (1994). Effects of presentation rate and individual differences in short-term memory capacity on an indirect measure of serial learning. Memory & Cognition, 22, 95–110. doi:10.3758/BF03202765.CrossRef Frensch, P. A., & Miner, C. S. (1994). Effects of presentation rate and individual differences in short-term memory capacity on an indirect measure of serial learning. Memory & Cognition, 22, 95–110. doi:10.​3758/​BF03202765.CrossRef
go back to reference Frensch, P. A., Wenke, D., & Rünger, D. (1999). A secondary tone-counting task suppresses expression of knowledge in the serial reaction task. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 260–274. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.25.1.260.CrossRef Frensch, P. A., Wenke, D., & Rünger, D. (1999). A secondary tone-counting task suppresses expression of knowledge in the serial reaction task. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 260–274. doi:10.​1037/​0278-7393.​25.​1.​260.CrossRef
go back to reference Haider, H., & Frensch, P. A. (2009). Conflicts between expected and actually performed behavior lead to verbal report of incidentally acquired sequential knowledge. Psychological Research/Psychologische Forschung, 73, 817–834. doi:10.1007/s00426-008-0199-6.CrossRef Haider, H., & Frensch, P. A. (2009). Conflicts between expected and actually performed behavior lead to verbal report of incidentally acquired sequential knowledge. Psychological Research/Psychologische Forschung, 73, 817–834. doi:10.​1007/​s00426-008-0199-6.CrossRef
go back to reference Macmillan, N. A., & Creelman, C. D. (1991). Detection Theory: A User’s Guide. New York: Cambridge University Press. Macmillan, N. A., & Creelman, C. D. (1991). Detection Theory: A User’s Guide. New York: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Miyawaki, K. (2006). The influence of the response-stimulus interval on implicit and explicit learning of stimulus sequence. Psychological Research/Psychologische Forschung, 70, 262–272. doi:10.1007/s00426-005-0216-y.CrossRef Miyawaki, K. (2006). The influence of the response-stimulus interval on implicit and explicit learning of stimulus sequence. Psychological Research/Psychologische Forschung, 70, 262–272. doi:10.​1007/​s00426-005-0216-y.CrossRef
go back to reference Reed, J., & Johnson, P. (1994). Assessing implicit learning with indirect tests: Determining what is learned about sequence structure. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 585–594. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.20.3.585.CrossRef Reed, J., & Johnson, P. (1994). Assessing implicit learning with indirect tests: Determining what is learned about sequence structure. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 585–594. doi:10.​1037/​0278-7393.​20.​3.​585.CrossRef
go back to reference Rose, M., Haider, H., Weiller, C., & Büchel, C. (2004). The relevance of the nature of learned associations for the differentiation of human memory systems. Learning & Memory, 11, 145–152. doi:10.1101/lm.67204. Rose, M., Haider, H., Weiller, C., & Büchel, C. (2004). The relevance of the nature of learned associations for the differentiation of human memory systems. Learning & Memory, 11, 145–152. doi:10.​1101/​lm.​67204.
go back to reference Rünger, D., & Frensch, P. A. (2008). How incidental sequence learning creates reportable knowledge: The role of unexpected events. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 1011–1026. doi:10.1037/a0012942.PubMedCrossRef Rünger, D., & Frensch, P. A. (2008). How incidental sequence learning creates reportable knowledge: The role of unexpected events. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 1011–1026. doi:10.​1037/​a0012942.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rünger, D., & Frensch, P. A. (2010). Defining consciousness in the context of incidental sequence learning: Theoretical considerations and empirical implications. Psychological Research/Psychologische Forschung, 74, 121–137. doi:10.1007/s00426-008-0225-8.CrossRef Rünger, D., & Frensch, P. A. (2010). Defining consciousness in the context of incidental sequence learning: Theoretical considerations and empirical implications. Psychological Research/Psychologische Forschung, 74, 121–137. doi:10.​1007/​s00426-008-0225-8.CrossRef
go back to reference Rünger, D., Nagy, G., & Frensch, P. A. (2009). Do recognition and priming index a unitary knowledge base? comment on Shanks et al. (2003). Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 572–585. doi:10.1037/a0014543.PubMedCrossRef Rünger, D., Nagy, G., & Frensch, P. A. (2009). Do recognition and priming index a unitary knowledge base? comment on Shanks et al. (2003). Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 572–585. doi:10.​1037/​a0014543.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Shanks, D. R. (2005). Implicit learning. In K. Lamberts & R. L. Goldstone (Eds.), Handbook of cognition (pp. 202–220). London: Sage. Shanks, D. R. (2005). Implicit learning. In K. Lamberts & R. L. Goldstone (Eds.), Handbook of cognition (pp. 202–220). London: Sage.
go back to reference Shanks, D. R., & Perruchet, P. (2002). Dissociation between priming and recognition in the expression of sequential knowledge. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 362–367.CrossRef Shanks, D. R., & Perruchet, P. (2002). Dissociation between priming and recognition in the expression of sequential knowledge. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 362–367.CrossRef
go back to reference Shanks, D. R., Wilkinson, L., & Channon, S. (2003). Relationship between priming and recognition in deterministic and probabilistic sequence learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 248–261. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.29.2.248.PubMedCrossRef Shanks, D. R., Wilkinson, L., & Channon, S. (2003). Relationship between priming and recognition in deterministic and probabilistic sequence learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 248–261. doi:10.​1037/​0278-7393.​29.​2.​248.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Thurstone, L. L., & Thurstone, T. G. (1941). Factorial Studies of Intelligence (Psychometric Monographs No. 2). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Thurstone, L. L., & Thurstone, T. G. (1941). Factorial Studies of Intelligence (Psychometric Monographs No. 2). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
go back to reference Willingham, D. B., Greenberg, A. R., & Thomas, R. C. (1997). Response-to-stimulus interval does not affect implicit motor sequence learning, but does affect performance. Memory & Cognition, 25, 534–542. doi:10.3758/BF03201128.CrossRef Willingham, D. B., Greenberg, A. R., & Thomas, R. C. (1997). Response-to-stimulus interval does not affect implicit motor sequence learning, but does affect performance. Memory & Cognition, 25, 534–542. doi:10.​3758/​BF03201128.CrossRef
go back to reference Willingham, D. B., Salidis, J., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2002). Direct comparison of neural systems mediating conscious and unconscious skill learning. Journal of Neurophysiology, 88, 1451–1460. Willingham, D. B., Salidis, J., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2002). Direct comparison of neural systems mediating conscious and unconscious skill learning. Journal of Neurophysiology, 88, 1451–1460.
go back to reference Woltz, D. J., Bell, B. G., Kyllonen, P. C., & Gardner, M. K. (1996). Memory for order of operations in the acquisition and transfer of sequential cognitive skills. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 438–457. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.22.2.438.CrossRef Woltz, D. J., Bell, B. G., Kyllonen, P. C., & Gardner, M. K. (1996). Memory for order of operations in the acquisition and transfer of sequential cognitive skills. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 438–457. doi:10.​1037/​0278-7393.​22.​2.​438.CrossRef
go back to reference Woltz, D. J., Gardner, M. K., & Bell, B. G. (2000). Negative transfer errors in sequential cognitive skills: Strong-but-wrong sequence application. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 601–625. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.26.3.601.PubMedCrossRef Woltz, D. J., Gardner, M. K., & Bell, B. G. (2000). Negative transfer errors in sequential cognitive skills: Strong-but-wrong sequence application. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 601–625. doi:10.​1037/​0278-7393.​26.​3.​601.PubMedCrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
How sequence learning creates explicit knowledge: the role of response–stimulus interval
Auteur
Dennis Rünger
Publicatiedatum
01-09-2012
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 5/2012
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-011-0367-y

Andere artikelen Uitgave 5/2012

Psychological Research 5/2012 Naar de uitgave