Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 3/2012

01-05-2012 | Original Article

Body movement enhances the extraction of temporal structures in auditory sequences

Auteurs: Yi-Huang Su, Ernst Pöppel

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 3/2012

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Auditory and motor systems interact in processing auditory rhythms. This study investigated the effect of intuitive body movement, such as head nodding or foot tapping, on listeners’ ability to entrain to the pulse of an auditory sequence. A pulse-finding task was employed using an isochronous sequence of tones in which tones were omitted at pseudorandom positions. Musicians and non-musicians identified their subjectively fitting pulse either using periodic body movement or through listening only. The identified pulse was measured subsequently by finger tapping. Movement appeared to assist pulse extraction especially for non-musicians. The chosen pulse tempi tended to be faster with movement. Additionally, movement led to higher synchronization stabilities of the produced pulse along the sequence, regardless of musical training. These findings demonstrated the facilitatory role of body movement in entraining to auditory rhythms and its interaction with musical training.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Voetnoten
1
The terms pulse and beat are often used interchangeably in a musical context. However, beat implies a defined metrical organization based on the alternating strong and weak accentuation (Cooper & Meyer, 1960), which involves the perceptual grouping of pulse, e.g. groups of two or four as in a duple meter, or groups of three as in a waltz meter. Pulse itself, on the other hand, is not confined by metrical specifications; it exists as long as the isochrony is felt by the listener, and is generalizable in processing rhythms across different cultures and musical genres. Therefore, we prefer to use the term pulse here.
 
2
A criterion of ITI stability for constituting a ‘pulse’ has not been established in the literature, as it would depend on the task condition and the given stimuli. A study on the perceptual threshold of pulse attribution (Madison & Merker, 2002) found an average 8.6% deviation of the inter-tone intervals in the sequence, beyond which the participants were unable to identify the pulse. Considering the higher difficulty in the present task as the tones in a sequence did not appear regularly, and that the pulse was measured by production, a criterion of 10% was used. This, together with the criterion on inter-pulse interval, appeared to reflect the interaction between musical training and movement well (Fig. 2).
 
3
For detailed classification criteria, see Figure S1 and the described procedure in the supplementary material.
 
4
Every participant was observed for around 20–30 min during the first experimental block, and also for a shorter while in the beginning of each successive block.
 
5
Often two kinds of movement were adopted together by the same participant. The frequency of each employed movement was as follows: head nodding (6), foot tapping (9), and arm swiveling (1).
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Arom, S. (1989). Time structure in the music of central Africa: periodicity, meter, rhythm and polyrhythmics. Leonardo, 22, 91–99.CrossRef Arom, S. (1989). Time structure in the music of central Africa: periodicity, meter, rhythm and polyrhythmics. Leonardo, 22, 91–99.CrossRef
go back to reference Bengtsson, S. L., Ullen, F., Ehrsson, H. H., Hashimoto, T., Kito, T., Naito, E., et al. (2009). Listening to rhythms activates motor and premotor cortices. Cortex, 45, 62–71.PubMedCrossRef Bengtsson, S. L., Ullen, F., Ehrsson, H. H., Hashimoto, T., Kito, T., Naito, E., et al. (2009). Listening to rhythms activates motor and premotor cortices. Cortex, 45, 62–71.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bolton, T. L. (1894). Rhythm. American Journal of Psychology, 6, 145–238.CrossRef Bolton, T. L. (1894). Rhythm. American Journal of Psychology, 6, 145–238.CrossRef
go back to reference Chen, J. L., Penhune, V. B., & Zatorre, R. J. (2008a). Moving on time: brain network for auditory–motor synchronization is modulated by rhythm complexity and musical training. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 226–239.PubMedCrossRef Chen, J. L., Penhune, V. B., & Zatorre, R. J. (2008a). Moving on time: brain network for auditory–motor synchronization is modulated by rhythm complexity and musical training. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 226–239.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Chen, J. L., Penhune, V. B., & Zatorre, R. J. (2008b). Listening to musical rhythms recruits motor regions of the brain. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 2844–2854.PubMedCrossRef Chen, J. L., Penhune, V. B., & Zatorre, R. J. (2008b). Listening to musical rhythms recruits motor regions of the brain. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 2844–2854.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Chen, J. L., Penhune, V. B., & Zatorre, R. J. (2009). The role of auditory and premotor cortex in sensorimotor transformations. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1169, 15–34.PubMedCrossRef Chen, J. L., Penhune, V. B., & Zatorre, R. J. (2009). The role of auditory and premotor cortex in sensorimotor transformations. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1169, 15–34.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cooper, G., & Meyer, A. B. (1960). The rhythmic structure of music. London: The University of Chicago Press. Cooper, G., & Meyer, A. B. (1960). The rhythmic structure of music. London: The University of Chicago Press.
go back to reference Drake, C., Jones, M. R., & Baruch, C. (2000). The development of rhythmic attending in auditory sequences: attunement, referent period, focal attending. Cognition, 77, 251–288.PubMedCrossRef Drake, C., Jones, M. R., & Baruch, C. (2000). The development of rhythmic attending in auditory sequences: attunement, referent period, focal attending. Cognition, 77, 251–288.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Drewing, K., & Aschersleben, G. (2003). Reduced timing variability during bimanual coupling: a role for sensory information. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 56, 329–350.CrossRef Drewing, K., & Aschersleben, G. (2003). Reduced timing variability during bimanual coupling: a role for sensory information. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 56, 329–350.CrossRef
go back to reference Franek, M., Mates, J., Radil, T., Beck, K., & Pöppel, E. (1994). Sensorimotor synchronization: motor responses to pseudoregular auditory patterns. Perception & Psychophysics, 55, 204–217.CrossRef Franek, M., Mates, J., Radil, T., Beck, K., & Pöppel, E. (1994). Sensorimotor synchronization: motor responses to pseudoregular auditory patterns. Perception & Psychophysics, 55, 204–217.CrossRef
go back to reference Fujioka, T., Trainor, L. J., Large, E. W., & Ross, B. (2009). Beta and gamma rhythms in human auditory cortex during musical beat processing. Annals of New York Academy of Science, 1169, 89–92.CrossRef Fujioka, T., Trainor, L. J., Large, E. W., & Ross, B. (2009). Beta and gamma rhythms in human auditory cortex during musical beat processing. Annals of New York Academy of Science, 1169, 89–92.CrossRef
go back to reference Grahn, J. A., & Brett, M. (2007). Rhythm and beat perception in motor areas of the brain. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 893–906.PubMedCrossRef Grahn, J. A., & Brett, M. (2007). Rhythm and beat perception in motor areas of the brain. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 893–906.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Grahn, J. A., & Brett, M. (2009). Impairment of beat-based rhythm discrimination in Parkinson’s disease. Cortex, 45, 54–61.PubMedCrossRef Grahn, J. A., & Brett, M. (2009). Impairment of beat-based rhythm discrimination in Parkinson’s disease. Cortex, 45, 54–61.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Grahn, J. A., & Rowe, J. B. (2009). Feeling the beat: premotor and striatal interactions in musicians and nonmusicians during beat perception. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 7540–7548.PubMedCrossRef Grahn, J. A., & Rowe, J. B. (2009). Feeling the beat: premotor and striatal interactions in musicians and nonmusicians during beat perception. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 7540–7548.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Helmuth, L. L., & Ivry, R. B. (1996). When two hands are better than one: reduced timing variability during bimanual movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 22, 278–293.PubMedCrossRef Helmuth, L. L., & Ivry, R. B. (1996). When two hands are better than one: reduced timing variability during bimanual movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 22, 278–293.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Humble, M. (2002). The development of rhythmic organization in Indian classical music. Unpublished dissertation, University of London. Humble, M. (2002). The development of rhythmic organization in Indian classical music. Unpublished dissertation, University of London.
go back to reference Iversen, J. R., Patel, A. D., & Ohgushi, K. (2008). Perception of rhythmic grouping depends on auditory experience. Journal of Acoustic Society America, 124(4), 2263–2271.CrossRef Iversen, J. R., Patel, A. D., & Ohgushi, K. (2008). Perception of rhythmic grouping depends on auditory experience. Journal of Acoustic Society America, 124(4), 2263–2271.CrossRef
go back to reference Kornysheva, K., von Cramon, D. Y., Jacobsen, T., & Schubotz, R. I. (2010). Tuning-into the beat: aesthetic appreciation of musical rhythms correlates with a premotor activity boost. Human Brain Mapping, 31, 48–64.PubMed Kornysheva, K., von Cramon, D. Y., Jacobsen, T., & Schubotz, R. I. (2010). Tuning-into the beat: aesthetic appreciation of musical rhythms correlates with a premotor activity boost. Human Brain Mapping, 31, 48–64.PubMed
go back to reference Large, E. W. (2008). Resonating to musical rhythm: theory and experiment. In S. Grondin (Ed.), The psychology of time (pp. 189–231). United Kingdom: Emerald. Large, E. W. (2008). Resonating to musical rhythm: theory and experiment. In S. Grondin (Ed.), The psychology of time (pp. 189–231). United Kingdom: Emerald.
go back to reference Large, E. W., & Snyder, J. S. (2009). Pulse and meter as neural resonance. Annals of New York Academy of Science, 1169, 46–57.CrossRef Large, E. W., & Snyder, J. S. (2009). Pulse and meter as neural resonance. Annals of New York Academy of Science, 1169, 46–57.CrossRef
go back to reference London, J. (2002). Cognitive constraints on metric systems: some observations and hypotheses. Music Perception, 19, 529–550.CrossRef London, J. (2002). Cognitive constraints on metric systems: some observations and hypotheses. Music Perception, 19, 529–550.CrossRef
go back to reference Macdougall, H. G., & Moore, S. T. (2005). Marching to the beat of the same drummer: the spontaneous tempo of human locomotion. Journal of Applied Physiology, 99, 1164–1173.PubMedCrossRef Macdougall, H. G., & Moore, S. T. (2005). Marching to the beat of the same drummer: the spontaneous tempo of human locomotion. Journal of Applied Physiology, 99, 1164–1173.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Madison, G., & Merker, B. (2002). On the limits of anisochrony in pulse attribution. Psychological Research, 66, 201–207.PubMedCrossRef Madison, G., & Merker, B. (2002). On the limits of anisochrony in pulse attribution. Psychological Research, 66, 201–207.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Merker, B., Madison, G., & Eckerdal, P. (2009). On the role and origin of isochrony in human rhythmic entrainment. Cortex, 45, 4–17.PubMedCrossRef Merker, B., Madison, G., & Eckerdal, P. (2009). On the role and origin of isochrony in human rhythmic entrainment. Cortex, 45, 4–17.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Moelants, D. (2002). Preferred tempo reconsidered. In C. Stevens, D. Burnham, G. McPherson, E. Schubert, & J. Renwick (Ed.), Proceedings of the 7th International Conference on Music Perception and Cognition (pp. 580–583). Sydney. Moelants, D. (2002). Preferred tempo reconsidered. In C. Stevens, D. Burnham, G. McPherson, E. Schubert, & J. Renwick (Ed.), Proceedings of the 7th International Conference on Music Perception and Cognition (pp. 580–583). Sydney.
go back to reference Patel, A., Iversen, J., Chen, Y., & Repp, B. H. (2005). The influence of metricality and modality on synchronization with a beat. Experimental Brain Research, 163, 226–238.CrossRef Patel, A., Iversen, J., Chen, Y., & Repp, B. H. (2005). The influence of metricality and modality on synchronization with a beat. Experimental Brain Research, 163, 226–238.CrossRef
go back to reference Pecenka, N., & Keller, P. (2009). Auditory pitch imagery and its relationship to musical synchronization. Annals of New York Academy of Science, 1169, 282–286.CrossRef Pecenka, N., & Keller, P. (2009). Auditory pitch imagery and its relationship to musical synchronization. Annals of New York Academy of Science, 1169, 282–286.CrossRef
go back to reference Phillips-Silver, J., & Trainor, L. J. (2005). Feeling the beat: movement influences infant rhythm perception. Science, 308, 1430.PubMedCrossRef Phillips-Silver, J., & Trainor, L. J. (2005). Feeling the beat: movement influences infant rhythm perception. Science, 308, 1430.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Phillips-Silver, J., & Trainor, L. J. (2007). Hearing what the body feels: auditory encoding of rhythmic movement. Cognition, 105, 533–546.PubMedCrossRef Phillips-Silver, J., & Trainor, L. J. (2007). Hearing what the body feels: auditory encoding of rhythmic movement. Cognition, 105, 533–546.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9, 129–154.CrossRef Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9, 129–154.CrossRef
go back to reference Repp, B. H., & Doggett, R. (2006). Tapping to a very slow beat: a comparison of musicians and nonmusicians. Music Perception, 24, 367–376.CrossRef Repp, B. H., & Doggett, R. (2006). Tapping to a very slow beat: a comparison of musicians and nonmusicians. Music Perception, 24, 367–376.CrossRef
go back to reference Repp, B. H., Iversen, J. R., & Patel, A. D. (2008). Tracking an imposed beat within a metrical grid. Music Perception, 26, 1–18.CrossRef Repp, B. H., Iversen, J. R., & Patel, A. D. (2008). Tracking an imposed beat within a metrical grid. Music Perception, 26, 1–18.CrossRef
go back to reference Salenius, S., & Hari, R. (2003). Synchronous cortical oscillatory activity during motor action. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 13, 678–684.PubMedCrossRef Salenius, S., & Hari, R. (2003). Synchronous cortical oscillatory activity during motor action. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 13, 678–684.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Soley, G., & Hannon, E. E. (2010). Infants prefer the musical meter of their own culture: a cross-cultural comparison. Developmental Psychology, 46, 286–292.PubMedCrossRef Soley, G., & Hannon, E. E. (2010). Infants prefer the musical meter of their own culture: a cross-cultural comparison. Developmental Psychology, 46, 286–292.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Todd, N. P. M. (1999). Motion in music: a neurobiological perspective. Music Perception, 17, 115–126. Todd, N. P. M. (1999). Motion in music: a neurobiological perspective. Music Perception, 17, 115–126.
go back to reference Todd, N. P. M., Lee, C. S., & O’Boyle, D. J. (2002). A sensorimotor theory of temporal tracking and beat induction. Psychological Research, 66, 26–39.PubMedCrossRef Todd, N. P. M., Lee, C. S., & O’Boyle, D. J. (2002). A sensorimotor theory of temporal tracking and beat induction. Psychological Research, 66, 26–39.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Trehub, S. E., & Hannon, E. E. (2009). Conventional rhythms enhance infants’ and adults’ perception of musical patterns. Cortex, 45, 110–118.PubMedCrossRef Trehub, S. E., & Hannon, E. E. (2009). Conventional rhythms enhance infants’ and adults’ perception of musical patterns. Cortex, 45, 110–118.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wing, A. M., & Kristofferson, A. B. (1973). Response delays and the timing of discrete motor responses. Perception & Psychophysics, 14, 5–12.CrossRef Wing, A. M., & Kristofferson, A. B. (1973). Response delays and the timing of discrete motor responses. Perception & Psychophysics, 14, 5–12.CrossRef
go back to reference Winkler, I., Háden, G. P., Ladinig, O., Sziller, I., & Honing, H. (2009). Newborn infants detect the beat in music. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 106, 2468–2471.CrossRef Winkler, I., Háden, G. P., Ladinig, O., Sziller, I., & Honing, H. (2009). Newborn infants detect the beat in music. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 106, 2468–2471.CrossRef
go back to reference Wöllner, C., & Cañal-Bruland, R. (2010). Keeping an eye on the violinist: motor experts show superior timing consistency in a visual perception task. Psychological Research, 74, 579–585.PubMedCrossRef Wöllner, C., & Cañal-Bruland, R. (2010). Keeping an eye on the violinist: motor experts show superior timing consistency in a visual perception task. Psychological Research, 74, 579–585.PubMedCrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Body movement enhances the extraction of temporal structures in auditory sequences
Auteurs
Yi-Huang Su
Ernst Pöppel
Publicatiedatum
01-05-2012
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 3/2012
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-011-0346-3

Andere artikelen Uitgave 3/2012

Psychological Research 3/2012 Naar de uitgave