Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 5/2011

01-09-2011 | Original Article

Perceiving performer identity and intended expression intensity in point-light displays of dance

Auteurs: Vassilis Sevdalis, Peter E. Keller

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 5/2011

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

This study investigated agent and expression intensity recognition in point-light displays depicting dancing performances. In a first session, participants danced with two different expression intensities to music, solo or in dyads. In a subsequent session, they watched point-light displays of 1–5-s duration, depicting their own, their partner’s or another participant’s recorded actions, and were asked to identify the agent (self vs. partner vs. stranger) and the intended expression intensity (expressive vs. inexpressive) of the performer. The results indicate that performer identity and expression intensity could be discerned reliably from displays as short as 1 s. The accuracy in judgment increased with exposure duration and, for performer identification, with higher expression intensity. Judgment accuracy in agent and expression intensity recognition tasks correlated with self-report empathy indices. Accuracy correlated also with confidence in judgment, but only in the intensity recognition task. The results are discussed in relation to perceptual and neural mechanisms underlying action and intention recognition.
Voetnoten
1
Kinematic analyses showed that the total distance travelled in space for each of the 13 motion capture markers attached to the dancers’ bodies (see below) was significantly higher in the expressive than in the inexpressive displays: ts > 16.95, ps < 0.001. This confirmed that the experimental manipulation had an effect on action execution, and that the participants were following the instructions.
 
2
In a preliminary analysis, observation condition (visual vs. audiovisual, i.e., with the original music being played along with the point-light movies) was included as an independent variable in the analyses, in order to compare the results with findings in our previous studies (Sevdalis & Keller, 2009, 2010). These analyses did not yield any differences between the observation conditions, thus, replicating our previous results. Henceforth, we dropped the observation condition variable from the analyses. Furthermore, we collapsed across musical piece, tempo, context, and selection, because these variables were not directly relevant to our research aims.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Abernethy, B., & Zawi, K. (2007). Pickup of essential kinematics underpins expert perception of movement patterns. Journal of Motor Behavior, 39, 353–367.PubMedCrossRef Abernethy, B., & Zawi, K. (2007). Pickup of essential kinematics underpins expert perception of movement patterns. Journal of Motor Behavior, 39, 353–367.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Atkinson, A. P., Dittrich, W. H., Gemmell, A. J., & Young, A. W. (2004). Emotion perception from dynamic and static body expressions in point-light and full-light displays. Perception, 33, 717–746.PubMedCrossRef Atkinson, A. P., Dittrich, W. H., Gemmell, A. J., & Young, A. W. (2004). Emotion perception from dynamic and static body expressions in point-light and full-light displays. Perception, 33, 717–746.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bastiaansen, J. A. C. J., Thioux, M., & Keysers, C. (2009). Evidence for mirror systems in emotions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society: B-Biological Sciences, 364, 2391–2404.CrossRef Bastiaansen, J. A. C. J., Thioux, M., & Keysers, C. (2009). Evidence for mirror systems in emotions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society: B-Biological Sciences, 364, 2391–2404.CrossRef
go back to reference Bate, S., Parris, B. A., Haslam, C., & Kay, J. M. (2010). Socio-emotional functioning and face recognition ability in the normal population. Personality and Individual Differences, 48, 239–242.CrossRef Bate, S., Parris, B. A., Haslam, C., & Kay, J. M. (2010). Socio-emotional functioning and face recognition ability in the normal population. Personality and Individual Differences, 48, 239–242.CrossRef
go back to reference Beintema, J. A., Georg, K., & Lappe, M. (2006). Perception of biological motion from limited-lifetime stimuli. Perception and Psychophysics, 68, 613–624.PubMedCrossRef Beintema, J. A., Georg, K., & Lappe, M. (2006). Perception of biological motion from limited-lifetime stimuli. Perception and Psychophysics, 68, 613–624.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Beintema, J. A., & Lappe, M. (2002). Perception of biological motion without local image motion. Proceeding of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 99, 5661–5663.CrossRef Beintema, J. A., & Lappe, M. (2002). Perception of biological motion without local image motion. Proceeding of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 99, 5661–5663.CrossRef
go back to reference Broughton, M., & Stevens, C. (2009). Music, movement and marimba: An investigation of the role of movement and gesture in communicating musical expression to an audience. Psychology of Music, 37, 137–153. Broughton, M., & Stevens, C. (2009). Music, movement and marimba: An investigation of the role of movement and gesture in communicating musical expression to an audience. Psychology of Music, 37, 137–153.
go back to reference Chang, D.H.F., & Troje, N.F. (2008). Perception of animacy and direction from local biological motion signals. Journal of Vision, 9, 3. Chang, D.H.F., & Troje, N.F. (2008). Perception of animacy and direction from local biological motion signals. Journal of Vision, 9, 3.
go back to reference Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 893–910.PubMedCrossRef Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 893–910.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Chouchourelou, A., Matsuka, T., Harber, K., & Shiffrar, M. (2006). The visual analysis of emotional actions. Social Neuroscience, 1, 63–74.PubMedCrossRef Chouchourelou, A., Matsuka, T., Harber, K., & Shiffrar, M. (2006). The visual analysis of emotional actions. Social Neuroscience, 1, 63–74.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Clarke, T. J., Bradshaw, M. F., Field, D. T., Hampson, S. E., & Rose, D. (2005). The perception of emotion from body movement in point-light displays of interpersonal dialogue. Perception, 34, 1171–1180.PubMedCrossRef Clarke, T. J., Bradshaw, M. F., Field, D. T., Hampson, S. E., & Rose, D. (2005). The perception of emotion from body movement in point-light displays of interpersonal dialogue. Perception, 34, 1171–1180.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cross, E. S., Kraemer, D. J. M., Hamilton, A. F. D. C., Kelley, W. M., & Grafton, S. T. (2009). Sensitivity of the action observation network to physical and observational learning. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 315–326.PubMedCrossRef Cross, E. S., Kraemer, D. J. M., Hamilton, A. F. D. C., Kelley, W. M., & Grafton, S. T. (2009). Sensitivity of the action observation network to physical and observational learning. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 315–326.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cutting, J. E., & Kozlowski, L. T. (1977). Recognizing friends by their walk-gait perception without familiarity cues. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 9, 353–356. Cutting, J. E., & Kozlowski, L. T. (1977). Recognizing friends by their walk-gait perception without familiarity cues. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 9, 353–356.
go back to reference Dahl, S., & Friberg, A. (2007). Visual perception of expressiveness in musicians’ body movements. Music Perception, 24, 433–454.CrossRef Dahl, S., & Friberg, A. (2007). Visual perception of expressiveness in musicians’ body movements. Music Perception, 24, 433–454.CrossRef
go back to reference Daprati, E., Wriessnegger, S., & Lacquaniti, F. (2007). Kinematic cues and recognition of self-generated actions. Experimental Brain Research, 177, 31–44.CrossRef Daprati, E., Wriessnegger, S., & Lacquaniti, F. (2007). Kinematic cues and recognition of self-generated actions. Experimental Brain Research, 177, 31–44.CrossRef
go back to reference Davidson, J. W. (1993). Visual perception of performance manner in the movements of solo musicians. Psychology of Music, 21, 103–113.CrossRef Davidson, J. W. (1993). Visual perception of performance manner in the movements of solo musicians. Psychology of Music, 21, 103–113.CrossRef
go back to reference Davis, M. H. (1980). A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. JSAS Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology, 10, 85. Davis, M. H. (1980). A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. JSAS Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology, 10, 85.
go back to reference Davis, M. H., Mitchell, K. V., Hall, J. A., Lothert, J., Snapp, T., & Meyer, N. (1999). Empathy, expectations, and situational preferences: Personality influences on the decision to participate in volunteer helping behaviors. Journal of Personality, 67, 469–503.PubMedCrossRef Davis, M. H., Mitchell, K. V., Hall, J. A., Lothert, J., Snapp, T., & Meyer, N. (1999). Empathy, expectations, and situational preferences: Personality influences on the decision to participate in volunteer helping behaviors. Journal of Personality, 67, 469–503.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference de Gelder, B. (2009). Why bodies? Twelve reasons for including bodily expressions in affective neuroscience. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364, 3475–3484.CrossRef de Gelder, B. (2009). Why bodies? Twelve reasons for including bodily expressions in affective neuroscience. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364, 3475–3484.CrossRef
go back to reference de Vignemont, F., & Singer, T. (2006). The empathic brain: How, when and why? Trends in Cognitive Science, 10, 435–441.CrossRef de Vignemont, F., & Singer, T. (2006). The empathic brain: How, when and why? Trends in Cognitive Science, 10, 435–441.CrossRef
go back to reference Decety, J., & Chaminade, T. (2003). Neural correlates of feeling sympathy. Neuropsychologia, 41, 127–138.PubMedCrossRef Decety, J., & Chaminade, T. (2003). Neural correlates of feeling sympathy. Neuropsychologia, 41, 127–138.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Dittrich, W. H., Troscianko, T., Lea, S. E. G., & Morgan, D. (1996). Perception of emotion from dynamic point-light displays represented in dance. Perception, 25, 727–738.PubMedCrossRef Dittrich, W. H., Troscianko, T., Lea, S. E. G., & Morgan, D. (1996). Perception of emotion from dynamic point-light displays represented in dance. Perception, 25, 727–738.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Eisenberg, N., & Miller, P. A. (1987). The relation of empathy to prosocial and related behaviors. Psychological Bulletin, 94, 100–131.CrossRef Eisenberg, N., & Miller, P. A. (1987). The relation of empathy to prosocial and related behaviors. Psychological Bulletin, 94, 100–131.CrossRef
go back to reference Fabbri-Destro, M., & Rizzolatti, G. (2008). Mirror neurons and mirror systems in monkeys and humans. Physiology, 23, 171–179.PubMedCrossRef Fabbri-Destro, M., & Rizzolatti, G. (2008). Mirror neurons and mirror systems in monkeys and humans. Physiology, 23, 171–179.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Flach, R., Knoblich, G., & Prinz, W. (2004). Recognizing one’s own clapping: The role of temporal cues. Psychological Research, 69, 147–156.PubMedCrossRef Flach, R., Knoblich, G., & Prinz, W. (2004). Recognizing one’s own clapping: The role of temporal cues. Psychological Research, 69, 147–156.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fogassi, L., Ferrari, P. F., Chersi, F., Gesierich, B., Rozzi, S., & Rizzolatti, G. (2005). Parietal lobe: From action organization to intention understanding. Science, 308, 662–667.PubMedCrossRef Fogassi, L., Ferrari, P. F., Chersi, F., Gesierich, B., Rozzi, S., & Rizzolatti, G. (2005). Parietal lobe: From action organization to intention understanding. Science, 308, 662–667.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gallese, V. (2006). Intentional attunement: A neurophysiological perspective on social cognition and its disruption in autism. Brain Research, 1079, 15–24.PubMedCrossRef Gallese, V. (2006). Intentional attunement: A neurophysiological perspective on social cognition and its disruption in autism. Brain Research, 1079, 15–24.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gallese, V. (2009). Motor abstraction: A neuroscientific account of how action goals and intentions are mapped and understood. Psychological Research, 73, 486–498.PubMedCrossRef Gallese, V. (2009). Motor abstraction: A neuroscientific account of how action goals and intentions are mapped and understood. Psychological Research, 73, 486–498.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gallese, V., Keysers, C., & Rizzolatti, G. (2004). A unifying view of the basis of social cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 396–403.PubMedCrossRef Gallese, V., Keysers, C., & Rizzolatti, G. (2004). A unifying view of the basis of social cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 396–403.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gazzola, V., Aziz-Zadeh, L., & Keysers, C. (2006). Empathy and the somatotopic auditory mirror system in humans. Current Biology, 16, 1824–1829.PubMedCrossRef Gazzola, V., Aziz-Zadeh, L., & Keysers, C. (2006). Empathy and the somatotopic auditory mirror system in humans. Current Biology, 16, 1824–1829.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Greitemeyer, T. (2009). Effects of songs with prosocial lyrics on prosocial thoughts, affect, and behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 186–190.CrossRef Greitemeyer, T. (2009). Effects of songs with prosocial lyrics on prosocial thoughts, affect, and behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 186–190.CrossRef
go back to reference Grèzes, J., Frith, C., & Passingham, R. E. (2004). Brain mechanisms for inferring deceit in the actions of others. Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 5500–5505.PubMedCrossRef Grèzes, J., Frith, C., & Passingham, R. E. (2004). Brain mechanisms for inferring deceit in the actions of others. Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 5500–5505.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Haggard, P., Clark, S., & Kalogeras, J. (2002). Voluntary action and conscious awareness. Nature Neuroscience, 5, 382–385.PubMedCrossRef Haggard, P., Clark, S., & Kalogeras, J. (2002). Voluntary action and conscious awareness. Nature Neuroscience, 5, 382–385.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hill, H., & Pollick, F. E. (2000). Exaggerating temporal differences enhances recognition of individuals from point light displays. Psychological Science, 11, 223–228.PubMedCrossRef Hill, H., & Pollick, F. E. (2000). Exaggerating temporal differences enhances recognition of individuals from point light displays. Psychological Science, 11, 223–228.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding: A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 869–878. Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding: A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 869–878.
go back to reference Hove, M. J., & Risen, J. L. (2009). It’s all in the timing: Interpersonal synchrony increases affiliation. Social Cognition, 27, 949–960.CrossRef Hove, M. J., & Risen, J. L. (2009). It’s all in the timing: Interpersonal synchrony increases affiliation. Social Cognition, 27, 949–960.CrossRef
go back to reference Iacoboni, M., Molnar-Szakacs, I., Gallese, V., Buccino, G., Mazziotta, J. C., & Rizzolatti, G. (2005). Grasping the intentions of others with one’s own mirror neuron system. PLoS Biology, 3, 529–535.CrossRef Iacoboni, M., Molnar-Szakacs, I., Gallese, V., Buccino, G., Mazziotta, J. C., & Rizzolatti, G. (2005). Grasping the intentions of others with one’s own mirror neuron system. PLoS Biology, 3, 529–535.CrossRef
go back to reference Johansson, G. (1976). Spatio-temporal differentiation and integration in visual motion perception. An experimental and theoretical analysis of calculus-like functions in visual data processing. Psychological Research, 38, 379–393.PubMedCrossRef Johansson, G. (1976). Spatio-temporal differentiation and integration in visual motion perception. An experimental and theoretical analysis of calculus-like functions in visual data processing. Psychological Research, 38, 379–393.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kaplan, J. T., & Iacoboni, M. (2006). Getting a grip on other minds: Mirror neurons, intention understanding, and cognitive empathy. Social Neuroscience, 1, 175–183.PubMedCrossRef Kaplan, J. T., & Iacoboni, M. (2006). Getting a grip on other minds: Mirror neurons, intention understanding, and cognitive empathy. Social Neuroscience, 1, 175–183.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Keller, P. E. (2008). Joint action in music performance. In F. Morganti, A. Carassa, & G. Riva (Eds.), Enacting intersubjectivity: A cognitive and social perspective on the study of interaction (pp. 205–221). Amsterdam: IOS. Keller, P. E. (2008). Joint action in music performance. In F. Morganti, A. Carassa, & G. Riva (Eds.), Enacting intersubjectivity: A cognitive and social perspective on the study of interaction (pp. 205–221). Amsterdam: IOS.
go back to reference Keller, P. E., Knoblich, G., & Repp, B. H. (2007). Pianists duet better when they play with themselves: On the possible role of action simulation in synchronization. Consciousness and Cognition, 16, 102–111.PubMedCrossRef Keller, P. E., Knoblich, G., & Repp, B. H. (2007). Pianists duet better when they play with themselves: On the possible role of action simulation in synchronization. Consciousness and Cognition, 16, 102–111.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Knoblich, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). Recognition of self-generated actions from kinematic displays of drawing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 456–465.PubMedCrossRef Knoblich, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). Recognition of self-generated actions from kinematic displays of drawing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 456–465.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kohler, E., Keysers, C., Umiltà, M. A., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (2002). Hearing sounds, understanding actions: Action representation in mirror neurons. Science, 297, 846–848.PubMedCrossRef Kohler, E., Keysers, C., Umiltà, M. A., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (2002). Hearing sounds, understanding actions: Action representation in mirror neurons. Science, 297, 846–848.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kozlowski, L. T., & Cutting, J. E. (1977). Recognizing the sex of a walker from a dynamic point-light display. Perception & Psychophysics, 21, 575–580.CrossRef Kozlowski, L. T., & Cutting, J. E. (1977). Recognizing the sex of a walker from a dynamic point-light display. Perception & Psychophysics, 21, 575–580.CrossRef
go back to reference Kuhlmann, S., de Lussanet, M. H. E., & Lappe, M. (2009). Perception of limited-lifetime biological motion from different viewpoints. Journal of Vision, 9, 11.PubMedCrossRef Kuhlmann, S., de Lussanet, M. H. E., & Lappe, M. (2009). Perception of limited-lifetime biological motion from different viewpoints. Journal of Vision, 9, 11.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lamm, C., Batson, C. D., & Decety, J. (2007). The neural substrate of human empathy: Effects of perspective-taking and cognitive appraisal. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 42–58.PubMedCrossRef Lamm, C., Batson, C. D., & Decety, J. (2007). The neural substrate of human empathy: Effects of perspective-taking and cognitive appraisal. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 42–58.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Loula, F., Prasad, S., Harber, K., & Shiffrar, M. (2005). Recognizing people from their movement. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 210–220.PubMedCrossRef Loula, F., Prasad, S., Harber, K., & Shiffrar, M. (2005). Recognizing people from their movement. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 210–220.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Macmillan, N. A., & Creelman, C. D. (1991). Detection theory: A user’s guide. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Macmillan, N. A., & Creelman, C. D. (1991). Detection theory: A user’s guide. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Manera, V., Schouten, B., Becchio, C., Bara, B. G., & Verfaillie, K. (2010). Inferring intentions from biological motion: A stimulus set of point-light communicative interactions. Behavior Research Methods, 42, 168–178.PubMedCrossRef Manera, V., Schouten, B., Becchio, C., Bara, B. G., & Verfaillie, K. (2010). Inferring intentions from biological motion: A stimulus set of point-light communicative interactions. Behavior Research Methods, 42, 168–178.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference McAleer, P., & Pollick, F. E. (2008). Understanding intention from minimal displays of human activity. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 830–839.PubMedCrossRef McAleer, P., & Pollick, F. E. (2008). Understanding intention from minimal displays of human activity. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 830–839.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Miles, L. K., Griffiths, J. L., Richardson, M. J., & Macrae, C. N. (2010). Too late to coordinate: Contextual influences on behavioral synchrony. European Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 52–60.CrossRef Miles, L. K., Griffiths, J. L., Richardson, M. J., & Macrae, C. N. (2010). Too late to coordinate: Contextual influences on behavioral synchrony. European Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 52–60.CrossRef
go back to reference Pfeifer, J. H., Iacoboni, M., Mazziotta, J. C., & Dapretto, M. (2008). Mirroring others’ emotions relates to empathy and interpersonal competence in children. Neuroimage, 39, 2076–2085.PubMedCrossRef Pfeifer, J. H., Iacoboni, M., Mazziotta, J. C., & Dapretto, M. (2008). Mirroring others’ emotions relates to empathy and interpersonal competence in children. Neuroimage, 39, 2076–2085.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pollick, F. E., Hill, H., Calder, A., & Paterson, H. (2003). Recognising facial expression from spatially and temporally modified movements. Perception, 32, 813–826.PubMedCrossRef Pollick, F. E., Hill, H., Calder, A., & Paterson, H. (2003). Recognising facial expression from spatially and temporally modified movements. Perception, 32, 813–826.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pollick, F. E., Paterson, H. M., Bruderlin, A., & Sanford, A. J. (2001). Perceiving affect from arm movement. Cognition, 82, B51–B61.PubMedCrossRef Pollick, F. E., Paterson, H. M., Bruderlin, A., & Sanford, A. J. (2001). Perceiving affect from arm movement. Cognition, 82, B51–B61.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Preston, S. D., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2002). Empathy: Its ultimate and proximate bases. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 1–20.PubMed Preston, S. D., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2002). Empathy: Its ultimate and proximate bases. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 1–20.PubMed
go back to reference Repp, B. H., & Keller, P. E. (2010). Self versus other in piano performance: Detectability of timing perturbations depends on personal playing style. Experimental Brain Research, 202, 101–110.CrossRef Repp, B. H., & Keller, P. E. (2010). Self versus other in piano performance: Detectability of timing perturbations depends on personal playing style. Experimental Brain Research, 202, 101–110.CrossRef
go back to reference Repp, B. H., & Knoblich, G. (2004). Perceiving action identity—How pianists recognize their own performances. Psychological Science, 15, 604–609.PubMedCrossRef Repp, B. H., & Knoblich, G. (2004). Perceiving action identity—How pianists recognize their own performances. Psychological Science, 15, 604–609.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Repp, B. H., & Knoblich, G. (2007). Toward a psychophysics of agency: Detecting gain and loss of control over auditory action effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33, 469–482.PubMedCrossRef Repp, B. H., & Knoblich, G. (2007). Toward a psychophysics of agency: Detecting gain and loss of control over auditory action effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33, 469–482.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169–192.PubMedCrossRef Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169–192.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Roether, C. L., Omlor, L., Christensen, A., & Giese, M. (2009). Critical features for the perception of emotion from gait. Journal of Vision, 9, 1–32.PubMedCrossRef Roether, C. L., Omlor, L., Christensen, A., & Giese, M. (2009). Critical features for the perception of emotion from gait. Journal of Vision, 9, 1–32.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Runeson, S., & Frykholm, G. (1981). Visual perception of lifted weight. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7, 733–740.PubMedCrossRef Runeson, S., & Frykholm, G. (1981). Visual perception of lifted weight. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7, 733–740.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Runeson, S., & Frykholm, G. (1983). Kinematic specification of dynamics as an informational basis for-person-and-action perception: Expectation, gender recognition, and deceptive intention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 112, 585–615.CrossRef Runeson, S., & Frykholm, G. (1983). Kinematic specification of dynamics as an informational basis for-person-and-action perception: Expectation, gender recognition, and deceptive intention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 112, 585–615.CrossRef
go back to reference Schulte-Rüther, M., Markowitsch, H. J., Fink, G. R., & Piefke, M. (2007). Mirror neuron and theory of mind mechanisms involved in face-to-face interactions: An fMRI approach to empathy. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 1354–1372.PubMedCrossRef Schulte-Rüther, M., Markowitsch, H. J., Fink, G. R., & Piefke, M. (2007). Mirror neuron and theory of mind mechanisms involved in face-to-face interactions: An fMRI approach to empathy. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 1354–1372.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sebanz, N., & Knoblich, G. (2009). Prediction in Joint Action: What, when, and where. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1, 353–367.CrossRef Sebanz, N., & Knoblich, G. (2009). Prediction in Joint Action: What, when, and where. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1, 353–367.CrossRef
go back to reference Sebanz, N., Knoblich, G., & Prinz, W. (2005). How two share a task: Corepresenting stimulus–response mappings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 1234–1246.PubMedCrossRef Sebanz, N., Knoblich, G., & Prinz, W. (2005). How two share a task: Corepresenting stimulus–response mappings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 1234–1246.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sebanz, N., & Shiffrar, M. (2009). Detecting deception in a bluffing body: The role of expertise. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 170–175.CrossRef Sebanz, N., & Shiffrar, M. (2009). Detecting deception in a bluffing body: The role of expertise. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 170–175.CrossRef
go back to reference Sevdalis, V., & Keller, P. E. (2009). Self-recognition in the perception of actions performed in synchrony with music. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1169, 499–502.PubMedCrossRef Sevdalis, V., & Keller, P. E. (2009). Self-recognition in the perception of actions performed in synchrony with music. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1169, 499–502.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sevdalis, V., & Keller, P. E. (2010). Cues for self-recognition in point-light displays of actions performed in synchrony with music. Consciousness and Cognition, 19(2), 617–626.PubMedCrossRef Sevdalis, V., & Keller, P. E. (2010). Cues for self-recognition in point-light displays of actions performed in synchrony with music. Consciousness and Cognition, 19(2), 617–626.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Singer, T., & Lamm, C. (2009). The social neuroscience of empathy. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1156, 81–96.PubMedCrossRef Singer, T., & Lamm, C. (2009). The social neuroscience of empathy. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1156, 81–96.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Singer, T., Seymour, B., O’Doherty, J. P., Stephan, K. E., Dolan, R. J., & Frith, C. (2004). Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory component of pain. Science, 303, 1157–1162.PubMedCrossRef Singer, T., Seymour, B., O’Doherty, J. P., Stephan, K. E., Dolan, R. J., & Frith, C. (2004). Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory component of pain. Science, 303, 1157–1162.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Synofzik, M., Vosgerau, G., & Newen, A. (2008). I move, therefore I am: A new theoretical framework to investigate agency and ownership. Consciousness and Cognition, 17(2), 411–424.PubMedCrossRef Synofzik, M., Vosgerau, G., & Newen, A. (2008). I move, therefore I am: A new theoretical framework to investigate agency and ownership. Consciousness and Cognition, 17(2), 411–424.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Thornton, I. M., Pinto, J., & Shiffrar, M. (1998). The visual perception of human locomotion. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 15, 535–552.CrossRef Thornton, I. M., Pinto, J., & Shiffrar, M. (1998). The visual perception of human locomotion. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 15, 535–552.CrossRef
go back to reference Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 675–691.PubMed Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 675–691.PubMed
go back to reference Umiltà, M. A., Kohler, E., Gallese, V., Fogassi, L., Fadiga, L., Keysers, C., et al. (2001). ‘I know what you are doing’: A neurophysiological study. Neuron, 32, 91–101. Umiltà, M. A., Kohler, E., Gallese, V., Fogassi, L., Fadiga, L., Keysers, C., et al. (2001). ‘I know what you are doing’: A neurophysiological study. Neuron, 32, 91–101.
go back to reference Zaki, J., Bolger, N., & Ochsner, K. N. (2008). It takes two: The interpersonal basis of empathic accuracy. Psychological Science, 19, 399–404.PubMedCrossRef Zaki, J., Bolger, N., & Ochsner, K. N. (2008). It takes two: The interpersonal basis of empathic accuracy. Psychological Science, 19, 399–404.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Zaki, J., Bolger, N., & Ochsner, K. N. (2009). Unpacking the informational bases of empathic accuracy. Emotion, 9, 478–487.PubMedCrossRef Zaki, J., Bolger, N., & Ochsner, K. N. (2009). Unpacking the informational bases of empathic accuracy. Emotion, 9, 478–487.PubMedCrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Perceiving performer identity and intended expression intensity in point-light displays of dance
Auteurs
Vassilis Sevdalis
Peter E. Keller
Publicatiedatum
01-09-2011
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 5/2011
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-010-0312-5

Andere artikelen Uitgave 5/2011

Psychological Research 5/2011 Naar de uitgave