Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 2/2009

01-03-2009 | Original Article

Microsaccadic modulation of response times in spatial attention tasks

Auteurs: Reinhold Kliegl, Martin Rolfs, Jochen Laubrock, Ralf Engbert

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 2/2009

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Covert shifts of attention are usually reflected in RT differences between responses to valid and invalid cues in the Posner spatial attention task. Such inferences about covert shifts of attention do not control for microsaccades in the cue-target interval. We analyzed the effects of microsaccade orientation on RTs in four conditions, crossing peripheral visual and auditory cues with peripheral visual and auditory discrimination targets. Reaction time was generally faster on trials without microsaccades in the cue-target interval. If microsaccades occurred, the target-location congruency of the last microsaccade in the cue-target interval interacted in a complex way with cue validity. For valid visual cues, irrespective of whether the discrimination target was visual or auditory, target-congruent microsaccades delayed RT. For invalid cues, target-incongruent microsaccades facilitated RTs for visual target discrimination but delayed RT for auditory target discrimination. No reliable effects on RT were associated with auditory cues or with the first microsaccade in the cue-target interval. We discuss theoretical implications on the relation about spatial attention and oculomotor processes.
Literatuur
go back to reference Baayen, R. H. (2008). Practical data analysis for the language sciences with R. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Baayen, R. H. (2008). Practical data analysis for the language sciences with R. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Barlow, H. B. (1952). Eye movements during fixation. Journal of Physiology, 116, 290–306.PubMed Barlow, H. B. (1952). Eye movements during fixation. Journal of Physiology, 116, 290–306.PubMed
go back to reference Bates, D. (2008). lme4: Linear mixed-effect models using S4 classes. R package version 0.999375–1 [Software]. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Bates, D. (2008). lme4: Linear mixed-effect models using S4 classes. R package version 0.999375–1 [Software]. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.
go back to reference Betta, E., Galfano, G., & Turatto, M. (2007). Microsaccadic response during inhibition of return in a target-target paradigm. Vision Research, 47, 428–436.PubMedCrossRef Betta, E., Galfano, G., & Turatto, M. (2007). Microsaccadic response during inhibition of return in a target-target paradigm. Vision Research, 47, 428–436.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Betta, E., & Turatto, M. (2006). Are you ready? I can tell by looking at your microsaccades. Neuroreport, 17, 1001–1004.PubMedCrossRef Betta, E., & Turatto, M. (2006). Are you ready? I can tell by looking at your microsaccades. Neuroreport, 17, 1001–1004.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Clowes, M. B. (1962). A note on colour discrimination under conditions of retinal image constraint. Optica Acta, 9, 65–68. Clowes, M. B. (1962). A note on colour discrimination under conditions of retinal image constraint. Optica Acta, 9, 65–68.
go back to reference Deubel, H., & Elsner, T. (1986). Threshold perception and saccadic eye movements. Biological Cybernetics, 54, 351–358.PubMedCrossRef Deubel, H., & Elsner, T. (1986). Threshold perception and saccadic eye movements. Biological Cybernetics, 54, 351–358.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ditchburn, R. W. (1955). Eye-movements in relation to retinal action. Optica Acta, 1, 171–176. Ditchburn, R. W. (1955). Eye-movements in relation to retinal action. Optica Acta, 1, 171–176.
go back to reference Ditchburn, R. W., & Ginsborg, B. L. (1953). Involuntary eye movements during fixation. Journal of Physiology, 119, 1–17.PubMed Ditchburn, R. W., & Ginsborg, B. L. (1953). Involuntary eye movements during fixation. Journal of Physiology, 119, 1–17.PubMed
go back to reference Donner, K., & Hemilä, S. (2007). Modelling the effect of microsaccades on retinal responses to stationary contrast patterns. Vision Research, 47, 1166–1177.PubMedCrossRef Donner, K., & Hemilä, S. (2007). Modelling the effect of microsaccades on retinal responses to stationary contrast patterns. Vision Research, 47, 1166–1177.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Elsner, T., & Deubel, H. (1986). The effect of saccades on threshold perception—A model study. Biological Cybernetics, 54, 359–366.PubMedCrossRef Elsner, T., & Deubel, H. (1986). The effect of saccades on threshold perception—A model study. Biological Cybernetics, 54, 359–366.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Engbert, R. (2006). Microsaccades: A microcosm for research on oculomotor control, attention, and visual perception. Progress in Brain Research, 154, 177–192.PubMedCrossRef Engbert, R. (2006). Microsaccades: A microcosm for research on oculomotor control, attention, and visual perception. Progress in Brain Research, 154, 177–192.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Engbert, R., & Kliegl, R. (2003). Microsaccades uncover the orientation of covert attention. Vision Research, 43, 1035–1045.PubMedCrossRef Engbert, R., & Kliegl, R. (2003). Microsaccades uncover the orientation of covert attention. Vision Research, 43, 1035–1045.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Engbert, R., & Mergenthaler, K. (2006). Microsaccades are triggered by low retinal image slip. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103, 7192–7197.CrossRef Engbert, R., & Mergenthaler, K. (2006). Microsaccades are triggered by low retinal image slip. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103, 7192–7197.CrossRef
go back to reference Galfano, G., Betta, E., & Turatto, M. (2004). Inhibition of return in microsaccades. Experimental Brain Research, 159, 400–404.CrossRef Galfano, G., Betta, E., & Turatto, M. (2004). Inhibition of return in microsaccades. Experimental Brain Research, 159, 400–404.CrossRef
go back to reference Gerrits, H. J. M., & Vendrik, A. J. H. (1974). The influence of stimulus movements on perception in parafoveal stabilized vision. Vision Research, 14, 175–180.PubMedCrossRef Gerrits, H. J. M., & Vendrik, A. J. H. (1974). The influence of stimulus movements on perception in parafoveal stabilized vision. Vision Research, 14, 175–180.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hafed, Z. M., & Clark, J. J. (2002). Microsaccades as an overt measure of covert attention shifts. Vision Research, 42, 2533–2545.PubMedCrossRef Hafed, Z. M., & Clark, J. J. (2002). Microsaccades as an overt measure of covert attention shifts. Vision Research, 42, 2533–2545.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Horowitz, T. S., Fine, E. M., Fencsik, D. E., Yurgenson, S., & Wolfe, J. M. (2007a). Fixational eye movements are not an index of covert attention. Psychological Science, 18, 356–363.PubMedCrossRef Horowitz, T. S., Fine, E. M., Fencsik, D. E., Yurgenson, S., & Wolfe, J. M. (2007a). Fixational eye movements are not an index of covert attention. Psychological Science, 18, 356–363.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Horowitz, T. S., Fencsik, D. E., Fine, E. M., Yurgenson, S., & Wolfe, J. M. (2007b). Microsaccades and attention: Does a weak correlation make an index? Reply to Laubrock, Engbert, Rolfs, & Kliegl (2007). Psychological Science, 18, 367–368.CrossRef Horowitz, T. S., Fencsik, D. E., Fine, E. M., Yurgenson, S., & Wolfe, J. M. (2007b). Microsaccades and attention: Does a weak correlation make an index? Reply to Laubrock, Engbert, Rolfs, & Kliegl (2007). Psychological Science, 18, 367–368.CrossRef
go back to reference Kliegl, R. (2007). Towards a perceptual-span theory of distributed processing in reading: A reply to Rayner, Pollatsek, Drieghe, Slattery, & Reichle (2007). Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 138, 530–537.CrossRef Kliegl, R. (2007). Towards a perceptual-span theory of distributed processing in reading: A reply to Rayner, Pollatsek, Drieghe, Slattery, & Reichle (2007). Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 138, 530–537.CrossRef
go back to reference Kliegl, R., Risse, S., & Laubrock, J. (2007). Preview benefit and parafoveal-on-foveal effects from word n + 2. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33, 1250–1255.PubMedCrossRef Kliegl, R., Risse, S., & Laubrock, J. (2007). Preview benefit and parafoveal-on-foveal effects from word n + 2. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33, 1250–1255.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Krummenacher, J., Müller, H.J., & Geyer, T. (2008). RT performance in visual search is affected by dimension- and space-based intertribal contingencies. Psychological Research. Krummenacher, J., Müller, H.J., & Geyer, T. (2008). RT performance in visual search is affected by dimension- and space-based intertribal contingencies. Psychological Research.
go back to reference Laubrock, J., Engbert, R., & Kliegl, R. (2005). Microsaccade dynamics during covert attention. Vision Research, 45, 721–730.PubMedCrossRef Laubrock, J., Engbert, R., & Kliegl, R. (2005). Microsaccade dynamics during covert attention. Vision Research, 45, 721–730.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Laubrock, J., Engbert, R., & Kliegl, R. (2008). Fixational eye movements predict the perceived direction of ambiguous apparent motion. Journal of Vision, 8(14), 1–17.PubMedCrossRef Laubrock, J., Engbert, R., & Kliegl, R. (2008). Fixational eye movements predict the perceived direction of ambiguous apparent motion. Journal of Vision, 8(14), 1–17.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Laubrock, J., Engbert, R., Rolfs, M., & Kliegl, R. (2007). Microsaccades are an index of covert attention. Commentary on Horowitz, Fine, Fencsik, Yurgenson, and Wolfe (2007). Psychological Science, 18, 364–366.PubMedCrossRef Laubrock, J., Engbert, R., Rolfs, M., & Kliegl, R. (2007). Microsaccades are an index of covert attention. Commentary on Horowitz, Fine, Fencsik, Yurgenson, and Wolfe (2007). Psychological Science, 18, 364–366.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Martinez-Conde, S., Macknik, S. L., Troncoso, X. G., & Dyar, T. A. (2006). Microsaccades counteract visual fading during fixation. Neuron, 49, 297–305.PubMedCrossRef Martinez-Conde, S., Macknik, S. L., Troncoso, X. G., & Dyar, T. A. (2006). Microsaccades counteract visual fading during fixation. Neuron, 49, 297–305.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Müller, H. J., & Rabbitt, P. M. (1989). Reflexive and voluntary orienting of visual attention: Time course of activation and resistance to interruption. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 315–330.PubMedCrossRef Müller, H. J., & Rabbitt, P. M. (1989). Reflexive and voluntary orienting of visual attention: Time course of activation and resistance to interruption. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 315–330.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pinheiro, J., & Bates, D. (2000). Mixed-effects models in S and S-Plus. New York: Springer. Pinheiro, J., & Bates, D. (2000). Mixed-effects models in S and S-Plus. New York: Springer.
go back to reference Posner, M. I. (1980). Orientation of attention. The VIIth Sir Frederic Bartlett lecture. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32A, 3–25. Posner, M. I. (1980). Orientation of attention. The VIIth Sir Frederic Bartlett lecture. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32A, 3–25.
go back to reference Posner, M. I., & Cohen, Y. (1984). Components of visual orienting. In H. Bouma & D. G. Bouwhuis (Eds.), Attention and Performance, X (pp. 531–556). Hillsdale: Erlbaum. Posner, M. I., & Cohen, Y. (1984). Components of visual orienting. In H. Bouma & D. G. Bouwhuis (Eds.), Attention and Performance, X (pp. 531–556). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Posner, M. I., Davidson, B. J., & Snyder, C. R. R. (1980). Attention and the detection of signals. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 109, 160–174.CrossRef Posner, M. I., Davidson, B. J., & Snyder, C. R. R. (1980). Attention and the detection of signals. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 109, 160–174.CrossRef
go back to reference Quené, H., & van den Bergh, H. (2004). On multi-level modeling of data from repeated measures designs: A tutorial. Speech Communication, 43, 103–121.CrossRef Quené, H., & van den Bergh, H. (2004). On multi-level modeling of data from repeated measures designs: A tutorial. Speech Communication, 43, 103–121.CrossRef
go back to reference R Development Core Team (2007). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. ISBN 3-900051-07-0, URL http://www.R-project.org. R Development Core Team (2007). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. ISBN 3-900051-07-0, URL http://​www.​R-project.​org.
go back to reference Ratliff, F., & Riggs, L. A. (1950). Involuntary motions of the eye during monocular fixation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40, 687–701.PubMedCrossRef Ratliff, F., & Riggs, L. A. (1950). Involuntary motions of the eye during monocular fixation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40, 687–701.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rattle, J. D., & Foley-Fisher, J. A. (1968). A relationship between vernier acuity and intersaccadic interval. Optica Acta, 15, 617–620.PubMed Rattle, J. D., & Foley-Fisher, J. A. (1968). A relationship between vernier acuity and intersaccadic interval. Optica Acta, 15, 617–620.PubMed
go back to reference Rolfs, M., Engbert, R., & Kliegl, R. (2004). Microsaccade orientation supports attentional enhancement opposite a peripheral cue. Psychological Science, 15, 705–707.PubMedCrossRef Rolfs, M., Engbert, R., & Kliegl, R. (2004). Microsaccade orientation supports attentional enhancement opposite a peripheral cue. Psychological Science, 15, 705–707.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rolfs, M., Engbert, R., & Kliegl, R. (2005). Crossmodal coupling of oculomotor control and spatial attention in vision and audition. Experimental Brain Research, 166, 427–439.CrossRef Rolfs, M., Engbert, R., & Kliegl, R. (2005). Crossmodal coupling of oculomotor control and spatial attention in vision and audition. Experimental Brain Research, 166, 427–439.CrossRef
go back to reference Rolfs, M., Kliegl, R., & Engbert, R. (2008). Toward a model of microsaccade generation: The case of microsaccadic inhibition. Journal of Vision, 8(11), 1–23.PubMedCrossRef Rolfs, M., Kliegl, R., & Engbert, R. (2008). Toward a model of microsaccade generation: The case of microsaccadic inhibition. Journal of Vision, 8(11), 1–23.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rolfs, M., Laubrock, J., & Kliegl, R. (2008). Microsaccade-induced prolongation of saccadic latencies depends on microsaccade amplitude. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 1(3), 1–8. Rolfs, M., Laubrock, J., & Kliegl, R. (2008). Microsaccade-induced prolongation of saccadic latencies depends on microsaccade amplitude. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 1(3), 1–8.
go back to reference Rolfs, M., Laubrock, J., & Kliegl, R. (2006). Shortening and prolongation of saccade latencies following microsaccades. Experimental Brain Research, 169, 369–376.CrossRef Rolfs, M., Laubrock, J., & Kliegl, R. (2006). Shortening and prolongation of saccade latencies following microsaccades. Experimental Brain Research, 169, 369–376.CrossRef
go back to reference Tse, P. U., Sheinberg, D. L., & Logothetis, N. K. (2002). Fixational eye movements are not affected by abrupt onsets that capture attention. Vision Research, 42, 1663–1669.PubMedCrossRef Tse, P. U., Sheinberg, D. L., & Logothetis, N. K. (2002). Fixational eye movements are not affected by abrupt onsets that capture attention. Vision Research, 42, 1663–1669.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Tse, P. U., Sheinberg, D. L., & Logothetis, N. K. (2003). Attentional enhancement opposite a peripheral flash revealed using change blindness. Psychological Science, 14, 91–99.PubMedCrossRef Tse, P. U., Sheinberg, D. L., & Logothetis, N. K. (2003). Attentional enhancement opposite a peripheral flash revealed using change blindness. Psychological Science, 14, 91–99.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Tse, P. U., Sheinberg, D. L., & Logothetis, N. K. (2004). The distribution of microsaccade directions need not reveal the location of attention. Psychological Science, 15, 708–710.CrossRef Tse, P. U., Sheinberg, D. L., & Logothetis, N. K. (2004). The distribution of microsaccade directions need not reveal the location of attention. Psychological Science, 15, 708–710.CrossRef
go back to reference Wickham, H. (2007a). Reshaping data with the reshape package. Journal of Statistical Software, 21,1–19. [Software] R package version 0.8.0. Wickham, H. (2007a). Reshaping data with the reshape package. Journal of Statistical Software, 21,1–19. [Software] R package version 0.8.0.
go back to reference Zuber, B. L., & Stark, L. (1966). Saccadic suppression: Elevation of visual threshold associated with saccadic eye movements. Experimental Neurology, 16, 65–79.PubMedCrossRef Zuber, B. L., & Stark, L. (1966). Saccadic suppression: Elevation of visual threshold associated with saccadic eye movements. Experimental Neurology, 16, 65–79.PubMedCrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Microsaccadic modulation of response times in spatial attention tasks
Auteurs
Reinhold Kliegl
Martin Rolfs
Jochen Laubrock
Ralf Engbert
Publicatiedatum
01-03-2009
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 2/2009
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-008-0202-2

Andere artikelen Uitgave 2/2009

Psychological Research 2/2009 Naar de uitgave