Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 6/2009

01-11-2009 | Original Article

Precueing imminent conflict does not override sequence-dependent interference adaptation

Auteurs: Gamze Alpay, Monique Goerke, Birgit Stürmer

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 6/2009

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Interference effects are largely reduced after cognitive conflicts in previous trials. This sequence-dependent interference adaptation is often seen as a consequence of strategic executive control. We sought to investigate whether sequential modulations are comparable with cue-induced strategic adjustments in spatial interference tasks. If so, reliable cues indicating the next compatibility condition should override effects caused by prior events. To this end, cues were introduced in a spatial stimulus-response compatibility task and a Simon task that either indicated the upcoming trial compatibility (rule cues) or the target position, which was not related to the S-R rule (position cues). The proportion of valid cues was either completely or predominantly valid. In both tasks cueing benefits for absolutely reliable rule cues were clearly present. Remarkably, sequential modulations were not influenced by effective rule cueing and vice versa. Even absolutely reliable information about prospective control demands did not cancel out sequence-dependent interference adaptation. In addition, the contingent negative variation—an event-related brain potential in the cue-target interval that is related to response preparation and readiness—showed additive effects of preceding compatibility and cue reliability. The present results indicate that processes underlying sequence-dependent interference adaptation differ from cue-induced strategic processes of cognitive control.
Voetnoten
1
A comparison of position cues predicting the visual half-field and the response hand revealed no significant difference, t(23) = 2.36, P > 0.08. We, therefore, averaged both position cue conditions for the reported analyses.
 
2
The results of the ANOVA including exact trial repetitions yielded a similar pattern of results: SRC, F(1, 23) = 35.07, P < 0.001; validity, F(1, 23) = 26.95, P < 0.001; cue, F(1, 23) = 21.25, P < 0.001; cue × validity, F(1, 23) = 15.17, P < 0.001; cue × validity × compatibility, F(1, 23) = 3.43, P = 0.07; compatibility × preceding compatibility, F(1, 23) = 12.04, P < 0.01; cue × compatibility × preceding compatibility, F(1, 23) = 6.39, P < 0.05.
 
3
ANOVA including exact trial repetitions revealed similar results: Compatibility, F(1, 23) = 44.54, P < 0.001; validity, F(1, 23) = 16.11, P < 0.001; preceding compatibility, F(2, 46) = 79.16, P < 0.001 (absent when excluding preceding NoGos); cue × validity, F(1, 23) = 5.96, P < 0.05; cue × compatibility, F(1, 23) = 37.72, P < 0.001; compatibility × preceding compatibility, F(1, 23) = 113.14, P < 0.001; cue × compatibility × preceding compatibility, F(1, 23) = 10.76, P < 0.01.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Akçay, C., & Hazeltine, E. (2007). Feature overlap and conflict monitoring: Two sources of sequential modulations. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 14, 742–748.PubMed Akçay, C., & Hazeltine, E. (2007). Feature overlap and conflict monitoring: Two sources of sequential modulations. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 14, 742–748.PubMed
go back to reference Baddeley, A. (1998). Recent developments in working memory. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 8, 234–238.PubMedCrossRef Baddeley, A. (1998). Recent developments in working memory. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 8, 234–238.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Beringer, J. (2001). Experimental Run Time System (ERTS), Version 3.32. Frankfurt: BeriSoft Cooperation. Beringer, J. (2001). Experimental Run Time System (ERTS), Version 3.32. Frankfurt: BeriSoft Cooperation.
go back to reference Botvinick, M. M. (2007). Conflict monitoring and decision making: Reconciling two perspectives on anterior cingulate function. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 7(4), 356–366.CrossRef Botvinick, M. M. (2007). Conflict monitoring and decision making: Reconciling two perspectives on anterior cingulate function. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 7(4), 356–366.CrossRef
go back to reference Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108, 624–652.PubMedCrossRef Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108, 624–652.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Botvinick, M. M., Cohen, J. D., & Carter, C. S. (2004). Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(12), 539–546.PubMedCrossRef Botvinick, M. M., Cohen, J. D., & Carter, C. S. (2004). Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(12), 539–546.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Braver, T. S., Gray, J. R., & Burgess, G. C. (2007). Explaining the many varieties of working memory variation: Dual mechanisms of cognitive control. In A. Conway, C. Jarrold, M. Kane, A. Miyake, J. Towse (Eds.), Variation of working memory. NY:Oxford University Press. Braver, T. S., Gray, J. R., & Burgess, G. C. (2007). Explaining the many varieties of working memory variation: Dual mechanisms of cognitive control. In A. Conway, C. Jarrold, M. Kane, A. Miyake, J. Towse (Eds.), Variation of working memory. NY:Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Chwilla, D. J., & Brunia, C. H. (1991). Event-related potentials to different feedback stimuli. Psychophysiology, 28, 123–132.PubMedCrossRef Chwilla, D. J., & Brunia, C. H. (1991). Event-related potentials to different feedback stimuli. Psychophysiology, 28, 123–132.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cohen, J. D., Botvinick, M. M., & Carter, C. S. (2000). Anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex: who’s in control? Nature Neuroscience, 3, 421–423.PubMedCrossRef Cohen, J. D., Botvinick, M. M., & Carter, C. S. (2000). Anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex: who’s in control? Nature Neuroscience, 3, 421–423.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Damen, E. J., & Brunia, C. H. (1994). Is a stimulus conveying task-relevant information a sufficient condition to elicit a stimulus-preceding negativity? Psychophysiology, 31, 129–139.PubMedCrossRef Damen, E. J., & Brunia, C. H. (1994). Is a stimulus conveying task-relevant information a sufficient condition to elicit a stimulus-preceding negativity? Psychophysiology, 31, 129–139.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference De Pisapia, N., & Braver, T. S. (2006). A model of dual control mechanisms through anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex interactions. Neurocomputing, 69, 1322–1326.CrossRef De Pisapia, N., & Braver, T. S. (2006). A model of dual control mechanisms through anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex interactions. Neurocomputing, 69, 1322–1326.CrossRef
go back to reference Dosenbach, N. U. F., Fair, D. A., Cohen, A. L., Schlaggar, B. L., & Peterson, S. E. (2008). A dual-networks architecture of top-down control. Trends in Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(3), 99–105.CrossRef Dosenbach, N. U. F., Fair, D. A., Cohen, A. L., Schlaggar, B. L., & Peterson, S. E. (2008). A dual-networks architecture of top-down control. Trends in Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(3), 99–105.CrossRef
go back to reference Egner, T. (2007). Congruency sequence effects and cognitive control. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 7(4), 380–390.CrossRef Egner, T. (2007). Congruency sequence effects and cognitive control. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 7(4), 380–390.CrossRef
go back to reference Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception & Psychophysics, 16, 143–149. Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception & Psychophysics, 16, 143–149.
go back to reference Fan, J., Kolster, R., Ghajar, J., Suh, M., Knight, R. T., Sarkar, R., et al. (2007). Response anticipation and response conflict: An event-related potential and functional magnetic resonance imaging study. The Journal of Neuroscience, 27(9), 2272–2282.PubMedCrossRef Fan, J., Kolster, R., Ghajar, J., Suh, M., Knight, R. T., Sarkar, R., et al. (2007). Response anticipation and response conflict: An event-related potential and functional magnetic resonance imaging study. The Journal of Neuroscience, 27(9), 2272–2282.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fernandez-Duque, D., & Knight, M. B. (2008). Cognitive control: Dynamic, sustained, and voluntary influences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 340–355.PubMedCrossRef Fernandez-Duque, D., & Knight, M. B. (2008). Cognitive control: Dynamic, sustained, and voluntary influences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 340–355.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Freitas, A. L., Bahar, M., Yang, S., & Banai, R. (2007). Contextual adjustments in cognitive control across tasks. Psychological Science, 18, 1040–1043.PubMedCrossRef Freitas, A. L., Bahar, M., Yang, S., & Banai, R. (2007). Contextual adjustments in cognitive control across tasks. Psychological Science, 18, 1040–1043.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gratton, G., Coles, M. G., & Donchin, E. (1992). Optimizing the use of information: strategic control of activation of responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 121, 480–506.CrossRef Gratton, G., Coles, M. G., & Donchin, E. (1992). Optimizing the use of information: strategic control of activation of responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 121, 480–506.CrossRef
go back to reference Hommel, B., Proctor, R. W., & Vu, K. P. (2004). A feature-integration account of sequential effects in the Simon task. Psychological Research, 68, 1–17.PubMedCrossRef Hommel, B., Proctor, R. W., & Vu, K. P. (2004). A feature-integration account of sequential effects in the Simon task. Psychological Research, 68, 1–17.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Huynh, H., & Feldt, L. S. (1976). Estimation of the box correction for degrees of freedom from sample data in the randomized block and split-plot designs. Journal of Educational Statistics, 1, 69–82.CrossRef Huynh, H., & Feldt, L. S. (1976). Estimation of the box correction for degrees of freedom from sample data in the randomized block and split-plot designs. Journal of Educational Statistics, 1, 69–82.CrossRef
go back to reference Hyman, R. (1953). Stimulus information as a determinant of reaction time. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 45, 188–196.PubMedCrossRef Hyman, R. (1953). Stimulus information as a determinant of reaction time. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 45, 188–196.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kerns, J. G. (2006). Anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex activity in an fMRI study of trial-to-trial adjustments on the Simon task. NeuroImage, 33, 399–405.PubMedCrossRef Kerns, J. G. (2006). Anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex activity in an fMRI study of trial-to-trial adjustments on the Simon task. NeuroImage, 33, 399–405.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kerns, J. G., Cohen, J. D., MacDonald, A. W., Cho, R. Y., Stenger, V. A., & Carter, C. S. (2004). Anterior cingulate conflict monitoring and adjustments in control. Science, 303, 1023–1026.PubMedCrossRef Kerns, J. G., Cohen, J. D., MacDonald, A. W., Cho, R. Y., Stenger, V. A., & Carter, C. S. (2004). Anterior cingulate conflict monitoring and adjustments in control. Science, 303, 1023–1026.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kerns, J. G., Cohen, J. D., MacDonald, A. W., I. I. I., Johnson, M. K., Stenger, V. A., Aizenstein, H., et al. (2005). Decreased conflict- and error-related activity in the anterior cingulate cortex in subjects with schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162(10), 1833–1839.PubMedCrossRef Kerns, J. G., Cohen, J. D., MacDonald, A. W., I. I. I., Johnson, M. K., Stenger, V. A., Aizenstein, H., et al. (2005). Decreased conflict- and error-related activity in the anterior cingulate cortex in subjects with schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162(10), 1833–1839.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lai, G., & Mangels, J. A. (2007). Cueing effects on semantic and perceptual categorization: ERPs reveal differential effects of validity as a function of processing stage. Neuropsychologia, 45, 2038–2050.PubMedCrossRef Lai, G., & Mangels, J. A. (2007). Cueing effects on semantic and perceptual categorization: ERPs reveal differential effects of validity as a function of processing stage. Neuropsychologia, 45, 2038–2050.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Leuthold, H., Sommer, W., & Ulrich, R. (2004). Preparing for Action: Inferences from CNV and LRP. Journal of Psychophysiology, 18, 77–88.CrossRef Leuthold, H., Sommer, W., & Ulrich, R. (2004). Preparing for Action: Inferences from CNV and LRP. Journal of Psychophysiology, 18, 77–88.CrossRef
go back to reference Loveless, N. E., & Sanford, A. J. (1974). Slow potential correlates of preparatory set. Biological Psychology, 1, 303–314.PubMedCrossRef Loveless, N. E., & Sanford, A. J. (1974). Slow potential correlates of preparatory set. Biological Psychology, 1, 303–314.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mayr, U., & Awh, E. (2008). The elusive link between conflict and conflict adaptation. Psychological Research, this issue. Mayr, U., & Awh, E. (2008). The elusive link between conflict and conflict adaptation. Psychological Research, this issue.
go back to reference Mayr, U., Awh, E., & Laurey, P. (2003). Conflict adaptation effects in the absence of executive control. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 450–452.PubMed Mayr, U., Awh, E., & Laurey, P. (2003). Conflict adaptation effects in the absence of executive control. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 450–452.PubMed
go back to reference Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24, 167–202.PubMedCrossRef Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24, 167–202.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Nieuwenhuis, S., Stins, J. F., Posthuma, D., Polderman, T. J., Boomsma, D. I., & de Geus, E. J. (2006). Accounting for sequential trial effects in the flanker task: conflict adaptation or associative priming? Memory and Cognition, 34, 1260–1272. Nieuwenhuis, S., Stins, J. F., Posthuma, D., Polderman, T. J., Boomsma, D. I., & de Geus, E. J. (2006). Accounting for sequential trial effects in the flanker task: conflict adaptation or associative priming? Memory and Cognition, 34, 1260–1272.
go back to reference Notebaert, W., & Verguts, T. (2006). Stimulus conflict predicts conflict adaptation in a numerical flanker task. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 13, 1078–1084.PubMed Notebaert, W., & Verguts, T. (2006). Stimulus conflict predicts conflict adaptation in a numerical flanker task. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 13, 1078–1084.PubMed
go back to reference Notebaert, W., & Verguts, T. (2007). Dissociating conflict adaptation from feature integration: a multiple regression account. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33, 1256–1260.PubMedCrossRef Notebaert, W., & Verguts, T. (2007). Dissociating conflict adaptation from feature integration: a multiple regression account. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33, 1256–1260.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Notebaert, W., Gevers, W., Verbruggen, F., & Liefooghe, B. (2006). Top-down and bottom-up sequential modulations of congruency effects. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 13, 112–117.PubMed Notebaert, W., Gevers, W., Verbruggen, F., & Liefooghe, B. (2006). Top-down and bottom-up sequential modulations of congruency effects. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 13, 112–117.PubMed
go back to reference Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97–113.PubMedCrossRef Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97–113.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Posner, M. I., Snyder, C. R. R., & Davidson, B. J. (1980). Attention and the detection of signals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109, 160–174.CrossRef Posner, M. I., Snyder, C. R. R., & Davidson, B. J. (1980). Attention and the detection of signals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109, 160–174.CrossRef
go back to reference Praamstra, P., Kleine, B. U., & Schnitzler, A. (1999). Magnetic stimulation of the dorsal premotor cortex modulates the Simon effect. NeuroReport, 10, 3671–3674.PubMedCrossRef Praamstra, P., Kleine, B. U., & Schnitzler, A. (1999). Magnetic stimulation of the dorsal premotor cortex modulates the Simon effect. NeuroReport, 10, 3671–3674.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Proctor, R. W., & Vu, K.-P. L. (2006). Stimulus-response compatibility principles: Data, theory, and application. Boca Raton: CRC Press. Proctor, R. W., & Vu, K.-P. L. (2006). Stimulus-response compatibility principles: Data, theory, and application. Boca Raton: CRC Press.
go back to reference Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2002). Micro- and macro-adjustments of task set: activation and suppression in conflict tasks. Psychological Research, 66, 312–323.PubMedCrossRef Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2002). Micro- and macro-adjustments of task set: activation and suppression in conflict tasks. Psychological Research, 66, 312–323.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ridderinkhof, K. R., Ullsperger, M., Crone, E. A., & Nieuwenhuis, S. (2004). The Role of the Medial Frontal Cortex in Cognitive Control. Science, 306(5695), 443–447.PubMedCrossRef Ridderinkhof, K. R., Ullsperger, M., Crone, E. A., & Nieuwenhuis, S. (2004). The Role of the Medial Frontal Cortex in Cognitive Control. Science, 306(5695), 443–447.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Roberts, A. C., Robbins, T. W., & Weiskrantz, L. (1998). The prefrontal cortex: Executive and Cognitive Functions. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Roberts, A. C., Robbins, T. W., & Weiskrantz, L. (1998). The prefrontal cortex: Executive and Cognitive Functions. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Simon, J. R. (1967). Choice reaction time as a function of auditory S-R correspondence, age and sex. Ergonomics, 10, 559–564.PubMedCrossRef Simon, J. R. (1967). Choice reaction time as a function of auditory S-R correspondence, age and sex. Ergonomics, 10, 559–564.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Smith, E. E., & Jonides, J. (1999). Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes. Science, 238, 1657–1661.CrossRef Smith, E. E., & Jonides, J. (1999). Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes. Science, 238, 1657–1661.CrossRef
go back to reference Stoffels, E.-J. (1996). Uncertainty and processing routes in the selection of a response: An S-R compatibility study. Acta Psychologica, 94(2), 227–252.PubMedCrossRef Stoffels, E.-J. (1996). Uncertainty and processing routes in the selection of a response: An S-R compatibility study. Acta Psychologica, 94(2), 227–252.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Stürmer, B., & Leuthold, H. (2003). Control over response priming in visuomotor processing: a lateralized event-related potential study. Experimental Brain Research, 153, 35–44.CrossRef Stürmer, B., & Leuthold, H. (2003). Control over response priming in visuomotor processing: a lateralized event-related potential study. Experimental Brain Research, 153, 35–44.CrossRef
go back to reference Stürmer, B., Soetens, E., Sommer, W., Leuthold, H., & Schröter, H. (2002). Control Over Location-Based Response Activation in the Simon Task: Behavioral and Electrophysiological Evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 28, 1345–1363.PubMed Stürmer, B., Soetens, E., Sommer, W., Leuthold, H., & Schröter, H. (2002). Control Over Location-Based Response Activation in the Simon Task: Behavioral and Electrophysiological Evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 28, 1345–1363.PubMed
go back to reference Stürmer, B., Redlich, M., Irlbacher, K., & Brandt, S. (2007). Executive control over response priming and conflict: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Experimental Brain Research, 183(3), 329–339.CrossRef Stürmer, B., Redlich, M., Irlbacher, K., & Brandt, S. (2007). Executive control over response priming and conflict: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Experimental Brain Research, 183(3), 329–339.CrossRef
go back to reference Ullsperger, M., Bylsma, L. M., & Botvinick, M. M. (2005). The conflict adaptation effect: It’s not just priming. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 5, 467–472.PubMedCrossRef Ullsperger, M., Bylsma, L. M., & Botvinick, M. M. (2005). The conflict adaptation effect: It’s not just priming. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 5, 467–472.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference van Boxtel, G. J. M., & Brunia, C. H. M. (1994). Motor and non-motor components of the Contingent Negative Variation. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 17, 269–279.PubMedCrossRef van Boxtel, G. J. M., & Brunia, C. H. M. (1994). Motor and non-motor components of the Contingent Negative Variation. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 17, 269–279.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Vossel, S., Thiel, C. M., & Fink, G. R. (2006). Cue validity modulates the neural correlates of covert endogenous orienting of attention in parietal and frontal cortex. Neuroimage, 32, 1257–1264.PubMedCrossRef Vossel, S., Thiel, C. M., & Fink, G. R. (2006). Cue validity modulates the neural correlates of covert endogenous orienting of attention in parietal and frontal cortex. Neuroimage, 32, 1257–1264.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Weerts, T. C., & Lang, P. J. (1973). The effects of eye fixation and stimulus and response location on the contingent negative variation (CNV). Biological Psychology, 1, 1–19.PubMedCrossRef Weerts, T. C., & Lang, P. J. (1973). The effects of eye fixation and stimulus and response location on the contingent negative variation (CNV). Biological Psychology, 1, 1–19.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wendt, M., Kluwe, R. H., & Peters, A. (2006). Sequential modulations of interference evoked by processing task-irrelevant stimulus features. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 644–667.PubMedCrossRef Wendt, M., Kluwe, R. H., & Peters, A. (2006). Sequential modulations of interference evoked by processing task-irrelevant stimulus features. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 644–667.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wühr, P., & Ansorge, U. (2005). Exploring trial-by-trial modulations of the Simon effect. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A, 58, 705–731. Wühr, P., & Ansorge, U. (2005). Exploring trial-by-trial modulations of the Simon effect. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A, 58, 705–731.
go back to reference Wühr, P., & Kunde, W. (2008). Precueing spatial S-R correspondence: is there regulation of expected response conflict? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34(4), 838–872.CrossRef Wühr, P., & Kunde, W. (2008). Precueing spatial S-R correspondence: is there regulation of expected response conflict? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34(4), 838–872.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Precueing imminent conflict does not override sequence-dependent interference adaptation
Auteurs
Gamze Alpay
Monique Goerke
Birgit Stürmer
Publicatiedatum
01-11-2009
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 6/2009
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-008-0196-9

Andere artikelen Uitgave 6/2009

Psychological Research 6/2009 Naar de uitgave