Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 4/2008

01-07-2008 | Original Article

The procedural learning of action order is independent of temporal learning

Auteur: Jacqueline C. Shin

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 4/2008

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

How does learning the timing of actions influence our ability to learn the order of actions? A sequence of responses cued by spatial stimuli was learned in a serial reaction time task where the response-to-stimulus intervals (RSIs) were random, constant, or followed a fixed sequence. In this final sequenced-RSI condition, the response and RSI sequences were consistently matched in phase and could be integrated into a common sequence representation. The main result was that the response sequence was learned to a similar degree in all RSI training conditions, indicating that neither the predictability of RSIs nor the integration of the phase-matched response and timing sequences benefited learning of the response sequence. Nevertheless, temporal learning and integration speeded up performance without strengthening the representation of response order.
Voetnoten
1
The data for two participants in the constant-RSI training condition were excluded from further analysis, because the percentage of key-presses extending into the following trials was greater than three standard deviations above the mean (1.5%) (>2%) and unusually high during some of the probe blocks (up to 7.3% in one participant and up to 27.3% in the other). The proportion correct was on average 0.950 (SD = 0.028). Data from three more participants were excluded due to mean proportion correct values that were less than three standard deviations below the mean (0.89)—one participant each from the constant-RSI training condition, the sequenced-R training condition, and the sequenced-C training condition.
 
2
Recently, it has been shown by various authors that sequences with reversals, or trills, such as “1-2-1”, elicit cognitive processes that serve to underestimate sequence learning when the proportion of reversals differ in sequenced versus random blocks. In the current study, the pattern of results was virtually identical regardless of whether trials that contained reversals in the response sequence (e.g., the second “1” in “1-2-1”) were included in the analysis or not. Therefore, the results from data inclusive of reversals are reported in this paper.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Baddeley, A. D. (1982). Domains of recollection. Psychological Review, 89, 708–729.CrossRef Baddeley, A. D. (1982). Domains of recollection. Psychological Review, 89, 708–729.CrossRef
go back to reference Bjork, R. A., & Richardson-Klavehn, A. (1989). On the puzzling relationship between environmental context and human memory. In: C. Izawa (Eds) Current issues in cognitive processes: the Tulane Floweree symposium on cognition, (pp. 313–344). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc. Bjork, R. A., & Richardson-Klavehn, A. (1989). On the puzzling relationship between environmental context and human memory. In: C. Izawa (Eds) Current issues in cognitive processes: the Tulane Floweree symposium on cognition, (pp. 313–344). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc.
go back to reference Buchner, A., & Steffens, M. C. (2001). Simultaneous learning of different regularities in sequence learning tasks: limits and characteristics. Psychological Research, 65, 71–80.PubMedCrossRef Buchner, A., & Steffens, M. C. (2001). Simultaneous learning of different regularities in sequence learning tasks: limits and characteristics. Psychological Research, 65, 71–80.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Eich E. (1985). Context, memory, and integrated item/context imagery. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition, 11, 764–770.CrossRef Eich E. (1985). Context, memory, and integrated item/context imagery. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition, 11, 764–770.CrossRef
go back to reference Geiselman, R. E., & Bjork, R. A. (1980). Primary versus secondary rehearsal in imagined voices: differential effects on recognition. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 188–205.PubMedCrossRef Geiselman, R. E., & Bjork, R. A. (1980). Primary versus secondary rehearsal in imagined voices: differential effects on recognition. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 188–205.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Geiselman, R. E., & Glenny, J. (1977). Effects of imagining speakers’ voices on the retention of words presented visually. Memory & Cognition, 5, 499–504. Geiselman, R. E., & Glenny, J. (1977). Effects of imagining speakers’ voices on the retention of words presented visually. Memory & Cognition, 5, 499–504.
go back to reference Gentner, D. R. (1987). Timing of skilled motor performance: tests of the proportional duration model. Psychological Review, 94, 255–276.CrossRef Gentner, D. R. (1987). Timing of skilled motor performance: tests of the proportional duration model. Psychological Review, 94, 255–276.CrossRef
go back to reference Heuer, H. (1988a). Adjustment and readjustment of the relative timing of a motor pattern. Psychological Research, 50, 83–93.PubMedCrossRef Heuer, H. (1988a). Adjustment and readjustment of the relative timing of a motor pattern. Psychological Research, 50, 83–93.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Heuer, H. (1988b). Testing the invariance of relative timing: comment on Gentner (1987). Psychological Review, 95, 552–557.PubMedCrossRef Heuer, H. (1988b). Testing the invariance of relative timing: comment on Gentner (1987). Psychological Review, 95, 552–557.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Heuer, H. (1991). Invariant relative timing in motor-program theory. In J. Fagard, P. H. Wolff (Eds.), The development of timing control and temporal organization in coordinated action: invariant timing, rhythms and coordination (pp. 37–68). Amsterdam: North-Holland.CrossRef Heuer, H. (1991). Invariant relative timing in motor-program theory. In J. Fagard, P. H. Wolff (Eds.), The development of timing control and temporal organization in coordinated action: invariant timing, rhythms and coordination (pp. 37–68). Amsterdam: North-Holland.CrossRef
go back to reference Jones, M. R., & Ralston, J. T. (1991). Some influences of accent structure on melody recognition. Memory & Cognition, 19, 8–20. Jones, M. R., & Ralston, J. T. (1991). Some influences of accent structure on melody recognition. Memory & Cognition, 19, 8–20.
go back to reference Jones, M. R., Summerell, L., & Marshburn, E. (1987). Recognizing melodies: a dynamic interpretation. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 39A, 89–121. Jones, M. R., Summerell, L., & Marshburn, E. (1987). Recognizing melodies: a dynamic interpretation. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 39A, 89–121.
go back to reference Keele, S. W., Ivry, R., Mayr, U., Hazeltine, E., & Heuer, H. (2003). The cognitive and neural architecture of sequence representation. Psychological Review, 110, 316–339.PubMedCrossRef Keele, S. W., Ivry, R., Mayr, U., Hazeltine, E., & Heuer, H. (2003). The cognitive and neural architecture of sequence representation. Psychological Review, 110, 316–339.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lee, D. N., Young, D. S., Reddish, P. E., Lough, S., & Clayton, T. M. H. (1983). Visual timing in hitting an accelerating ball. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 35A, 333–346. Lee, D. N., Young, D. S., Reddish, P. E., Lough, S., & Clayton, T. M. H. (1983). Visual timing in hitting an accelerating ball. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 35A, 333–346.
go back to reference Murnane, K., & Phelps, M. P. (1993). A global activation approach to the effect of changes in environmental context on recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 19, 882–894.CrossRef Murnane, K., & Phelps, M. P. (1993). A global activation approach to the effect of changes in environmental context on recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 19, 882–894.CrossRef
go back to reference Murnane, K., & Phelps, M. P. (1994). When does a different environmental context make a difference in recognition? A global activation model. Memory & Cognition, 22, 584–590. Murnane, K., & Phelps, M. P. (1994). When does a different environmental context make a difference in recognition? A global activation model. Memory & Cognition, 22, 584–590.
go back to reference Nissen, M. J., & Bullemer, P. (1987). Attentional requirements of learning: evidence from performance measures. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 1–32.CrossRef Nissen, M. J., & Bullemer, P. (1987). Attentional requirements of learning: evidence from performance measures. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 1–32.CrossRef
go back to reference Quesada, D. C., & Schmidt, R. A. (1970). A test of the Adams-Creamer decay hypothesis for the timing of motor responses. Journal of Motor Behavior, 2, 273–283. Quesada, D. C., & Schmidt, R. A. (1970). A test of the Adams-Creamer decay hypothesis for the timing of motor responses. Journal of Motor Behavior, 2, 273–283.
go back to reference Rah, S. K.-Y., Reber, A. S., & Hsiao, A. T. (2000). Another wrinkle on the dual-task SRT experiment: it’s probably not dual task. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 7, 309–313. Rah, S. K.-Y., Reber, A. S., & Hsiao, A. T. (2000). Another wrinkle on the dual-task SRT experiment: it’s probably not dual task. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 7, 309–313.
go back to reference Rantanen, E., & Xu, X. (2001). Human performance in timing of discrete actions. Paper presented at the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society 45th annual meeting. St. Paul, MN. Rantanen, E., & Xu, X. (2001). Human performance in timing of discrete actions. Paper presented at the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society 45th annual meeting. St. Paul, MN.
go back to reference Rosenbaum, D. A., & Patashnik, O. (1980). A mental clock-setting process revealed by reaction times. In G. E. Stelmach, & J. Requin (Eds.), Tutorials in motor behavior (pp. 487–499). Amsterdam: North-Holland.CrossRef Rosenbaum, D. A., & Patashnik, O. (1980). A mental clock-setting process revealed by reaction times. In G. E. Stelmach, & J. Requin (Eds.), Tutorials in motor behavior (pp. 487–499). Amsterdam: North-Holland.CrossRef
go back to reference Schmidt, R. A. (1975). A schema theory of discrete motor skill learning. Psychological Review, 82, 225–260.CrossRef Schmidt, R. A. (1975). A schema theory of discrete motor skill learning. Psychological Review, 82, 225–260.CrossRef
go back to reference Schmidt, R. A. (1980). On the theoretical status of time in motor program representations. In G. E. Stelmach, & J. Requin (Eds.), Tutorials in motor behavior (pp. 145–166). Amsterdam: North-Holland.CrossRef Schmidt, R. A. (1980). On the theoretical status of time in motor program representations. In G. E. Stelmach, & J. Requin (Eds.), Tutorials in motor behavior (pp. 145–166). Amsterdam: North-Holland.CrossRef
go back to reference Schmidtke, V., & Heuer, H. (1997). Task integration as a factor in secondary-task effects on sequence learning. Psychological Research, 60, 53–71.CrossRef Schmidtke, V., & Heuer, H. (1997). Task integration as a factor in secondary-task effects on sequence learning. Psychological Research, 60, 53–71.CrossRef
go back to reference Shaffer, L. H. (1981). Performances of Chopin, Bach, and Bartok: studies in motor programming. Cognitive Psychology, 13, 326–376.CrossRef Shaffer, L. H. (1981). Performances of Chopin, Bach, and Bartok: studies in motor programming. Cognitive Psychology, 13, 326–376.CrossRef
go back to reference Shaffer, L. H. (1984). Timing in musical performance. In J. Gibbon, & L. Allen (Eds.), Timing and time perception (pp. 420–428). New York: New York Academy of Sciences. Shaffer, L. H. (1984). Timing in musical performance. In J. Gibbon, & L. Allen (Eds.), Timing and time perception (pp. 420–428). New York: New York Academy of Sciences.
go back to reference Shin, J. C., & Ivry, R. B. (2002). Concurrent learning of temporal and spatial sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28, 445–457.PubMedCrossRef Shin, J. C., & Ivry, R. B. (2002). Concurrent learning of temporal and spatial sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28, 445–457.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Smith, S. M. (1986). Environmental context-dependent recognition memory using a short-term memory task for input. Memory & Cognition, 14, 347–354. Smith, S. M. (1986). Environmental context-dependent recognition memory using a short-term memory task for input. Memory & Cognition, 14, 347–354.
go back to reference Smith, S. M., & Vela, E. (1992). Environmental context-dependent eyewitness recognition. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 6, 125–139.CrossRef Smith, S. M., & Vela, E. (1992). Environmental context-dependent eyewitness recognition. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 6, 125–139.CrossRef
go back to reference Summers, J. J. (1975). The role of timing in motor program representation. Journal of Motor Behavior, 7, 229–241. Summers, J. J. (1975). The role of timing in motor program representation. Journal of Motor Behavior, 7, 229–241.
go back to reference Summers, J. J. (1977). The relationship between the sequencing and timing components of a skill. Journal of Motor Behavior, 9, 49–59. Summers, J. J. (1977). The relationship between the sequencing and timing components of a skill. Journal of Motor Behavior, 9, 49–59.
go back to reference Terzuolo, C. A., & Viviani, P. (1980). Determinants and characteristics of motor patterns used for typing. Neuroscience, 5, 1085–1103.PubMedCrossRef Terzuolo, C. A., & Viviani, P. (1980). Determinants and characteristics of motor patterns used for typing. Neuroscience, 5, 1085–1103.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Viviani, P., & Terzuolo, C. (1980). Space-time invariance in learned motor skills. In G. E. Stelmach, & J. Requin (Eds.), Tutorials in motor behavior (pp. 525–533). Amsterdam: North-Holland.CrossRef Viviani, P., & Terzuolo, C. (1980). Space-time invariance in learned motor skills. In G. E. Stelmach, & J. Requin (Eds.), Tutorials in motor behavior (pp. 525–533). Amsterdam: North-Holland.CrossRef
go back to reference Willingham, D. B., Greenberg, A. R., & Thomas, R. C. (1997). Response-to-stimulus interval does not affect implicit motor sequence learning, but does affect performance. Memory and Cognition, 25, 534–542. Willingham, D. B., Greenberg, A. R., & Thomas, R. C. (1997). Response-to-stimulus interval does not affect implicit motor sequence learning, but does affect performance. Memory and Cognition, 25, 534–542.
Metagegevens
Titel
The procedural learning of action order is independent of temporal learning
Auteur
Jacqueline C. Shin
Publicatiedatum
01-07-2008
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 4/2008
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-007-0115-5

Andere artikelen Uitgave 4/2008

Psychological Research 4/2008 Naar de uitgave