Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 3/2007

01-05-2007 | Original Article

Spatial updating in virtual reality: the sufficiency of visual information

Auteurs: Bernhard E. Riecke, Douglas W. Cunningham, Heinrich H. Bülthoff

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 3/2007

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Robust and effortless spatial orientation critically relies on “automatic and obligatory spatial updating”, a largely automatized and reflex-like process that transforms our mental egocentric representation of the immediate surroundings during ego-motions. A rapid pointing paradigm was used to assess automatic/obligatory spatial updating after visually displayed upright rotations with or without concomitant physical rotations using a motion platform. Visual stimuli displaying a natural, subject-known scene proved sufficient for enabling automatic and obligatory spatial updating, irrespective of concurrent physical motions. This challenges the prevailing notion that visual cues alone are insufficient for enabling such spatial updating of rotations, and that vestibular/proprioceptive cues are both required and sufficient. Displaying optic flow devoid of landmarks during the motion and pointing phase was insufficient for enabling automatic spatial updating, but could not be entirely ignored either. Interestingly, additional physical motion cues hardly improved performance, and were insufficient for affording automatic spatial updating. The results are discussed in the context of the mental transformation hypothesis and the sensorimotor interference hypothesis, which associates difficulties in imagined perspective switches to interference between the sensorimotor and cognitive (to-be-imagined) perspective.
Voetnoten
1
Gender is known to correlate, under certain circumstances, with spatial abilities like imagined perspective changes and spatial updating of path layouts (Mou et al., 2004; Sholl & Bartels, 2002). Studies using a methodology comparable to the current study did, however, not show any gender effects. Hence, the female bias in the current participant population does not seem critical. Moreover, only withinsubject analyzes were used in the current study.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Avraamides, M. N., & Ioannidou, L. M. (2005). Locating targets from imagined perspectives: Labeling vs. pointing. In Proceedings of the XXVII Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, Stresa, Italy (pp. 175–180). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum. Avraamides, M. N., & Ioannidou, L. M. (2005). Locating targets from imagined perspectives: Labeling vs. pointing. In Proceedings of the XXVII Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, Stresa, Italy (pp. 175–180). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.
go back to reference Batschelet, E. (1981). Circular statistics in biology. London: Academic. Batschelet, E. (1981). Circular statistics in biology. London: Academic.
go back to reference Bremmer, F., & Lappe, M. (1999). The use of optical velocities for distance discrimination and reproduction during visually simulated self motion. Experimental Brain Research, 127(1), 33–42.CrossRef Bremmer, F., & Lappe, M. (1999). The use of optical velocities for distance discrimination and reproduction during visually simulated self motion. Experimental Brain Research, 127(1), 33–42.CrossRef
go back to reference Brockmole, J. R., & Wang, R. X. F. (2003). Changing perspective within and across environments. Cognition, 87(2), B59–B67.PubMedCrossRef Brockmole, J. R., & Wang, R. X. F. (2003). Changing perspective within and across environments. Cognition, 87(2), B59–B67.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Chance, S. S., Gaunet, F., Beall, A. C., & Loomis, J. M. (1998). Locomotion mode affects the updating of objects encountered during travel: The contribution of vestibular and proprioceptive inputs to path integration. Presence Teleoperators and Virtual Environments, 7(2), 168–178.CrossRef Chance, S. S., Gaunet, F., Beall, A. C., & Loomis, J. M. (1998). Locomotion mode affects the updating of objects encountered during travel: The contribution of vestibular and proprioceptive inputs to path integration. Presence Teleoperators and Virtual Environments, 7(2), 168–178.CrossRef
go back to reference Cooper, L. A., & Shepard, R. N. (1973). Time required to prepare for a rotated stimulus. Memory & Cognition, 1(3), 246–250. Cooper, L. A., & Shepard, R. N. (1973). Time required to prepare for a rotated stimulus. Memory & Cognition, 1(3), 246–250.
go back to reference de Vega, M., & Rodrigo, M. J. (2001). Updating spatial layouts mediated by pointing and labelling under physical and imaginary rotation. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 13(3), 369–393. de Vega, M., & Rodrigo, M. J. (2001). Updating spatial layouts mediated by pointing and labelling under physical and imaginary rotation. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 13(3), 369–393.
go back to reference Dichgans, J., & Brandt, T. (1978). Visual vestibular interaction: Effects on self-motion perception and postural control. In R. Held, H. W. Leibowitz, & H.L. Teuber (Eds.), Perception (vol. VIII, pp. 756–804). Berlin Heidelberg New York: Springer. Dichgans, J., & Brandt, T. (1978). Visual vestibular interaction: Effects on self-motion perception and postural control. In R. Held, H. W. Leibowitz, & H.L. Teuber (Eds.), Perception (vol. VIII, pp. 756–804). Berlin Heidelberg New York: Springer.
go back to reference Diwadkar, V. A., & McNamara, T. P. (1997). Viewpoint dependence in scene recognition. Psychological Science, 8(4), 302–307.CrossRef Diwadkar, V. A., & McNamara, T. P. (1997). Viewpoint dependence in scene recognition. Psychological Science, 8(4), 302–307.CrossRef
go back to reference Easton, R. D., & Sholl, M. J. (1995). Object-array structure, frames of reference, and retrieval of spatial knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory and Cognition, 21(2), 483–500.CrossRef Easton, R. D., & Sholl, M. J. (1995). Object-array structure, frames of reference, and retrieval of spatial knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory and Cognition, 21(2), 483–500.CrossRef
go back to reference Farrell, M. J., & Robertson, I. H. (1998). Mental rotation and the automatic updating of bodycentered spatial relationships. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory and Cognition, 24(1), 227–233.CrossRef Farrell, M. J., & Robertson, I. H. (1998). Mental rotation and the automatic updating of bodycentered spatial relationships. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory and Cognition, 24(1), 227–233.CrossRef
go back to reference Farrell, M. J., & Robertson, I. H. (2000). The automatic updating of egocentric spatial relationships and its impairment due to right posterior cortical lesions. Neuropsychologia, 38(5), 585–595.PubMedCrossRef Farrell, M. J., & Robertson, I. H. (2000). The automatic updating of egocentric spatial relationships and its impairment due to right posterior cortical lesions. Neuropsychologia, 38(5), 585–595.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Farrell, M. J., & Thomson, J. A. (1998). Automatic spatial updating during locomotion without vision. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A Human Experimental Psychology, 51(3), 637–654. Farrell, M. J., & Thomson, J. A. (1998). Automatic spatial updating during locomotion without vision. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A Human Experimental Psychology, 51(3), 637–654.
go back to reference Hettinger, L. J. (2002). Illusory self-motion in virtual environments. In K. M. Stanney (Ed.), Handbook of virtual environments (pp. 471–492). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum. Hettinger, L. J. (2002). Illusory self-motion in virtual environments. In K. M. Stanney (Ed.), Handbook of virtual environments (pp. 471–492). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.
go back to reference Hintzman, D. L., O’Dell, C. S., & Arndt, D. R. (1981). Orientation in cognitive maps. Cognitive Psychology, 13(2), 149–206.PubMedCrossRef Hintzman, D. L., O’Dell, C. S., & Arndt, D. R. (1981). Orientation in cognitive maps. Cognitive Psychology, 13(2), 149–206.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hirtle, S. C., & Jonides, J. (1985). Evidence of hierarchies in cognitive maps. Memory & Cognition, 13(3), 208–217. Hirtle, S. C., & Jonides, J. (1985). Evidence of hierarchies in cognitive maps. Memory & Cognition, 13(3), 208–217.
go back to reference Holmes, M. C., & Sholl, M. J. (2005). Allocentric coding of object-to-object relations in overlearned and novel environments. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning. Memory and Cognition, 31(5), 1069–1087.CrossRef Holmes, M. C., & Sholl, M. J. (2005). Allocentric coding of object-to-object relations in overlearned and novel environments. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning. Memory and Cognition, 31(5), 1069–1087.CrossRef
go back to reference Ivanenko, Y. P., Viaud-Delmon, I., Siegler, I., Israël, I., & Berthoz, A. (1998). The vestibulo-ocular reflex and angular displacement perception in darkness in humans: adaptation to a virtual environment. Neuroscience Letters, 241(2–3), 167–170.PubMed Ivanenko, Y. P., Viaud-Delmon, I., Siegler, I., Israël, I., & Berthoz, A. (1998). The vestibulo-ocular reflex and angular displacement perception in darkness in humans: adaptation to a virtual environment. Neuroscience Letters, 241(2–3), 167–170.PubMed
go back to reference Klatzky, R. L., Loomis, J. M., Beall, A. C., Chance, S. S., & Golledge, R. G. (1998). Spatial updating of self-position and orientation during real, imagined, and virtual locomotion. Psychology Science, 9(4), 293–298.CrossRef Klatzky, R. L., Loomis, J. M., Beall, A. C., Chance, S. S., & Golledge, R. G. (1998). Spatial updating of self-position and orientation during real, imagined, and virtual locomotion. Psychology Science, 9(4), 293–298.CrossRef
go back to reference Loomis, J. M., & Beall, A. C. (1998). Visually controlled locomotion: Its dependence on optic flow, three-dimensional space perception, and cognition. Ecological Psychology, 10(3–4), 271–285.CrossRef Loomis, J. M., & Beall, A. C. (1998). Visually controlled locomotion: Its dependence on optic flow, three-dimensional space perception, and cognition. Ecological Psychology, 10(3–4), 271–285.CrossRef
go back to reference Loomis, J. M., Da Silva, J. A., Philbeck, J. W., & Fukusima, S. S. (1996). Visual perception of location and distance. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 5(3), 72–77.CrossRef Loomis, J. M., Da Silva, J. A., Philbeck, J. W., & Fukusima, S. S. (1996). Visual perception of location and distance. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 5(3), 72–77.CrossRef
go back to reference May, M. (1996). Cognitive and embodied modes of spatial imagery. Psychologische Beiträge, 38(3/4), 418–434. May, M. (1996). Cognitive and embodied modes of spatial imagery. Psychologische Beiträge, 38(3/4), 418–434.
go back to reference May, M. (2000). Kognition im Umraum [cognition in spatial surroundings]. Wiesbaden: DUV: Kognitionswissenschaft. May, M. (2000). Kognition im Umraum [cognition in spatial surroundings]. Wiesbaden: DUV: Kognitionswissenschaft.
go back to reference May, M. (2001). Mechanismen räumlicher Perspektivwechsel [mechanisms of spatial perspective switches]. In R. K. Silbereisen & M. Reitzle (Eds.), Psychologie 2000 (pp. 627–634). Lengerich: Pabst. May, M. (2001). Mechanismen räumlicher Perspektivwechsel [mechanisms of spatial perspective switches]. In R. K. Silbereisen & M. Reitzle (Eds.), Psychologie 2000 (pp. 627–634). Lengerich: Pabst.
go back to reference May, M. (2004). Imaginal perspective switches in remembered environments: Transformation versus interference accounts. Cognitive Psychology, 48(2), 163–206.PubMedCrossRef May, M. (2004). Imaginal perspective switches in remembered environments: Transformation versus interference accounts. Cognitive Psychology, 48(2), 163–206.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference May, M., & Klatzky, R. L. (2000). Path integration while ignoring irrelevant movement. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory and Cognition, 26(1), 169–186.CrossRef May, M., & Klatzky, R. L. (2000). Path integration while ignoring irrelevant movement. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory and Cognition, 26(1), 169–186.CrossRef
go back to reference McNamara, T. P. (1986). Mental representations of spatial relations. Cognitive Psychology, 18(1), 87–121.PubMedCrossRef McNamara, T. P. (1986). Mental representations of spatial relations. Cognitive Psychology, 18(1), 87–121.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mou, W. M., McNamara, T. P., Valiquette, C. M., & Rump, B. (2004). Allocentric and egocentric updating of spatial memories. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory and Cognition, 30(1), 142–157.CrossRef Mou, W. M., McNamara, T. P., Valiquette, C. M., & Rump, B. (2004). Allocentric and egocentric updating of spatial memories. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory and Cognition, 30(1), 142–157.CrossRef
go back to reference Presson, C. C., & Montello, D. R. (1994). Updating after rotational and translational body movements: Coordinate structure of perspective space. Perception, 23(12), 1447–1455.PubMedCrossRef Presson, C. C., & Montello, D. R. (1994). Updating after rotational and translational body movements: Coordinate structure of perspective space. Perception, 23(12), 1447–1455.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Riecke, B. E., van Veen, H. A. H. C., & Bülthoff, H. H. (2002). Visual homing is possible without landmarks: A path integration study in virtual reality. Presence Teleoperators and Virtual Environments, 11(5), 443–473.CrossRef Riecke, B. E., van Veen, H. A. H. C., & Bülthoff, H. H. (2002). Visual homing is possible without landmarks: A path integration study in virtual reality. Presence Teleoperators and Virtual Environments, 11(5), 443–473.CrossRef
go back to reference Riecke, B. E., von der Heyde, M., & Bülthoff, H. H. (2004). Spatial updating in real and virtual environments contribution and interaction of visual and vestibular cues. In ACM SIGGRAPH Symposium on Applied Perception in Graphics and Visualization (APGV) (pp. 9–17). Los Angeles, USA. (Available: http://www.kyb.mpg.de/publication.html?publ=2764). Riecke, B. E., von der Heyde, M., & Bülthoff, H. H. (2004). Spatial updating in real and virtual environments contribution and interaction of visual and vestibular cues. In ACM SIGGRAPH Symposium on Applied Perception in Graphics and Visualization (APGV) (pp. 9–17). Los Angeles, USA. (Available: http://​www.​kyb.​mpg.​de/​publication.​html?​publ=​2764).
go back to reference Riecke, B. E., Schulte-Pelkum, J., & Bülthoff, H. H. (2005a). Perceiving simulated ego-motions in virtual reality comparing large screen displays with HMDs. In B. E. Rogowitz, T. N. Pappas, & S. J. Daly (Eds.), SPIE invited paper on VALVE: Vision, action, and locomotion in virtual (and real) environments (pp. 344–355). San Jose, CA, USA. Riecke, B. E., Schulte-Pelkum, J., & Bülthoff, H. H. (2005a). Perceiving simulated ego-motions in virtual reality comparing large screen displays with HMDs. In B. E. Rogowitz, T. N. Pappas, & S. J. Daly (Eds.), SPIE invited paper on VALVE: Vision, action, and locomotion in virtual (and real) environments (pp. 344–355). San Jose, CA, USA.
go back to reference Riecke, B. E., Schulte-Pelkum, J., Caniard, F., & Bülthoff, H. H. (2005b). Towards lean and elegant self-motion simulation in virtual reality. In Proceedings of IEEE VR2005 (pp. 131–138). Bonn, Germany. Riecke, B. E., Schulte-Pelkum, J., Caniard, F., & Bülthoff, H. H. (2005b). Towards lean and elegant self-motion simulation in virtual reality. In Proceedings of IEEE VR2005 (pp. 131–138). Bonn, Germany.
go back to reference Riecke, B. E., Västfjäll, D., Larsson, P., & Schulte-Pelkum, J. (2005c). Topdown and multimodal influences on self-motion perception in virtual reality. In Proceedings of HCI International 2005. Las Vegas, NV, USA. Riecke, B. E., Västfjäll, D., Larsson, P., & Schulte-Pelkum, J. (2005c). Topdown and multimodal influences on self-motion perception in virtual reality. In Proceedings of HCI International 2005. Las Vegas, NV, USA.
go back to reference Riecke, B. E., von der Heyde, M., & Bülthoff, H. H. (2005d). Visual cues can be sufficient for triggering automatic, reflex-like spatial updating. ACM Transactions on Applied Perception (TAP), 2(3), 183–215.CrossRef Riecke, B. E., von der Heyde, M., & Bülthoff, H. H. (2005d). Visual cues can be sufficient for triggering automatic, reflex-like spatial updating. ACM Transactions on Applied Perception (TAP), 2(3), 183–215.CrossRef
go back to reference Riecke, B. E., Schulte-Pelkum, J., & Caniard, F. (2006). Using the perceptually oriented approach to optimize spatial presence & egomotion simulation. In Handbook of Presence. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum (submitted). Riecke, B. E., Schulte-Pelkum, J., & Caniard, F. (2006). Using the perceptually oriented approach to optimize spatial presence & egomotion simulation. In Handbook of Presence. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum (submitted).
go back to reference Rieser, J. J. (1989). Access to knowledge of spatial structure at novel points of observation. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory and Cognition, 15(6), 1157–1165.CrossRef Rieser, J. J. (1989). Access to knowledge of spatial structure at novel points of observation. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory and Cognition, 15(6), 1157–1165.CrossRef
go back to reference Roskos-Ewoldsen, B., McNamara, T. P., Shelton, A. L., & Carr, W. (1998). Mental representations of large and small spatial layouts are orientation dependent. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory and Cognition, 24(1), 215–226.CrossRef Roskos-Ewoldsen, B., McNamara, T. P., Shelton, A. L., & Carr, W. (1998). Mental representations of large and small spatial layouts are orientation dependent. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory and Cognition, 24(1), 215–226.CrossRef
go back to reference Schulte-Pelkum, J., Riecke, B. E., Caniard, F., & Bülthoff, H. H. (2006). Influence of brief physical accelerations and vibrations on visually induced linear vection (manuscript in preparation). Schulte-Pelkum, J., Riecke, B. E., Caniard, F., & Bülthoff, H. H. (2006). Influence of brief physical accelerations and vibrations on visually induced linear vection (manuscript in preparation).
go back to reference Shepard, R. N., & Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of 3-dimensional objects. Science, 171(3972), 701–703.PubMedCrossRef Shepard, R. N., & Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of 3-dimensional objects. Science, 171(3972), 701–703.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sholl, M. J., & Bartels, G. P. (2002). The role of self-to-object updating in orientation-free performance on spatial memory tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning. Memory and Cognition, 28(3), 422–436.CrossRef Sholl, M. J., & Bartels, G. P. (2002). The role of self-to-object updating in orientation-free performance on spatial memory tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning. Memory and Cognition, 28(3), 422–436.CrossRef
go back to reference Sholl, M. J., & Nolin, T. L. (1997). Orientation specificity in representations of place. Journal of Experimental Psychology.Learning, Memory and Cognition, 23(6), 1494–1507.CrossRef Sholl, M. J., & Nolin, T. L. (1997). Orientation specificity in representations of place. Journal of Experimental Psychology.Learning, Memory and Cognition, 23(6), 1494–1507.CrossRef
go back to reference Simons, D. J., & Wang, R. F. (1998). Perceiving real-world viewpoint changes. Psychological Science, 9(4), 315–320.CrossRef Simons, D. J., & Wang, R. F. (1998). Perceiving real-world viewpoint changes. Psychological Science, 9(4), 315–320.CrossRef
go back to reference Simons, D. J., Wang, R. X. F., & Roddenberry, D. (2002). Object recognition is mediated by extra-retinal information. Perception & Psychophysics, 64(4), 521–530. Simons, D. J., Wang, R. X. F., & Roddenberry, D. (2002). Object recognition is mediated by extra-retinal information. Perception & Psychophysics, 64(4), 521–530.
go back to reference Stevens, A., & Coupe, P. (1978). Distortions in judged spatial relations. Cognitive Psychology, 10, 422–437.PubMedCrossRef Stevens, A., & Coupe, P. (1978). Distortions in judged spatial relations. Cognitive Psychology, 10, 422–437.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Waller, D., Montello, D. R., Richardson, A. E., & Hegarty, M. (2002). Orientation specificity and spatial updating of memories for layouts. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory and Cognition, 28(6), 1051–1063.CrossRef Waller, D., Montello, D. R., Richardson, A. E., & Hegarty, M. (2002). Orientation specificity and spatial updating of memories for layouts. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory and Cognition, 28(6), 1051–1063.CrossRef
go back to reference Wang, R. F. (1999). Representing a stable environment by egocentric updating and invariant representations. Spatial Cognition and Computation, 1, 431–445.CrossRef Wang, R. F. (1999). Representing a stable environment by egocentric updating and invariant representations. Spatial Cognition and Computation, 1, 431–445.CrossRef
go back to reference Wang, R. F. (2005). Beyond imagination: Perspective change problems revisited. Psicologica, 26(1), 25–38. Wang, R. F. (2005). Beyond imagination: Perspective change problems revisited. Psicologica, 26(1), 25–38.
go back to reference Wang, R. F., & Spelke, E. S. (2002). Human spatial representation: Insights from animals. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(9), 376–382.PubMedCrossRef Wang, R. F., & Spelke, E. S. (2002). Human spatial representation: Insights from animals. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(9), 376–382.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wang, R. X. F., & Brockmole, J. R. (2003). Human navigation in nested environments. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory and Cognition, 29(3), 398–404.CrossRef Wang, R. X. F., & Brockmole, J. R. (2003). Human navigation in nested environments. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory and Cognition, 29(3), 398–404.CrossRef
go back to reference Wang, R. X. F., & Simons, D. J. (1999). Active and passive scene recognition across views. Cognition, 70(2), 191–210.PubMedCrossRef Wang, R. X. F., & Simons, D. J. (1999). Active and passive scene recognition across views. Cognition, 70(2), 191–210.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wang, R. X. F., & Spelke, E. S. (2000). Updating egocentric representations in human navigation. Cognition, 77(3), 215–250.PubMedCrossRef Wang, R. X. F., & Spelke, E. S. (2000). Updating egocentric representations in human navigation. Cognition, 77(3), 215–250.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Warren, R., & Wertheim, A. H. (Eds.) (1990). Perception & control of self-motion. New Jersey, London: Erlbaum. Warren, R., & Wertheim, A. H. (Eds.) (1990). Perception & control of self-motion. New Jersey, London: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Warren, W. H., Kay, B. A., Zosh, W. D., Duchon, A. P., & Sahuc, S. (2001). Optic flow is used to control human walking. Nature Neuroscience, 4(2), 213–216.PubMedCrossRef Warren, W. H., Kay, B. A., Zosh, W. D., Duchon, A. P., & Sahuc, S. (2001). Optic flow is used to control human walking. Nature Neuroscience, 4(2), 213–216.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wong, S. C. P., & Frost, B. J. (1981). The effect of visual-vestibular conflict on the latency of steadystate visually induced subjective rotation. Perception & Psychophysics, 30(3), 228–236. Wong, S. C. P., & Frost, B. J. (1981). The effect of visual-vestibular conflict on the latency of steadystate visually induced subjective rotation. Perception & Psychophysics, 30(3), 228–236.
go back to reference Wraga, M. (2003). Thinking outside the body: An advantage for spatial updating during imagined versus physical self-rotation. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory and Cognition, 29(5), 993–1005.CrossRef Wraga, M. (2003). Thinking outside the body: An advantage for spatial updating during imagined versus physical self-rotation. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory and Cognition, 29(5), 993–1005.CrossRef
go back to reference Wraga, M., Creem, S. H., & Proffitt, D. R. (2000). Updating displays after imagined object and viewer rotations. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory and Cognition, 26(1), 151–168.CrossRef Wraga, M., Creem, S. H., & Proffitt, D. R. (2000). Updating displays after imagined object and viewer rotations. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory and Cognition, 26(1), 151–168.CrossRef
go back to reference Wraga, M., Creem-Regehr, S. H., & Proffitt, D. R. (2004). Spatial updating of virtual displays during self and display rotation. Memory & Cognition, 32(3), 399–415. Wraga, M., Creem-Regehr, S. H., & Proffitt, D. R. (2004). Spatial updating of virtual displays during self and display rotation. Memory & Cognition, 32(3), 399–415.
go back to reference Yardley, L., & Higgins, M. (1998). Spatial updating during rotation: The role of vestibular information and mental activity. Journal of Vestibular Research. Equilibrium and Orientation, 8(6), 435–442. Yardley, L., & Higgins, M. (1998). Spatial updating during rotation: The role of vestibular information and mental activity. Journal of Vestibular Research. Equilibrium and Orientation, 8(6), 435–442.
Metagegevens
Titel
Spatial updating in virtual reality: the sufficiency of visual information
Auteurs
Bernhard E. Riecke
Douglas W. Cunningham
Heinrich H. Bülthoff
Publicatiedatum
01-05-2007
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 3/2007
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-006-0085-z

Andere artikelen Uitgave 3/2007

Psychological Research 3/2007 Naar de uitgave