Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 5/2007

01-09-2007 | Original Article

Item-specific congruency effects in nonverbal auditory Stroop

Auteurs: Launa C. Leboe, Todd A. Mondor

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 5/2007

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

In the current study, participants judged as ‘low’ or ‘high’ either the location or the frequency of a single tone presented in one of two locations at one of two frequencies. The classification associated with the irrelevant feature could be either congruent or incongruent with the required response. The results of Experiment 1 revealed that responses were made more slowly on incongruent than on congruent trials, regardless of whether participants judged sounds according to their location or their pitch. In Experiment 2, we manipulated the probability that the classification associated with the irrelevant acoustic feature was consistent with the classification associated with the task-relevant dimension. In this experiment responses were made more quickly on congruent trials when the response associated with the irrelevant feature was likely to be consistent with the required response, and on incongruent trials when the response associated with the irrelevant feature was likely to be inconsistent with the required response.
Literatuur
go back to reference Adam, J. J., Boon, B., Paas, F. G. W. C., & Umiltà, C. (1998). The up-right/down-left advantage for vertically oriented stimuli and horizontally oriented responses: A dual-strategy hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1582–1595CrossRef Adam, J. J., Boon, B., Paas, F. G. W. C., & Umiltà, C. (1998). The up-right/down-left advantage for vertically oriented stimuli and horizontally oriented responses: A dual-strategy hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1582–1595CrossRef
go back to reference Adobe Systems Incorporated (2005). Adobe Audition 1.5. San Jose, CA Adobe Systems Incorporated (2005). Adobe Audition 1.5. San Jose, CA
go back to reference Bauer, D. W., & Miller, J. (1982). Stimulus–response compatibility and the motor system. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 34A, 367–380 Bauer, D. W., & Miller, J. (1982). Stimulus–response compatibility and the motor system. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 34A, 367–380
go back to reference Besner, D., & Stolz, J. A. (1999). Unconsciously controlled processing: The Stroop effect reconsidered. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 6, 449–455PubMed Besner, D., & Stolz, J. A. (1999). Unconsciously controlled processing: The Stroop effect reconsidered. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 6, 449–455PubMed
go back to reference Broadbent, D. E. (1954). The role of auditory localization in attention and memory span. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47, 191–196PubMedCrossRef Broadbent, D. E. (1954). The role of auditory localization in attention and memory span. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47, 191–196PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cherry, C. (1953). Some experiments on the recognition of speech with one and with two ears. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 25, 975–979CrossRef Cherry, C. (1953). Some experiments on the recognition of speech with one and with two ears. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 25, 975–979CrossRef
go back to reference Cho, Y. S., & Proctor, R. W. (2004). Stimulu-set location does not affect orthogonal stimulus–response compatibility. Psychological Research, 69, 106–114PubMedCrossRef Cho, Y. S., & Proctor, R. W. (2004). Stimulu-set location does not affect orthogonal stimulus–response compatibility. Psychological Research, 69, 106–114PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cohen, J. D., Dunbar, K., & McClelland, J. L. (1990). On the control of automatic processes: A parallel distributed processing model of the Stroop effect. Psychological Review, 97, 332–361PubMedCrossRef Cohen, J. D., Dunbar, K., & McClelland, J. L. (1990). On the control of automatic processes: A parallel distributed processing model of the Stroop effect. Psychological Review, 97, 332–361PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Danziger, S., Estevez, A. F., & Mari-Beffa, P. (2002). Stroop interference effects in partially colored Stroop words. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9, 536–541PubMed Danziger, S., Estevez, A. F., & Mari-Beffa, P. (2002). Stroop interference effects in partially colored Stroop words. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9, 536–541PubMed
go back to reference Green, E. J., & Barber, P. J. (1981). An auditory Stroop effect with judgments of speaker gender. Perception and Psychophysics, 30, 459–466PubMed Green, E. J., & Barber, P. J. (1981). An auditory Stroop effect with judgments of speaker gender. Perception and Psychophysics, 30, 459–466PubMed
go back to reference Green, E. J., & Barber, P. J. (1983). Interference effects in an auditory Stroop task: Congruence and correspondence. Acta Psychologica, 53, 183–194PubMedCrossRef Green, E. J., & Barber, P. J. (1983). Interference effects in an auditory Stroop task: Congruence and correspondence. Acta Psychologica, 53, 183–194PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hall, M. D., & Blasko, D. G. (2005). Attentional interference in judgments of musical timbre: Individual differences in working memory. The Journal of General Psychology, 132, 94–112PubMedCrossRef Hall, M. D., & Blasko, D. G. (2005). Attentional interference in judgments of musical timbre: Individual differences in working memory. The Journal of General Psychology, 132, 94–112PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hamers, J. F., & Lambert, W. E. (1972). Bilingual interdependencies in auditory perception. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 11, 303–310CrossRef Hamers, J. F., & Lambert, W. E. (1972). Bilingual interdependencies in auditory perception. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 11, 303–310CrossRef
go back to reference Hintzman, D. L. (1976). Repetition and memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation (pp. 47–91). New York: Academic Hintzman, D. L. (1976). Repetition and memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation (pp. 47–91). New York: Academic
go back to reference Jacoby, L. L. (1978). On interpreting the effects of repetition: Solving a problem versus remembering a solution. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 17, 649–667CrossRef Jacoby, L. L. (1978). On interpreting the effects of repetition: Solving a problem versus remembering a solution. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 17, 649–667CrossRef
go back to reference Jacoby, L. L., & Brooks, L. R. (1984). Nonanalytic cognition: Memory, perception and concept formation. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 18, 1–47 Jacoby, L. L., & Brooks, L. R. (1984). Nonanalytic cognition: Memory, perception and concept formation. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 18, 1–47
go back to reference Jacoby, L. L., Lindsay, D. S., & Hessels, S. (2003). Item-specific control of automatic processes: Stroop process dissociations. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 10(3), 638–644PubMed Jacoby, L. L., Lindsay, D. S., & Hessels, S. (2003). Item-specific control of automatic processes: Stroop process dissociations. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 10(3), 638–644PubMed
go back to reference Kahneman, D., & Henik, A. (1981). Perceptual organization and attention. In M. Kubovy & J. R. Pomerantz (Eds.), Perceptual Organization (pp. 181–211). Hillsdale: Erlbaum Kahneman, D., & Henik, A. (1981). Perceptual organization and attention. In M. Kubovy & J. R. Pomerantz (Eds.), Perceptual Organization (pp. 181–211). Hillsdale: Erlbaum
go back to reference Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus–response compatibility—a model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97, 253–270PubMedCrossRef Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus–response compatibility—a model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97, 253–270PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference LaBerge, D., & Samuels, S. J. (1974). Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading. Cognitive Psychology, 6, 293–323CrossRef LaBerge, D., & Samuels, S. J. (1974). Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading. Cognitive Psychology, 6, 293–323CrossRef
go back to reference Lindsay, D. S., & Jacoby, L. L. (1994). Stroop process dissociations: The relationship between facilitation and interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 20, 219–234CrossRef Lindsay, D. S., & Jacoby, L. L. (1994). Stroop process dissociations: The relationship between facilitation and interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 20, 219–234CrossRef
go back to reference Logan, G. D. (1988). Toward an instance theory of automatization. Psychological Review, 95, 492–527CrossRef Logan, G. D. (1988). Toward an instance theory of automatization. Psychological Review, 95, 492–527CrossRef
go back to reference Logan, G. D. (2002). An instance theory of attention and memory. Psychological Review, 109, 376–400PubMedCrossRef Logan, G. D. (2002). An instance theory of attention and memory. Psychological Review, 109, 376–400PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Logan, G. D., Zbrodoff, N. J., & Williamson, J. (1984). Strategies in the color-word Stroop task. Bulletin of Psychonomic Society, 22, 135–138 Logan, G. D., Zbrodoff, N. J., & Williamson, J. (1984). Strategies in the color-word Stroop task. Bulletin of Psychonomic Society, 22, 135–138
go back to reference MacLeod, C. M. (1991). Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 163–203PubMedCrossRef MacLeod, C. M. (1991). Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 163–203PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference MacLeod, C. M. (1998). Training on integrated versus separated Stroop tasks: The progression of interference and facilitation. Memory & Cognition, 26, 201–211 MacLeod, C. M. (1998). Training on integrated versus separated Stroop tasks: The progression of interference and facilitation. Memory & Cognition, 26, 201–211
go back to reference MacLeod, C. M., & MacDonald, P. A. (2000). Interdimensional interference in the Stroop effect: uncovering the cognitive and neural anatomy of attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 383–391PubMedCrossRef MacLeod, C. M., & MacDonald, P. A. (2000). Interdimensional interference in the Stroop effect: uncovering the cognitive and neural anatomy of attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 383–391PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference MacLeod, C. M., & Dunbar, K. (1988). Training and Stroop-like interference: evidence for a continuum of automaticity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10, 304–315 MacLeod, C. M., & Dunbar, K. (1988). Training and Stroop-like interference: evidence for a continuum of automaticity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10, 304–315
go back to reference Martino, G., & Marks, L. E. (1999). Perceptual and linguistic interactions in speeded classification: Tests of semantic coding hypothesis. Perception, 28, 903–923PubMedCrossRef Martino, G., & Marks, L. E. (1999). Perceptual and linguistic interactions in speeded classification: Tests of semantic coding hypothesis. Perception, 28, 903–923PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Martino, G., & Marks, L. E. (2001). Synethesia: strong and weak. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 10, 61–65CrossRef Martino, G., & Marks, L. E. (2001). Synethesia: strong and weak. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 10, 61–65CrossRef
go back to reference McClain, L. (1983). Stimulus–response compatibility affects auditory Stroop interference. Perception & Psychophysics, 33, 266–270 McClain, L. (1983). Stimulus–response compatibility affects auditory Stroop interference. Perception & Psychophysics, 33, 266–270
go back to reference Mondor, T. A., Zatorre, R. J., & Terrio, N. A. (1998). Constraints on the selection of auditory information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 66–79CrossRef Mondor, T. A., Zatorre, R. J., & Terrio, N. A. (1998). Constraints on the selection of auditory information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 66–79CrossRef
go back to reference Morgan, A. L. R., & Brandt, J. F. (1989). An auditory Stroop effect for pitch, loudness, and time. Brain and Language, 36, 592–603PubMedCrossRef Morgan, A. L. R., & Brandt, J. F. (1989). An auditory Stroop effect for pitch, loudness, and time. Brain and Language, 36, 592–603PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Neely, J. H., & Kahan, T. A. (2001). Is semantic activation automatic? A critical re-evaluation. In H. L. Roediger III, J. S. Nairne, I. Neath, & A. M. Surprenant (Eds.), The nature of remembering: essays in honour of Robert G. Crowder (pp. 69–93). Washington: American Psychological AssociationCrossRef Neely, J. H., & Kahan, T. A. (2001). Is semantic activation automatic? A critical re-evaluation. In H. L. Roediger III, J. S. Nairne, I. Neath, & A. M. Surprenant (Eds.), The nature of remembering: essays in honour of Robert G. Crowder (pp. 69–93). Washington: American Psychological AssociationCrossRef
go back to reference Psychology Software Tools, Inc. (1999). E-Prime Software System. Pittsburg, PA Psychology Software Tools, Inc. (1999). E-Prime Software System. Pittsburg, PA
go back to reference Roe, W. T., Wilsoncroft, W. E., & Griffiths, R. S. (1980). Effects of motor and verbal practice on the Stroop task. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 50, 647–650PubMed Roe, W. T., Wilsoncroft, W. E., & Griffiths, R. S. (1980). Effects of motor and verbal practice on the Stroop task. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 50, 647–650PubMed
go back to reference Rusconi, E., Kwan, B., Giordano, B. L., Umiltà, C., & Butterworth, B. (2006). Spatial representations of pitch height: the SMARC effect. Cognition (in press) Rusconi, E., Kwan, B., Giordano, B. L., Umiltà, C., & Butterworth, B. (2006). Spatial representations of pitch height: the SMARC effect. Cognition (in press)
go back to reference Schmit, V., & Davis, R. (1974). The role of hemispheric specialization in the analysis of Stroop stimuli. Acta Psychologica, 38, 149–158PubMedCrossRef Schmit, V., & Davis, R. (1974). The role of hemispheric specialization in the analysis of Stroop stimuli. Acta Psychologica, 38, 149–158PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Simon, J. R., Mewaldt, S. P., Acosta, E., Jr., & Hu, J. (1976). Processing auditory information: Interaction of two population stereotypes. Journal of Applied Psychology, 61, 354–358PubMedCrossRef Simon, J. R., Mewaldt, S. P., Acosta, E., Jr., & Hu, J. (1976). Processing auditory information: Interaction of two population stereotypes. Journal of Applied Psychology, 61, 354–358PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662CrossRef Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662CrossRef
go back to reference Virzi, R. A., & Egeth, H. E. (1985). Toward a translational model of Stroop interference. Memory and Cognition, 13, 304–319 Virzi, R. A., & Egeth, H. E. (1985). Toward a translational model of Stroop interference. Memory and Cognition, 13, 304–319
go back to reference Warren, L. R., & Marsh, G. R. (1979). Changes in event related potentials during processing of Stroop stimuli. International Journal of Neuroscience, 9, 217–223PubMedCrossRef Warren, L. R., & Marsh, G. R. (1979). Changes in event related potentials during processing of Stroop stimuli. International Journal of Neuroscience, 9, 217–223PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Weeks, D. J., & Proctor, R. W. (1990). Salient-features coding in the translation between orthogonal stimulus–response dimensions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 119, 355–366CrossRef Weeks, D. J., & Proctor, R. W. (1990). Salient-features coding in the translation between orthogonal stimulus–response dimensions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 119, 355–366CrossRef
go back to reference Zhang, H., & Kornblum, S. (1998). The effects of stimulus–response mapping and irrelevant stimulus–response and stimulus–stimulus overlap in four-choice Stroop tasks with single-carrier stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 3–19PubMedCrossRef Zhang, H., & Kornblum, S. (1998). The effects of stimulus–response mapping and irrelevant stimulus–response and stimulus–stimulus overlap in four-choice Stroop tasks with single-carrier stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 3–19PubMedCrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Item-specific congruency effects in nonverbal auditory Stroop
Auteurs
Launa C. Leboe
Todd A. Mondor
Publicatiedatum
01-09-2007
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 5/2007
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-006-0049-3

Andere artikelen Uitgave 5/2007

Psychological Research 5/2007 Naar de uitgave