Skip to main content
Log in

Feature integration across perception and action: event files affect response choice

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Five experiments investigated the spontaneous integration of stimulus and response features. Participants performed simple, prepared responses (R1) to the mere presence of Go signals (S1) before carrying out another, freely chosen response (R2) to another stimulus (S2), the main question being whether the likelihood of repeating a response depends on whether or not the stimulus, or some of its features, are repeated. Indeed, participants were more likely to repeat the previous response if stimulus form or color was repeated than if it was alternated. The same was true for stimulus location, but only if location was made task-relevant, whether by defining the response set in terms of location, by requiring the report of S2 location, or by having S1 to be selected against a distractor. These findings suggest that task-relevant stimulus and response features are spontaneously integrated into independent, local event files, each linking one stimulus to one response feature. Upon reactivation of one member of the binary link activation is spread to the other, thereby increasing the likelihood to repeat a response if one or more stimulus features are repeated. These findings support the idea that both perceptual events and action plans are cognitively represented in terms of their features, and that feature-integration processes cross borders between perception and action.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Fig. 8
Fig. 9
Fig. 10
Fig. 11

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The basic idea underlying this (uncommon but not a priori unreasonable) consideration assumes that binding may not be obligatory or necessary for perception or identification to occur. Indeed, if only one event would be represented at a given time there would be no real binding problem: the different features of the event would be coded in different feature maps or systems, but they could still converge on a particular behavior, be that a conscious experience or an overt response, without being "tagged" or "bound". However, binding problems do come into play if more than one event is to be represented at a time, because this raises the question of which features belong to which event. Experiments 4 and 5 aimed at creating such problems.

  2. In the present study, these binary linkages showed up as main effects of stimulus-feature repetitions on the dependent variable response repetition. These main effects of form, color, and location repetition explained substantial portions of the variance: η2=0.18–0.43, η2=0.14–0.24, η2=0.11–0.31, and η2=0.11–0.19 in Experiments 2, 3, 4, and 5, respectively. The variance explained by two-way interactions between these three variables (which imply three-way interactions including response repetition) accounted for less variance without exception, η2=0.02–0.08, η2=0.001–0.04, η2=0.004–0.04, and η2=0.001–0.08, respectively, and the contribution from the three-way interaction (implying a four-way interaction involving all stimulus features and response repetition) was negligible, η2<0.02 in all cases.

References

  • Alexander, G. E. & Crutcher, M D. (1990). Preparation for movement: neural representations of intended direction in three motor areas of the monkey. Journal of Neurophysiology, 64, 133–150.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D. (1966). The capacity for generating information by randomization. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 119–129.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 577–609.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berlyne, D. E. (1957). Uncertainty and conflict: a point of contact between information-theory and behavior-theory concepts. Psychological Review, 64, 329–339.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bertelson, P. (1961). Sequential redundancy and speed in a serial two-choice responding task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 13, 90–102.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bonnet, M. & MacKay, W. A. (1989). Changes in contingent-negative variation and reaction time related to precueing of direction and force of a forearm movement. Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 33, 147–152.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bundesen, C. (1990). A theory of visual attention. Psychological Review, 97, 523–547.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. D., Dunbar, K., & McClelland, J. M. (1990). On the control of automatic processes: a parallel distributed processing account of the Stroop effect. Psychological Review, 97, 332–361.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. (1989). Time-locked multiregional retroactivation. Cognition, 33, 25–62.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DeYoe, E. A., & Van Essen, D. C. (1988). Concurrent processing streams in monkey visual cortex. Trends in Neuroscience, 11, 219–226.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, J. (1996). Cooperating brain systems in selective perception and action. In T. Inui & J. L. McClelland (Eds.). Attention and performance, vol XVI (pp. 549–578). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

  • Duncan, J. & Humphreys, G. W. (1989). Visual search and stimulus similarity. Psychological Review, 96, 433–458.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Elsner, B., & Hommel, B. (2001). Effect anticipation and action control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 229–240.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fagioli, S., Hommel, B., & Schubotz, R. I. (2005). Intentional control of attention: Action planning primes action-related stimulus dimensions. Psychological Research, this issue.

  • Folk, C. L., Remington, R. W., & Johnston, J. C. (1992). Involuntary covert orienting is contingent of attentional control settings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 1030–1044.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gehring, W. J., Goss, B., Coles, M. G. H., Meyer, D. E., & Donchin, E. (1993). A neural system for error detection and compensation. Psychological Science, 4, 385–390.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Georgopoulos, A. P. (1990). Neurophysiology of reaching. In M. Jeannerod (Ed.), Attention and performance XIII: motor representation and control (pp. 227–263). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B. (1996a). The cognitive representation of action: automatic integration of perceived action effects. Psychological Research, 59, 176–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B. (1996b). S–R compatibility effects without response uncertainty. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A, 546–571.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B. (1997). Toward an action-concept model of stimulus–response compatibility. In B. Hommel & W. Prinz (Eds.), Theoretical issues in stimulus–response compatibility (pp. 281–320). Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B. (1998). Event files: evidence for automatic integration of stimulus–response episodes. Visual Cognition, 5, 183–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B. (2002). Responding to object files: automatic integration of spatial information revealed by stimulus–response compatibility effects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55A, 567–580.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B. (2004). Event files: feature binding in and across perception and action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 494–500.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B., & Colzato, L. S. (2004). Visual attention and the temporal dynamics of feature integration. Visual Cognition, 11, 483–521.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–878.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B., Pösse, B., & Waszak, F. (2000). Contextualization in perception and action. Psychologica Belgica, 40, 227–245.

    Google Scholar 

  • Houghton, G., & Tipper, S. P., (1994). A model of inhibitory mechanisms in selective attention. In D. Dagenbach & T. Carr (Eds.). Inhibitory mechanisms in attention memory an language. San Diego: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hyman, R. (1953). Stimulus information as a determinant of reaction time. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 45, 188–196.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., Treisman, A., & Gibbs, B. (1992). The reviewing of object files: object-specific integration of information. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 175–219.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kalaska, J. F. & Hyde, M. L. (1985). Area 4 and area 5: differences between the load direction-dependent discharge variability of cells during active postural fixation. Experimental Brain Research, 59, 197–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirby, N. (1980). Sequential effects in choice reaction time. In A. T. Welford (Ed.), Reaction times (pp. 129–172). London: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kornblum, S. (1973). Sequential effects in choice RT: a tutorial review. In S. Kornblum (Ed.). Attention & performance: vol IV (pp. 259–288). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

  • Kornblum, S., & Stevens, G. (2002). Sequential effects of dimensional overlap: findings and issues. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.). Common mechanisms in perception and action: attention & performance: vol XIX (pp. 9–54). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  • Kutas, M. & Donchin, E. (1980). Preparation to respond as manifested by movement-related brain potentials. Brain Research, 202, 95–115.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Memelink, J., & Hommel, B. (2005). Tailoring perception and action to the task at hand. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology. in press.

  • Lavie, N. (1995). Perceptual load as a necessary condition for selective attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 451–468.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leslie, A. M., Xu, F., Tremoulet, P. D., & Sholl, B. J. (1998). Indexing and the object concept: developing ’what’ and ’where’ systems. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 10–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayr, U., & Bryck, R. L. (2005). Low-level constraints on high-level selection: switching tasks with and without switching stimulus/response objects. Psychological Research, this issue.

  • Milliken, B., & Lupiáñez, J. (2005). Repetition costs in word identification: evaluating a stimulus–response integration account. Psychological Research, this issue.

  • Odom, R. D. (1972). Effects of perceptual salience on the recall of relevant and incidental dimensional values: a developmental study. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 92, 285–291.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Osman, A., Bashore, T. R., Coles, M. G. H., Donchin, E. & Meyer, D. E. (1992). On the transmission of partial information: inferences from movement-related brain research. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 217–232.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Phaf, R. H., Van der Heijden, A. H. C., & Hudson, P. T. W. (1990). SLAM: a connectionist model for attention in visual selection tasks. Cognitive Psychology, 22, 273–341.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 2–25.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pratt, J., & Hommel, B. (2003). Symbolic control of visual attention: the role of working memory and attentional control settings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 835–845.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raffone, A., & Wolters, G. (2001). A cortical mechanism for binding in visual working memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 766–785.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Riehle, A. & Requin, J. (1989). Monkey primary motor and premotor cortex: single-cell activity related to prior information about direction and extent of an intended movement. Journal of Neurophysiology, 61, 534–549.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbaum, D. A. (1987). Successive approximations to a model of human motor programming. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 21, 153–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbaum, D. A., Weber, R. J., Hazelett, W. M. & Hindorff, V. (1986). The parameter remapping effect in human performance: evidence from tongue twisters and finger fumblers. Journal of Memory and Language, 25, 710–725.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Soetens, E., Boer, L. C., & Hueting, J. E. (1985). Expectancy or automatic facilitation? Separating sequential effects in two-choice reaction time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 11, 598–616.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stoet, G., & Hommel, B. (1999). Action planning and the temporal binding of response codes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1625–1640.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stoet, G., & Hommel, B. (2002). Interaction between feature binding in perception and action. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.). Common mechanisms in perception and action: attention & performance: vol XIX (pp. 538–552). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  • Stoffer, T. H., & Yakin, A. R. (1994). The functional role of attention for spatial coding on the Simon effect. Psychological Research, 53, 127–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treisman, A. (1988). Features and objects. In The 14th Bartlett Memorial Lecture. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40A, 201–237.

  • Treisman, A. (1996). The binding problem. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 6, 171–178.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Treisman, A. M., & Gelade, G. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97–136.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tune, G. S. (1964). A brief survey of variables that influence random generation. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 18, 705–710.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ungerleider, L. G., & Haxby, J. V. (1994). ‘What’ and ‘where’ in the human brain. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 4, 157–165.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van der Heijden, A. H. C. (1993). The role of position in object selection in vision. Psychological Research, 56, 44–58.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vidal, F., Bonnet, M. & Macar, F. (1991). Programming response duration in a precueing reaction time paradigm. Journal of Motor Behavior, 23, 226–234.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wenke, D., Gaschler, R., & Nattkemper, D. (2005). Instruction-induced feature binding. Psychological Research, this issue.

  • Wickens, J., Hyland, B. & Anson, G. (1994). Cortical cell assemblies: a possible mechanism for motor programs. Journal of Motor Behavior, 26, 66–82.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wolfe, J. M. (1994). Guided Search 2.0: a revised model of visual search. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 1, 202–238.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolfe, J. M. (1998). Visual search. In H. Pashler (Ed.). Attention (pp. 13–74). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Part of this research was carried out at and supported by the Max Planck Institute for Psychological Research, Cognition & Action, in Munich, Germany, and other parts were supported by a grant from the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft. I wish to thank Matt Crump, Jason Leboe, and Bruce Milliken for comments on previous versions of this paper, and Patrizia Falkenberg, Alexandra Heinrich, and Nicola Korherr at the Max Planck, and Tijmen Moerland, Shalu Saini, and Menno van der Woude at Leiden University for running the experiments and collecting the data.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Bernhard Hommel.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Hommel, B. Feature integration across perception and action: event files affect response choice. Psychological Research 71, 42–63 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-005-0035-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-005-0035-1

Keywords

Navigation