Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 6/2006

01-11-2006 | Original Article

Processing of irrelevant location information under dual-task conditions

Auteurs: Jochen Müsseler, Peter Wühr, Carlo Umiltá

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 6/2006

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

This study deals with the problem of whether the processing of irrelevant location information in Simon-like tasks is triggered exogenously or endogenously. In Experiment 1, the primary task required one to press, as fast as possible, a left-hand-side key or a right-hand-side key (R1) to the pitch of a tone that was presented binaurally (S1). The secondary task required identifying, without time constraints, a visual stimulus (S2) that appeared randomly to the left or right of screen center. Results showed that there was a correspondence (i.e., a cross-task Simon effect) between the location of R1 and the location of S2 when S2 was presented alone. The cross-task Simon effect became much smaller (and in-significant) when a noise stimulus was presented contralateral to S2. Experiment 2 was similar to Experiment 1, except that S2 appeared unpredictably in only one-third of the trials. Results of Experiment 2 closely replicated those of Experiment 1: the cross-task Simon effect was much greater when S2 was presented alone. Experiment 3 differed from Experiment 1 because S2 had to be processed in only one-third of the trials, in which its identity was to be reported. In the remaining two-thirds of the trials, participants could ignore S2. Results confirmed that the cross-task Simon effect was much greater when S2 was presented alone. In contrast, it did not matter whether S2 had to be processed or not. In conclusion, the present study supports the hypothesis that the task-irrelevant spatial code of the stimulus is formed automatically, likely through an exogenously triggered selection. The role of endogenously initiated selection, if any, is much less important.
Literatuur
go back to reference Acosta, E., & Simon, J. R. (1976). The effect of irrelevant information on the stages of processing. Journal of Motor Behavior, 8, 181–187. Acosta, E., & Simon, J. R. (1976). The effect of irrelevant information on the stages of processing. Journal of Motor Behavior, 8, 181–187.
go back to reference Brainard, D. H. (1997). The Psychophysics toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10, 433–436.PubMed Brainard, D. H. (1997). The Psychophysics toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10, 433–436.PubMed
go back to reference Hedge, A., & Marsh, N. W. (1975). The effect of irrelevant spatial correspondence on two-choice response-time. Acta Psychologica, 39, 427–439.CrossRefPubMed Hedge, A., & Marsh, N. W. (1975). The effect of irrelevant spatial correspondence on two-choice response-time. Acta Psychologica, 39, 427–439.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1993). The role of attention for the Simon effect. Psychological Research, 55, 208–222.CrossRefPubMed Hommel, B. (1993). The role of attention for the Simon effect. Psychological Research, 55, 208–222.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1995). Conflict versus misguided search as explanation of S-R correspondence effects. Acta Psychologica, 89, 37–51.CrossRef Hommel, B. (1995). Conflict versus misguided search as explanation of S-R correspondence effects. Acta Psychologica, 89, 37–51.CrossRef
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1997). Toward an action concept model of stimulus-response compatibility. In B. Hommel & W. Prinz (Eds.), Theoretical issues on stimulus-response compatibility (pp. 281–320). Amsterdam: Elsevier. Hommel, B. (1997). Toward an action concept model of stimulus-response compatibility. In B. Hommel & W. Prinz (Eds.), Theoretical issues on stimulus-response compatibility (pp. 281–320). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
go back to reference Hommel, B., & Lippa, Y. (1995). S-R compatibility effects due to context-dependent spatial stimulus coding. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2, 370–374. Hommel, B., & Lippa, Y. (1995). S-R compatibility effects due to context-dependent spatial stimulus coding. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2, 370–374.
go back to reference Jolicœur, P., Sessa, P., Dell’Acqua, R., & Robitaille, N. (2005). On the control of visual spatial attention: Evidence from human electrophysiology. Psychological Research, this issue. Jolicœur, P., Sessa, P., Dell’Acqua, R., & Robitaille, N. (2005). On the control of visual spatial attention: Evidence from human electrophysiology. Psychological Research, this issue.
go back to reference Jonides, J. (1981). Voluntary versus automatic control over the mind’s eye’s movement. In J. B. Long & A. D. Baddeley (Eds.), Attention and Performance IX (pp. 187–203). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Jonides, J. (1981). Voluntary versus automatic control over the mind’s eye’s movement. In J. B. Long & A. D. Baddeley (Eds.), Attention and Performance IX (pp. 187–203). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Jonides, J., & Yantis, S. (1988). Uniqueness of abrupt visual onset in capturing attention. Perception and Psychophysics, 43, 346–354.PubMed Jonides, J., & Yantis, S. (1988). Uniqueness of abrupt visual onset in capturing attention. Perception and Psychophysics, 43, 346–354.PubMed
go back to reference Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility. A model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97, 253–270. Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility. A model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97, 253–270.
go back to reference Lu, C. H., & Proctor, R. W. (1995). The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2, 174–207. Lu, C. H., & Proctor, R. W. (1995). The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2, 174–207.
go back to reference Müsseler, J., Wühr, P., Danielmeier, C., & Zysset, S. (2004). Action-induced blindness with lateralized stimuli and responses. Experimental Brain Research, 160, 214–222CrossRef Müsseler, J., Wühr, P., Danielmeier, C., & Zysset, S. (2004). Action-induced blindness with lateralized stimuli and responses. Experimental Brain Research, 160, 214–222CrossRef
go back to reference Müsseler, J., Koch, I., & Wühr, P. (2005). Testing the boundary conditions for processing irrelevant location information: The cross-task Simon effect. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology (in press) Müsseler, J., Koch, I., & Wühr, P. (2005). Testing the boundary conditions for processing irrelevant location information: The cross-task Simon effect. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology (in press)
go back to reference Nicoletti, R., & Umiltá, C. (1994). Attention shifts produce spatial stimulus codes. Psychological Research, 56(3), 144–150.CrossRefPubMed Nicoletti, R., & Umiltá, C. (1994). Attention shifts produce spatial stimulus codes. Psychological Research, 56(3), 144–150.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Notebaert, W., & Soetens, E. (2003). Irrelevant auditory attention shifts prime corresponding responses. Psychological Research, 67, 253–260.CrossRefPubMed Notebaert, W., & Soetens, E. (2003). Irrelevant auditory attention shifts prime corresponding responses. Psychological Research, 67, 253–260.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Pelli, D. G. (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: Transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision, 10, 437–442.PubMed Pelli, D. G. (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: Transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision, 10, 437–442.PubMed
go back to reference Proctor, R. W., & Lu, C.-H. (1994). Referential coding and attention-shifting accounts of the Simon effect. Psychological Research, 56, 185–195.CrossRefPubMed Proctor, R. W., & Lu, C.-H. (1994). Referential coding and attention-shifting accounts of the Simon effect. Psychological Research, 56, 185–195.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Rubichi, S., Nicoletti, R., Iani, C., & Umiltà, C. (1997). The Simon effect occurs relative to the direction of an attention shift. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 23, 1353–1364.CrossRef Rubichi, S., Nicoletti, R., Iani, C., & Umiltà, C. (1997). The Simon effect occurs relative to the direction of an attention shift. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 23, 1353–1364.CrossRef
go back to reference Simon, J., & Berbaum, K. (1990). Effect of conflicting cues on information processing: The ’Stroop effect’ vs. the ’Simon effect. ’Acta Psychologica, 73, 159–170.CrossRefPubMed Simon, J., & Berbaum, K. (1990). Effect of conflicting cues on information processing: The ’Stroop effect’ vs. the ’Simon effect. ’Acta Psychologica, 73, 159–170.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Stoffer, T. H. (1991). Attentional focussing and spatial stimulus-response compatibility. Psychological Research, 53, 127–135.CrossRefPubMed Stoffer, T. H. (1991). Attentional focussing and spatial stimulus-response compatibility. Psychological Research, 53, 127–135.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Stoffer, T. H., & Umilta, C. (1997). Spatial stimulus coding and the focus of attention in S-R compatibility and the Simon effect. In B. Hommel & W. Prinz (Eds.), Theoretical issues on stimulus-response compatibility (pp. 181–208). Amsterdam: Elsevier. Stoffer, T. H., & Umilta, C. (1997). Spatial stimulus coding and the focus of attention in S-R compatibility and the Simon effect. In B. Hommel & W. Prinz (Eds.), Theoretical issues on stimulus-response compatibility (pp. 181–208). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
go back to reference Umiltà, C. (2000). Visuospatial attention. In F. Boller & J. Grafman (Eds.), Handbook of Neuropsychology (Vol. 1, pp. 339–425). Amsterdam: Elsevier. Umiltà, C. (2000). Visuospatial attention. In F. Boller & J. Grafman (Eds.), Handbook of Neuropsychology (Vol. 1, pp. 339–425). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
go back to reference Umiltà, C., & Nicoletti, R. (1990). Spatial S-R compatibility. In R. W. Proctor & T. G. Reeve (Eds.), Stimulus-response compatibility: An integrated perspective (pp. 89–116). Amsterdam: Elsevier. Umiltà, C., & Nicoletti, R. (1990). Spatial S-R compatibility. In R. W. Proctor & T. G. Reeve (Eds.), Stimulus-response compatibility: An integrated perspective (pp. 89–116). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
go back to reference Umiltà, C., Nicoletti, R., Simion, F., Tagliabue, M. E., & Bagnara, S. (1992). The cost of a strategy. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 4, 21–40. Umiltà, C., Nicoletti, R., Simion, F., Tagliabue, M. E., & Bagnara, S. (1992). The cost of a strategy. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 4, 21–40.
go back to reference Zorzi, M., & Umiltà, C. (1995). A computational model of the Simon effect. Psychological Research, 58, 193–205.CrossRefPubMed Zorzi, M., & Umiltà, C. (1995). A computational model of the Simon effect. Psychological Research, 58, 193–205.CrossRefPubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Processing of irrelevant location information under dual-task conditions
Auteurs
Jochen Müsseler
Peter Wühr
Carlo Umiltá
Publicatiedatum
01-11-2006
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 6/2006
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-005-0010-x

Andere artikelen Uitgave 6/2006

Psychological Research 6/2006 Naar de uitgave