Skip to main content
Log in

Recognizing one’s own clapping: The role of temporal cues

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Do individuals recognize their own clapping? We investigated this question with an off-line paradigm in which the perception of the claps is temporally separated from their generation. In a first experiment, we demonstrated that acoustic recordings of clapping provide sufficient information for discriminating between one’s own performance and that of another participant. Self-recognition was still preserved when the claps were reproduced by uniform tones that only retained their temporal pattern. In a subsequent experiment, we verified that the general tempo provides an important cue for self-recognition. Finally, we showed that self-recognition does not rely on concurrent synchronization of actual movements with the auditory sequences. The results are discussed in the light of current theories about perception-action links.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. We are grateful to Bruno Repp for suggesting this interpretation. In Repp’s (1987) study, the participants were not asked to maintain a constant tempo in the recording session. In addition, they were not asked to accompany the recorded claps with synchronization responses in the test session.

  2. We determined the general tempo of a sequence by the average of all complete sequence periods. The accuracy of sequence reproduction was determined by the ratios of adjacent intervals. These observed ratios were then compared with the expected ratios, as indicated in Fig. 1, by calculating the differences, squaring them, adding up the squared differences, and deriving the square root of the sum. This variable error has the advantage that it is logically independent of the differences in general tempo. Next, we assessed the reliability of these inter-individual difference measures. Overall correlations of these measures between the two sessions as well as individual comparisons for each participant pair showed that the general tempo, but not the accuracy, of sequence reproduction was a reliable inter-individual difference measure. A standard regression analysis of the self-recognition measure revealed only general tempo to be a significant predictor. Pairs of participants who showed large differences in general tempo were more likely to recognize their own clapping sequences than pairs of participants who did not show large differences in general tempo.

  3. In contrast to Experiment 1, Experiment 2 revealed a significant effect of authorship on synchronization performance. Participants were less variable when synchronizing with self-generated sequences than when synchronizing with other sequences. Moreover, the more variable the participant’s synchronization performance was with regard to the other participant’s sequences than with regard to his or her own sequences, the more often the self-recognition judgments were correct. The fact that the analysis of the synchronization performance did not reveal more significant effects of authorship may be rooted in the fact that the overall synchronization performance was rather variable and error-prone. This fact was especially challenging for the pre-analysis of the synchronization data. Nonetheless, the analysis of the synchronization data reliably showed significant block effects for the constant and the variable error of synchronization, decreasing with increasing practice.

References

  • Decety, J., & Jeannerod, M. (1996). Mentally simulated movements in virtual reality: Does Fitt’s law hold in motor imagery. Behavioural Brain Research, 72, 127–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Drake, C., Penel, A., & Bigand, E. (2000). Tapping in time with mechanically and expressively performed music. Music Perception, 18, 1–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flach, R., Knoblich, G., & Prinz, W. (2003). Off-line authorship effects in action perception. Brain & Cognition, 53, 503–513.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fraisse, P. (1982). Rhythm and tempo. In D. Deutsch (Ed.), The Psychology of Music (pp. 149–180). New York, Academic Press.

  • Gérard, C., & Rosenfeld, M. (1995). Pratique musicale et régulations temporelles [Musical practice and temporal control]. L’Année psychologique, 95, 571–591.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G. (1970). Sensory feedback mechanisms in performance control: With special reference to the ideo-motor mechanism. Psychological Review, 77, 73–99.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC). A framework for perception and action. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 24, 849–937.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ivry, R. B., & Hazeltine, E. (1995). Perception and production of temporal intervals across a range of durations: Evidence for a common timing mechanism. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 3–18.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ivry, R. B., & Keele, S. W. (1989). Timing functions of the cerebellum. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 1, 136–152.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keele, S. W., Pokorny, R. A., Corcos, D. M., & Ivry, R. (1985). Do perception and motor production share common timing mechanisms: A correlational analysis. Acta Psychologica, 60, 173–191.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Knoblich, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). Recognition of self-generated actions from kinematic displays of drawing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 456–465.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Knoblich, G., Seigerschmidt, E., Flach, R., & Prinz, W. (2002). Authorship effects in the prediction of handwriting strokes: Evidence for action simulation during action perception. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55, 1027–1046.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parncutt, R. (1994). A perceptual model of pulse salience and metrical accent in musical rhythms. Music Perception, 11, 409–464.

    Google Scholar 

  • Povel, D.-J. (1981). Internal representation of simple temporal patterns. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7, 3–18.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9, 129–154.

    Google Scholar 

  • Repp, B. (1987). The sound of two hands clapping: An exploratory study. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 81, 1100–1109.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rimoldi, H. J. A. (1951). Personal tempo. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 46, 283–303.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sirigu, A., Duhamel, J.-R., Cohen, L., Pillon, B., Dubois, B., & Agid, Y. (1996). The mental representation of hand movements after parietal damage. Science, 273, 1564–1568.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, J. (1982). Reproduction and representation of musical rhythms: The effects of musical skill. In D. Rogers & J. A. Sloboda (Eds.), The acquisition of symbolic skills (pp. 273–282). New York, Plenum.

  • Smoll, F. L. (1975). Preferred tempo of motor performance: Individual differences in within-individual variability. Journal of Motor Behavior, 7, 259–263.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smoll, F. L., & Schutz, R. W. (1978). Relationships among measures of preferred tempo and motor rhythm. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 46, 883–894.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Treisman, M., Faulkner, A., Naish, P. L. N., & Brogan, D. (1990). The internal clock: Evidence for a temporal oscillator underlying time perception with some estimates of its characteristic frequency. Perception, 19, 705–743.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Treisman, M., Faulkner, A., & Naish, P. L. N. (1992). On the relation between time perception and the timing of motor action: Evidence for a temporal oscillator controlling the timing of movement. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 45A, 255–263.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vorberg, D., & Wing, A. (1996). Modeling variability and dependence in timing. In H. Heuer & S. W. Keele (Eds.), Handbook of Perception and Action (vol. 2, pp. 181–262). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

  • Zelaznik, H. N., Spencer, R. M., & Doffin, J. G. (2000). Temporal precision in tapping and circle drawing movements at preferred rates is not correlated: Further evidence against timing as a general-purpose ability. Journal of Motor Behavior, 32, 193–199.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Rüdiger Flach, Günther Knoblich and Wolfgang Prinz, Arbeitsgruppe Kognition & Handlung. We thank Patric Bach, Jana Hiller, and Nadja Hristova for their help in collecting the data.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Rüdiger Flach.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Flach, R., Knoblich, G. & Prinz, W. Recognizing one’s own clapping: The role of temporal cues. Psychological Research 69, 147–156 (2004). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-003-0165-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-003-0165-2

Keywords

Navigation