Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 3/2003

01-08-2003 | Original Article

Temporal response-effect compatibility

Auteur: Wilfried Kunde

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 3/2003

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract.

This study investigated the impact of duration-varying response effects on the generation and execution of duration-varying responses. Participants performed short or long keypresses which produced auditory effects of corresponding duration (short response ->short tone, long response ->long tone) or of noncorresponding duration (short response ->long tone, long response ->short tone). Experiment 1 revealed faster responding with a corresponding than with a noncorresponding Response-Effect (R-E) mapping; that is, a temporal R-E compatibility effect. Additionally, increasing effect duration increased response latencies, whereas it decreased keypress duration. Experiment 2 showed that the influence of temporal R-E compatibility persists even when responses are cued in advance, suggesting that at least part of it originates from response generation processes occurring later than a traditional response selection stage. These findings corroborate and complement effect-based theories of action control which assume that the selection, initiation, and execution of movements is mediated by anticipation of their sensory effects.
Voetnoten
1
Whereas dit responses profited considerably from a compatible (short) tone in comparison to an incompatible (long) tone, dah responses profited only marginally from a compatible (long) tone in comparison to an incompatible (short) tone. It is thus tempting to speculate that the R-E compatibility effect is stronger for dit than for dah reactions. Yet, such an inference is not possible. Because of potential baseline differences between individual responses or response effects, the only valid measure for a compatibility effect is the interaction between them, which includes all responses and effects and thereby eliminates all baseline differences between them. It is therefore not meaningful to asses a compatibility influence for either a single stimulus, response, or response effect in isolation (cf. Kornblum & Lee, 1995, p.860 for a comprehensive discussion of this methodical issue).
 
2
This observation additionally provides evidence against a perceptual explanation of R-E compatibility effects: One might argue that the stimuli in the present experiments acquire the meaning of the responses they are assigned to (e.g. a red stimulus may become "short" because it required a short response). The present results may thus be construed as a kind of (acquired) S-R compatibility effect rather than an R-E compatibility effect (cf. Hasbroucq & Guiard, 1991). Previous research has already rejected this account by demonstrating R-E compatibility effects without response-specific stimuli, which makes the acquisition of response-specific stimulus meaning impossible (cf. Kunde, 2001a, Experiment 3). Moreover, the informative cues in the present study were 100% valid, turning the subsequent stimuli functionally into GO-Signals. Stimulus color was thus irrelevant and with all likelihood ignored. A perceptual explanation faces serious problems in explaining why R-E compatibility effects persist even 2000 ms after cue presentation, i.e. long after the perceptual analysis of the cue has finished.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Abrams, R.A. & Balota, D.A. (1991). Mental Chronometry: Beyond reaction time. Psychological Science, 2, 153–157. Abrams, R.A. & Balota, D.A. (1991). Mental Chronometry: Beyond reaction time. Psychological Science, 2, 153–157.
go back to reference Ansorge, U. (2002). Spatial intention-response compatibility. Acta Psychologica, 109. 285–299. Ansorge, U. (2002). Spatial intention-response compatibility. Acta Psychologica, 109. 285–299.
go back to reference Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (1997). Delayed auditory feedback in synchronization. Journal of Motor Behavior, 29(1) 35–46. Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (1997). Delayed auditory feedback in synchronization. Journal of Motor Behavior, 29(1) 35–46.
go back to reference Aschersleben, G., Stenneken, P., Cole, J., & Prinz, W. (2002). Timing mechanisms in sensorimotor synchronization. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Common mechanisms in perception and action. Attention and Performance XIX (pp. 227–244). Cambridge: MIT Press. Aschersleben, G., Stenneken, P., Cole, J., & Prinz, W. (2002). Timing mechanisms in sensorimotor synchronization. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Common mechanisms in perception and action. Attention and Performance XIX (pp. 227–244). Cambridge: MIT Press.
go back to reference Desmurget, M. & Grafton, S. (2000). Forward modelling allows feedback control for fast reaching movements. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 423–431. Desmurget, M. & Grafton, S. (2000). Forward modelling allows feedback control for fast reaching movements. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 423–431.
go back to reference Fitts, P. M., & Seeger, C. M. (1953). S-R compatibility: spatial characteristics of stimulus and response codes. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46 199–210. Fitts, P. M., & Seeger, C. M. (1953). S-R compatibility: spatial characteristics of stimulus and response codes. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46 199–210.
go back to reference Glover, S. & Dixon, P. (2001). Dynamic illusion effects in a reaching task: Evidence for separate visual representations in the planning and control of reaching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(3): 560–572. Glover, S. & Dixon, P. (2001). Dynamic illusion effects in a reaching task: Evidence for separate visual representations in the planning and control of reaching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(3): 560–572.
go back to reference Greenwald, A. G. (1970). Sensory feedback mechanisms in performance control: With special reference to the ideo-motor mechanism. Psychological Review. 77(2) 73–99. Greenwald, A. G. (1970). Sensory feedback mechanisms in performance control: With special reference to the ideo-motor mechanism. Psychological Review. 77(2) 73–99.
go back to reference Grosjean, M. & Mordkoff, T. (2001). Temporal Stimulus-Response compatibility. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 27. 870–878. Grosjean, M. & Mordkoff, T. (2001). Temporal Stimulus-Response compatibility. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 27. 870–878.
go back to reference Hasbroucq, T., & Guiard, Y. (1991). Stimulus-response compatibility and the Simon effect: Toward a conceptual clarification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 17(1) 246–266. Hasbroucq, T., & Guiard, Y. (1991). Stimulus-response compatibility and the Simon effect: Toward a conceptual clarification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 17(1) 246–266.
go back to reference Henry, F. M., & Rogers, D. E. (1960). Increased response latency for complicated movements and a "memory drum" theory of neuromotor reaction. Research Quarterly of the American Association for Health, Physical Education, and Recreation, 31, 448–458. Henry, F. M., & Rogers, D. E. (1960). Increased response latency for complicated movements and a "memory drum" theory of neuromotor reaction. Research Quarterly of the American Association for Health, Physical Education, and Recreation, 31, 448–458.
go back to reference Hoffmann, J. (1993). Vorhersage und Erkenntnis [Anticipation and Cognition]. Göttingen: Hogrefe. Hoffmann, J. (1993). Vorhersage und Erkenntnis [Anticipation and Cognition]. Göttingen: Hogrefe.
go back to reference Hoffmann, J., Sebald, A., & Stöcker, C. (2001). Irrelevant response effects improve serial learning in Serial Reaction Time tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27(2) 470–482. Hoffmann, J., Sebald, A., & Stöcker, C. (2001). Irrelevant response effects improve serial learning in Serial Reaction Time tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27(2) 470–482.
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1993). Inverting the Simon effect by intention: Determinants of direction and extent of effects of irrelevant spatial information. Psychological Research, 55(4) 270–279. Hommel, B. (1993). Inverting the Simon effect by intention: Determinants of direction and extent of effects of irrelevant spatial information. Psychological Research, 55(4) 270–279.
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1995). Stimulus-response compatibility and the Simon-Effect: Toward an empirical clarification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(4), 764–775. Hommel, B. (1995). Stimulus-response compatibility and the Simon-Effect: Toward an empirical clarification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(4), 764–775.
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1996a). The cognitive representation of action: Automatic integration of perceived action effects. Psychological Research 59(3) 176–186. Hommel, B. (1996a). The cognitive representation of action: Automatic integration of perceived action effects. Psychological Research 59(3) 176–186.
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1996b). S-R compatibility effects without response uncertainty. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 49A(3) 546–571. Hommel, B. (1996b). S-R compatibility effects without response uncertainty. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 49A(3) 546–571.
go back to reference Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). A Theory of Event Coding (TEC): A Framework for Percpetion and Action Planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–937. Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). A Theory of Event Coding (TEC): A Framework for Percpetion and Action Planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–937.
go back to reference Hommel, B. & W. Prinz (1997) Theoretical issues in stimulus-response compatibility. Amsterdam: North Holland: Elsevier. Hommel, B. & W. Prinz (1997) Theoretical issues in stimulus-response compatibility. Amsterdam: North Holland: Elsevier.
go back to reference Ivry, R. (1986). Force and timing components of the motor program. Journal of Motor Behavior. 18. 449–474. Ivry, R. (1986). Force and timing components of the motor program. Journal of Motor Behavior. 18. 449–474.
go back to reference James, W. (1981 (orig. 1890)). The principles of psychology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. James, W. (1981 (orig. 1890)). The principles of psychology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
go back to reference Klapp, S. T. (1995). Motor response programming during simple choice reaction time: The role of practice. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(5) 1015–1027. Klapp, S. T. (1995). Motor response programming during simple choice reaction time: The role of practice. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(5) 1015–1027.
go back to reference Koch, I. & Kunde, W. (2003) Verbal response-effect compatibility. Memory and Cognition (in press). Koch, I. & Kunde, W. (2003) Verbal response-effect compatibility. Memory and Cognition (in press).
go back to reference Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulusesponse compatibility: A model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97(2) 253–270. Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulusesponse compatibility: A model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97(2) 253–270.
go back to reference Kornblum, S. & Lee, J.W. (1995). Stimulus-Response Compatibility with relevant and irrelevant stimulus dimensions that do and do not overlap with the response. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 21. 855–875. Kornblum, S. & Lee, J.W. (1995). Stimulus-Response Compatibility with relevant and irrelevant stimulus dimensions that do and do not overlap with the response. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 21. 855–875.
go back to reference Kunde, W. (2001a). Response-effect compatibility in manual choice reaction tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 27, (2), 387–394. Kunde, W. (2001a). Response-effect compatibility in manual choice reaction tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 27, (2), 387–394.
go back to reference Kunde, W. (2001b). Exploring the hyphen in ideo-motor action. Commentary in Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 24, 891–892. Kunde, W. (2001b). Exploring the hyphen in ideo-motor action. Commentary in Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 24, 891–892.
go back to reference Kunde, W. & Stöcker, C. (2002). A Simon effect for Stimulus-Response Duration. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. Section A: Human Experimental Psychology. 55 (A). 581–592. Kunde, W. & Stöcker, C. (2002). A Simon effect for Stimulus-Response Duration. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. Section A: Human Experimental Psychology. 55 (A). 581–592.
go back to reference Kunde, W., Hoffmann, J. & Zellmann, P. (2002). The impact of anticipated action effects on action planning. Acta Psychologica. 109 (2). 137–155. Kunde, W., Hoffmann, J. & Zellmann, P. (2002). The impact of anticipated action effects on action planning. Acta Psychologica. 109 (2). 137–155.
go back to reference Kunde, W., Koch, I., & Hoffmann, J. (2003). Anticipated action effects affect the selection, initiation and execution of actions. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. Section A. Human Experimental Psychology (in press). Kunde, W., Koch, I., & Hoffmann, J. (2003). Anticipated action effects affect the selection, initiation and execution of actions. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. Section A. Human Experimental Psychology (in press).
go back to reference Mordkoff, T., Miller, J., & Roch, A-C. (1996). Absence of coactivation in the motor component: Evidence from psychophysiological measures of target detection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22, 25–41. Mordkoff, T., Miller, J., & Roch, A-C. (1996). Absence of coactivation in the motor component: Evidence from psychophysiological measures of target detection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22, 25–41.
go back to reference Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9(2) 129–154. Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9(2) 129–154.
go back to reference Riggio, L., Gawryszewski, L. G., & Umiltà, C. (1986). What is crossed in crossed-hand effects? Acta Psychologica, 62(1) 89–100. Riggio, L., Gawryszewski, L. G., & Umiltà, C. (1986). What is crossed in crossed-hand effects? Acta Psychologica, 62(1) 89–100.
go back to reference Sanders, A. (1980). Stage analysis of reaction processes. In G. E. Stelmach & J. Requin (Eds.), Tutorials in motor behavior (pp. 331–354). Amsterdam: North Holland. Sanders, A. (1980). Stage analysis of reaction processes. In G. E. Stelmach & J. Requin (Eds.), Tutorials in motor behavior (pp. 331–354). Amsterdam: North Holland.
go back to reference Simon, J. R. (1969). Reactions toward the source of stimulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81, 174–176. Simon, J. R. (1969). Reactions toward the source of stimulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81, 174–176.
go back to reference Spijkers, W. A., & Walter, A. (1985). Response processing stages in choice reactions. Acta Psychologica, 58, 191–204. Spijkers, W. A., & Walter, A. (1985). Response processing stages in choice reactions. Acta Psychologica, 58, 191–204.
go back to reference Stöcker, C., Hoffmann, J. & A. Sebald. (2003). The Influence of Response-Effect Compatibility in a Serial Reaction Time Task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. Section A. Human Experimental Psychology (in press). Stöcker, C., Hoffmann, J. & A. Sebald. (2003). The Influence of Response-Effect Compatibility in a Serial Reaction Time Task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. Section A. Human Experimental Psychology (in press).
go back to reference Tlauka, M. & McKenna, F.P. (1998). Mental imagery yields stimulus-response compatibility. Acta Psychologica. 94. 227–252. Tlauka, M. & McKenna, F.P. (1998). Mental imagery yields stimulus-response compatibility. Acta Psychologica. 94. 227–252.
go back to reference Ulrich, R., Rinkenauer, G., & Miller, J. (1998). Effects of stimulus duration and intensity on simple reaction time and response force. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 915–928 Ulrich, R., Rinkenauer, G., & Miller, J. (1998). Effects of stimulus duration and intensity on simple reaction time and response force. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 915–928
Metagegevens
Titel
Temporal response-effect compatibility
Auteur
Wilfried Kunde
Publicatiedatum
01-08-2003
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 3/2003
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-002-0114-5

Andere artikelen Uitgave 3/2003

Psychological Research 3/2003 Naar de uitgave