Skip to main content
Log in

Effects of priming goal pursuit on implicit sequence learning

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Implicit learning, the type of learning that occurs without intent to learn or awareness of what has been learned, has been thought to be insensitive to the effects of priming, but recent studies suggest this is not the case. One study found that learning in the serial reaction time (SRT) task was improved by nonconscious goal pursuit, primed via a word search task (Eitam et al. in Psychol Sci 19:261–267, 2008). In two studies, we used the goal priming word search task from Eitam et al., but with a different version of the SRT, the alternating serial reaction time task (ASRT). Unlike the SRT, which often results in explicit knowledge and assesses sequence learning at one point in time, the ASRT has been shown to be implicit through sensitive measures of judgment, and it enables sequence learning to be measured continuously. In both studies, we found that implicit learning was superior in the groups that were primed for goal achievement compared to control groups, but the effect was transient. We discuss possible reasons for the observed time course of the positive effects of goal priming, as well as some future areas of investigation to better understand the mechanisms that underlie this effect, which could lead to methods to prolong the positive effects.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Aarts H (2007) On the emergence of human goal pursuit: the nonconscious regulation and motivation of goals. Soc Pers Psychol Compass 1:183–201

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aarts H, Custers R, Holland RW (2007) The nonconscious cessation of goal pursuit: when goals and negative affect are coactivated. J Pers Soc Psychol 92:165–178

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bargh JA, Chen M, Burrows L (1996) Automaticity of social behavior: direct effects of trait construct and stereotype activation on action. J Pers Soc Psychol 71:230–244

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bargh JA, Gollwitzer PM, Lee-Cahi A, Barndollar K, Trotschel R (2001) The automated will: nonconscious activation and pursuit of behavioral goals. J Pers Soc Psychol 81:1014–1027

    Article  PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bennett IJ, Howard JH Jr, Howard DV (2007) Age-related differences in implicit learning of subtle third-order sequential structure. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 62B:98–103

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cleeremans A, Destrebecqz A, Boyer M (1998) Implicit learning: news from the front. Trends Cogn Sci 2:406–416

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Custers R, Aarts H (2005) Positive affect as implicit motivator: on the nonconscious operation of behavioral goals. J Pers Soc Psychol 89:129–142

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dijksterhuis A, Aarts H, Bargh JA, van Knippenberg A (2000) On the relation between associative strength and automatic behavior. J Exp Soc Psychol 36:531–544

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eitam B, Hassin RR, Schul Y (2008) Nonconscious goal pursuit in novel environments—the case of implicit learning. Psychol Sci 19:261–267

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gamble KR, Cummings TJ Jr, Lo SE, Ghosh PT, Howard JH Jr, Howard DV (2014) Implicit sequence learning in people with Parkinson’s disease. Front Hum Neurosci 8:563. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00563

  • Hassin RR, Bargh JA, Zimerman S (2009) Automatic and flexible: the case of non-conscious goal pursuit. Soc Cogn 27:20–36

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Heine SJ, Proulx T, Vohs KD (2006) The meaning maintenance model: on the coherence of social motivations. Pers Soc Psychol Rev 10:88–110

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Holland RW, Hendriks M, Aarts H (2005) Smells like clean spirit: nonconscious effects of scent on cognition and behavior. Psychol Sci 16:689–693

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Howard JH Jr, Howard DV (1997) Age differences in implicit learning of higher order dependencies in serial patterns. Psychol Aging 12:634–656

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Howard DV, Howard JH Jr, Japikse K, DiYanni C, Thompson A, Somberg R (2004a) Implicit sequence learning: effects of level of structure, adult age, and extended practice. Psychol Aging 19:79–92

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Howard JH Jr, Howard DV, Dennis NA, Yankovich H, Vaidya CJ (2004b) Implicit spatial contextual learning in healthy aging. Neuropsychology 18:124–134

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kempf F, Brücke C, Kühn AA, Schneider G-H, Kupsch A, Chu Chen C, Androulidakis AG, Wang S, Vandenberghe W, Nuttin B, Aziz T, Brown P (2007) Modulation by dopamine of human basal ganglia involvement in feedback control of movement. Curr Biol 17:R587–R598

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kühn AA, Brücke C, Hübl J, Schneider G-H, Kupsch A, Eusebio A, Ashkan K, Holland P, Aziz T, Vandenberghe W, Nuttin B, Brown P (2008) Motivation modulates motor-related feedback activity in the human basal ganglia. Curr Biol 18:R648–R650

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McCulloch KC, Ferguson MJ, Kawada CCK, Bargh JA (2008) Taking a closer look: on the operation of nonconscious impression formation. J Exp Soc Psychol 44:614–623

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nissen MJ, Bullemer P (1987) Attentional requirements of learning: evidence from performance measures. Cogn Psychol 19:1–32

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reber AS (1989) Implicit learning and tacit knowledge. J Exp Psychol Gen 118:219–235

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salamone JD, Correa M, Mingote SM, Weber SM (2005) Beyond the reward hypothesis: alternative functions of nucleus accumbens dopamine. Curr Opin Pharmacol 5:34–41

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Shang J, Fu Q, Dienes Z, Shao C, Fu X (2013) Negative affect reduces performance in implicit sequence learning. PLoS ONE 8:e54693–e54693

    Article  PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Simon JR, Stollstorff M, Westbay LC, Vaidya CJ, Howard JH Jr, Howard DV (2011) Dopamine transporter genotype predicts implicit sequence learning. Behav Brain Res 216:452–457

    Article  PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wilkinson L, Khan Z, Jahanshahi M (2009) The role of the basal ganglia and its cortical connections in sequence learning: evidence from implicit and explicit sequence learning in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia 47:2564–2573

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wulf G, Chiviacowsky S, Lewthwaite R (2012) Altering mindset can enhance motor learning in older adults. Psychol Aging 27:14–21

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the NIH/NIA for funding this research through RO1AG036863 to D.V.H. and J.H.H.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Katherine R. Gamble.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Gamble, K.R., Lee, J.M., Howard, J.H. et al. Effects of priming goal pursuit on implicit sequence learning. Exp Brain Res 232, 3635–3643 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-014-4054-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-014-4054-2

Keywords

Navigation