Skip to main content
Log in

Hemifield or hemispace: what accounts for the ipsilateral advantages in visually guided aiming?

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

An Erratum to this article was published on 16 October 2013

Abstract

Aiming movements to targets presented on the same side as the reaching limb are faster and more accurate than movements made across the body. These advantages are typically attributed to within-hemisphere sensorimotor control. However, contrary to the within- versus between-hemisphere model, we have shown that some of these advantages tend to go with the side of the movement, rather than the side of the target (Carey et al. Exp Brain Res 112:496–504, 1996; Carey and Otto-de Haart Neuropsychologia 39:894, 2001). Barthélémy and Boulinghez (Exp Brain Res 147:305–312, 2002) acknowledge that our biomechanical account fits data for post-onset movement parameters such as peak velocity and duration, yet they report evidence for some within- versus between-hemisphere contributions to reaction time (RT) advantages. To examine a possible difference between early and late movement kinematics fitting these alternative models, we have dissociated field and space in a different way, which required arm movements with differential inertial consequences, as well as unpredictability of target location in terms of visual field. The data suggest that visual field may contribute some of the variance to hemispatial effects, but only for the right hand. In a second experiment, we used an antipointing task to examine hemispatial versus visual field effects on RTs and to revisit the possible hand difference identified in experiment 1. We found that hemispace accounted for all of the ipsilateral advantages, including RT, for both right and left hands. Results are discussed in terms of the computational requirements of eye–hand coordination in relative unconstrained conditions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. For durations, with the left hand 25 Ps show hemispatial effects in pointing and 27 in antipointing; for the right, 26 in pointing and 27 in antipointing. For peak velocity, left hand 22 for pointing and 26 for antipointing, right hand 28 in pointing and 27 in antipointing. Fourteen of our 28 participants show hemispatial effects in antipointing for both hands on all three measures. Of the remaining 14, all but two show hemispatial effects in all three measures for one hand and 2/3 measures on the other.

References

  • Adam JJ, Parthoens S, Pratt J (2006) Distinct mechanisms for planning keypress and reaching responses: a developmental study. Hum Mov Sci 25(3):293–309

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barthélémy S, Boulinguez P (2001) Manual reaction time asymmetries in human participants: the role of movement planning and attention. Neurosci Lett 315:41–44

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barthélémy S, Boulinguez P (2002) Manual asymmetries in the directional coding of reaching: further evidence for hemispatial effects and right hemisphere dominance for movement planning. Exp Brain Res 147:305–312

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bestelmeyer PEG, Carey DP (2004) Processing biases towards the preferred hand: valid and invalid cueing of left-versus right-hand movements. Neuropsychologia 42:1162–1167

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boulinguez P, Nougier V, Velay JL (2001) Manual asymmetries in reaching movement control. I: study of right-handers. Cortex 37(1):101–122

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bradshaw JL, Bradshaw JA, Nettleton NC (1990) Abduction, adduction and hand differences in simple and serial movements. Neuropsychologia 28:917–931

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bryden PJ, Pryde KM, Roy EA (2000) A developmental analysis of the relationship between hand preference and performance: II. A performance-based method of measuring hand preference in children. Brain Cogn 43:60–64

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Buckingham G, Main JC, Carey DP (2011) Asymmetries in motor attention during bimanual reaching: left and right handers compared. Cortex 47:432–440

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Butler AJ, Fink GR, Dohle C, Wunderlich G, Tellmann L, Seitz RJ, Zille K, Freund H-J (2004) Neural mechanisms underlying reaching for remembered targets cued kinesthetically or visually in left or right hemispace. Hum Brain Mapp 21:165–177

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carey DP, Otto-de Haart EG (2001) Hemispatial differences in visually guided aiming are neither hemispatial nor visual. Neuropsychologia 39:885–894

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Carey DP, Hargreaves EL, Goodale MA (1996) Reaching to ipsilateral or contralateral targets: within-hemisphere visuomotor processing cannot explain hemispatial differences in motor control. Exp Brain Res 112:496–504

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Carnahan H (1998) Manual asymmetries when pointing and grasping in response to rapid target movement. Brain Cogn 37:237–253

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Carson RG (1996) Putative right hemisphere contributions to the preparation of reaching and aiming movements. In: Elliott D, Roy EA (eds) Manual asymmetries in motor performance. CRC Press, Boca Raton, pp 159–172

    Google Scholar 

  • Carson RG, Chua R, Elliott D, Goodman D (1990) The contribution of vision to asymmetries in manual aiming. Neuropsychologia 28:1215–1220

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Carson RG, Goodman D, Elliott D (1992) Asymmetries in the discrete and pseudocontinuous regulation of visually guided reaching. Brain Cogn 18:169–191

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Carson RG, Goodman D, Chua R, Elliott D (1993) Asymmetries in the regulation of visually guided aiming. J Mot Behav 25:21–32

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Chua R, Carson RG, Goodman D, Elliott D (1992) Asymmetries in the spatial localization of transformed targets. Brain Cogn 20:227–235

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Chua RG, Goodman D, Byblow WD, Elliott D (1995) The preparation of aiming movements. Brain Cogn 28:133–154

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Elias LJ, Bryden MP (1998) Footedness is a better predictor of language lateralisation than handedness. Laterality 3:41–51

    Google Scholar 

  • Elliot D, Roy EA, Goodman D, Carson RG, Chua R, Maraj BKV (1993) Asymmetries in the preparation and control of aiming movements. Can J Exp Psychol 47:570–589

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elliott D, Chua R (1996) Manual asymmetries in goal-directed movement. In: Elliott D, Roy EA (eds) Manual asymmetries in motor performance. CRC Press, Boca Raton, pp 143–158

    Google Scholar 

  • Fisk JD, Goodale MA (1985) The organization of eye and limb movements during unrestricted reaching to targets in ipsilateral and contralateral space. Exp Brain Res 60:159–178

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fisk JD, Goodale MA (1988) The effects of unilateral brain damage on visually-guided reaching: hemispheric differences in the nature of the deficit. Exp Brain Res 72:425–35

    Google Scholar 

  • Flash (1987) The control of hand equilibrium trajectories in multi-joint arm movements. Biol Cybern 57:257–274

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gabbard C, Helbig CR (2004) What drives children’s limb selection for reaching in hemispace? Exp Brain Res 156:325–332

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ghez C, Gordon J, Ghilardi MF (1993) Programming of extent and direction in human reaching movements. Biomed Res 14(Suppl 1):1–5

    Google Scholar 

  • Gonzalez CL, Whitwell RL, Morrissey B, Ganel T, Goodale MA (2007) Left handedness does not extend to visually guided precision grasping. Exp Brain Res 182:275–279

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goodale MA (1990) Brain asymmetries in the control of reaching. In: Goodale M (ed) Vision and action: the control of grasping. Ablex Publishing, Norwood, NJ, pp 14–32

    Google Scholar 

  • Gordon J, Gilhardi MF, Cooper SE, Ghez C (1994) Accuracy of planar reaching movements. II. Systematic errors resulting from inertial anisotropy. Exp Brain Res 99:112–130

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Guitton D, Buchtel HA, Douglas RM (1985) Frontal lobe lesions in man cause difficulties in suppressing reflexive glances and in generating goal-directed saccades. Exp Brain Res 58:455–472

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Haaland KJ, Harrington D (1996) Hemispheric asymmetry of movement. Curr Opin Neurobiol 6:796–800

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Heath M, Bell J, Holroyd CB, Krigolson O (2012) Electroencephalographic evidence of vector inversion in antipointing. Exp Brain Res 221:19–26

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hodges NJ, Lyons J, Cockell D, Reed A, Elliott D (1997) Hand, space and attentional asymmetries in goal-directed manual aiming. Cortex 33:251–269

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hogan (1985) The mechanics of mulit-joint posture and movement control. Biol Cybern 52:315–331

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Horstmann A, Hoffmann KP (2005) Target selection in eye–hand coordination: Do we reach to where we look or do we look to where we reach? Exp Brain Res 167:187–195

    Google Scholar 

  • Ingum J, Bjorklund R (1994) Effects of flunitrazepam on responses to lateralized visual stimuli: evidence for cerebral asymmetry of execution of manual movements to targets in contralateral and ipsilateral visual space. Psychopharmacology 114:551–558

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Jeannerod M (1988) The neural and behavioural organization of goal-directed movements. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Kim W, Buchanan J, Gabbard G (2011) Constraints on arm selection processes when reaching: degrees of freedom and joint amplitudes interact to influence limb selection. J Mot Behav 43:403–411

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Levin MF (1996) Interjoint coordination during pointing movements is disrupted in spastic hemiparesis. Brain 119:281–293

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maraj A, Heath M (2010) Antipointing: perception-based visual information renders an offline mode of control. Exp Brain Res 202:55–64

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mieschke PE, Elliott D, Helsen WF, Carson RG, Coull JA (2001) Manual asymmetries in the preparation and control of goal-directed movements. Brain Cogn 45:129–40

    Google Scholar 

  • Miles WR (1929) Ocular dominance demonstrated by unconscious sighting. J Exp Psychol 12:113–126

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prablanc C, Echallier JF, Komilis E, Jeannerod M (1979) Optimal response of the eye and hand motor systems in pointing at a visual target. I. Spatio-temporal characteristics of eye and hand movements and their relationships when varying the amount of visual information. Biol Cybern 35:113–124

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rossit S, Malhotra P, Muir K, Reeves I, Duncan G, Harvey M (2011) The role of right temporal lobe structures in off-line action: evidence from lesion-behavior mapping in stroke patients. Cereb Cortex 21:2751–2761

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Seegelke C, Hughes CML, Schack T (2011) An investigation into manual asymmetries in grasp behaviour and kinematics during an object manipulation task. Exp Brain Res 215:65–75

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Steenhuis RE, Bryden MP (1989) Different dimensions of hand preference that relate to skilled and unskilled activities. Cortex 25:289–304

    Google Scholar 

  • van Der Staak (1975) Intra- and interhemispheric visual-motor control of human arm movements. Neuropsycholgia 13:439–448

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Velay JL, Daffaure V, Raphael N, Benoit-Dubrocard S (2001) Hemispheric asymmetry and interhemispheric transfer in pointing depend on the spatial components of the movement. Cortex 37(1):75–90

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Jim Urquhart and Irene Logan for computer programming and developing and maintaining our stimulus board. Gavin Buckingham and Leah T. Johnstone provided helpful comments on a draft of this manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to David P. Carey.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Carey, D.P., Liddle, J. Hemifield or hemispace: what accounts for the ipsilateral advantages in visually guided aiming?. Exp Brain Res 230, 323–331 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-013-3657-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-013-3657-3

Keywords

Navigation