Skip to main content
Log in

Developmental changes of the biomechanical effect in motor imagery

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Motor imagery has been investigated in childhood and early adolescence, but not across adolescence stages; moreover, available evidence did not clarify whether the involvement of motor information in mental rotation of body parts becomes stronger or weaker during development. In the present study, we employed the hand laterality task to assess motor imagery in ninety-seven typically developing adolescents divided into three age groups (i.e., 11–12, 14–15, and 17–18 years); mental rotation of objects and letters were also assessed. As a specific marker of the motor involvement in mental rotation of body parts, we assessed the so-called biomechanical effect, that is, the advantage for judging hand pictures showing physically comfortable positions with respect to hand pictures showing physically impossible or awkward positions. Results demonstrated that the biomechanical effect did not significantly affect early adolescents’ performance, whereas it became significant in 14- to 15-year-old participants and even more stronger in 17- to 18-year-old participants; this pattern did not depend on an increase in processing speed to mentally rotate both corporeal and non-corporeal (objects and letters) stimuli. The present findings demonstrated that: (1) motor imagery undergoes a continuous and progressive refinement throughout adolescence, and (2) full exploitation of motor information to mentally transform corporeal stimuli can be attained in late adolescence only.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. To verify whether the lack of a significant biomechanical effect in early adolescents could be accounted for by increased variance in their performance blurring the effect, we tested for the homogeneity of variances between the different age groups. To this aim, we first calculated difference in RTs for mental rotation of 90° oriented right (awkward position) and left hand (comfortable orientation), and the difference in RTs for 270° oriented left (awkward position) and right hand (comfortable orientation; e.g., Conson et al. 2010). Then, these delta values underwent Levene’s test of homogeneity of variances between groups. Levene’s test showed significantly larger variance in middle versus early adolescents on the 270° delta value (F = 5.896, p = .018), whereas all remaining comparisons were not significant (early vs. middle adolescence: 90°, F = 1.407, p = .240; early vs. late adolescence: 90°, F = .305, p = .583; 270°, F = .342, p = .561; middle vs. late adolescence: 90°, F = 3.871, p = .053; 270°, F = 2.792, p = .100). These data ruled out the possibility that the lack of a significant biomechanical effect in early adolescents could be ascribed to increased variability of their performance with respect to the other two age groups.

References

  • Acredolo LP, Adams A, Goodwyn SW (1984) The role of self-produced movement and visual tracking in infant spatial orientation. J Exp Child Psychol 38:312–327

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bakker M, de Lange FP, Helmich RC, Scheeringa R, Bloem BR, Toni I (2008) Cerebral correlates of motor imagery of normal and precision gait. Neuroimage 41:998–1010

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Benson JB, Uzgiris IC (1985) Effect of self-initiated locomotion on infant search activity. Dev Psychol 21:923–931

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blakemore S-J, Sirigu A (2003) Action prediction in the cerebellum and in the parietal lobe. Exp Brain Res 153:239–245

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Caeyenberghs K, Tsoupas J, Wilson PH, Smits-Engelsman BC (2009a) Motor imagery development in primary school children. Dev Neuropsychol 34:103–121

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Caeyenberghs K, Wilson PH, van Roon D, Swinnen SP, Smits-Engelsman BC (2009b) Increasing convergence between imagined and executed movement across development: evidence for the emergence of movement representations. Dev Sci 12:474–483

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Choudhury S, Charman T, Bird V, Blakemore S (2007a) Development of action representation during adolescence. Neuropsychologia 45:255–262

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Choudhury S, Charman T, Bird V, Blakemore S (2007b) Adolescent development of motor imagery in a visually guided pointing task. Conscious Cogn 16:886–896

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Conson M, Sacco S, Sarà M, Pistoia F, Grossi D, Trojano L (2008) Selective motor imagery defect in patients with locked-in syndrome. Neuropsychologia 46:2622–2628

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Conson M, Sarà M, Pistoia F, Trojano L (2009) Action observation improves motor imagery: specific interactions between simulative processes. Exp Brain Res 199:71–81

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Conson M, Pistoia F, Sarà M, Grossi D, Trojano L (2010) Recognition and mental manipulation of body parts dissociate in locked-in syndrome. Brain Cogn 73:189–193

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cooper LA, Shepard RN (1975) Mental transformations in the identification of left and right hands. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 1:48–56

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Lange FP, Helmich RC, Toni I (2006) Posture influences motor imagery: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 33:609–617

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deconinck FJ, Spitaels L, Fias W, Lenoir M (2009) Is developmental coordination disorder a motor imagery deficit? J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 31:720–730

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Desmurget M, Sirigu A (2009) A parietal-premotor network for movement intention and motor awareness. Trends Cogn Sci 13:411–419

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Estes D (1998) Young children’s awareness of their mental activity: the case of mental rotation. Child Dev 69:1345–1360

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Frassinetti F, Ferri F, Maini M, Benassi MG, Gallese V (2011) Bodily self: an implicit knowledge of what is explicitly unknown. Exp Brain Res 212:153–160

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frick A, Daum MM, Walser S, Mast FW (2009a) Motor processes in children’s mental rotation. J Cogn Dev 10:18–40

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frick A, Daum MM, Wilson M, Wilkening F (2009b) Effects of action on children’s and adults’ mental imagery. J Exp Child Psychol 104:34–51

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Funk M, Brugger P, Wilkening F (2005) Motor processes in children’s imagery: the case of mental rotation of hands. Dev Sci 8:402–408

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gabbard C (2009) Studying action representation in children via motor imagery. Brain Cogn 71:234–239

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gabbard C, Cordova A, Lee S (2007) Examining the effects of postural constraints on estimating reach. J Mot Behav 39:242–246

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gogtay N, Giedd JN, Lusk L, Hayashi KM, Greenstein D, Vaituzis AC, Nugent TF 3rd, Herman DH, Clasen LS, Toga AW, Rapoport JL, Thompson PM (2004) Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 101:8174–8179

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Jeannerod M (2001) Neural simulation of action: a unifying mechanism for motor cognition. Neuroimage 14:S103–S109

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn SM, Margolis JA, Barrett AM, Goldknopf EJ, Daly PF (1990) Age differences in imagery abilities. Child Dev 61:995–1010

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn SM, Di Girolamo GJ, Thompson WL, Alpert NM (1998) Mental rotation of objects versus hands: neural mechanisms revealed by positron emission tomography. Psychophysiology 35:151–161

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn SM, Thompson WL, Wraga M, Alpert NM (2001) Imagining rotation by endogenous versus exogenous forces: distinct neural mechanisms. NeuroReport 12:2519–2525

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Krüger M, Krist H (2009) Imagery and motor processes—when are they connected? The mental rotation of body parts in development. J Cogn Dev 10:239–261

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marmor GS (1975) Development of kinetic images: when does the child first represent movement in mental images? Cogn Psychol 7:548–559

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marmor GS (1977) Mental rotation and number conservation: are they related? Dev Psychol 13:320–325

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Orsini A (1993) WISC-R. Contributo alla taratura italiana. O.S. Organizzazioni Speciali, Firenze

    Google Scholar 

  • Parsons LM (1987) Imagined spatial transformation of one’s hands and feet. Cogn Psychol 19:178–241

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Parsons LM (1994) Temporal and kinematic properties of motor behavior reflected in mentally simulated action. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 20:709–730

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Paus T (2005) Mapping brain maturation and cognitive development during adolescence. Trends Cogn Sci 9:60–68

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rieser JJ, Garing AE, Young MF (1994) Imagery, action, and young children’s spatial orientation: it’s not being there that counts, it’s what one has in mind. Child Dev 65:1262–1278

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sanders AF (1998) Elements of human performance. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah NJ

    Google Scholar 

  • Sekiyama K (1982) Kinesthetic aspects of mental representations in the identification of left and right hands. Percept Psychophys 32:89–95

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Shepard RN, Cooper LA (1982) Mental images and their transformations. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Shepard RN, Metzler J (1971) Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science 171:701–703

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Urgesi C, Candidi M, Ionta S, Aglioti SM (2007) Representation of body identity and body actions in extrastriate body area and ventral premotor cortex. Nat Neurosci 10:30–31

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • van Nuenen BF, Helmich RC, Buenen N, van de Warrenburg BP, Bloem BR, Toni I (2012) Compensatory activity in the extrastriate body area of Parkinson’s disease patients. J Neurosci 32:9546–9953

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Westlye LT, Walhovd KB, Dale AM, Bjørnerud A, Due-Tønnessen P, Engvig A, Grydeland H, Tamnes CK, Østby Y, Fjell AM (2010) Differentiating maturational and aging-related changes of the cerebral cortex by use of thickness and signal intensity. Neuroimage 52:172–185

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wolpert DM, Ghahramani Z, Jordan MI (1995) An internal model for sensorimotor integration. Science 269:1880–1882

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Massimiliano Conson or Luigi Trojano.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Conson, M., Mazzarella, E. & Trojano, L. Developmental changes of the biomechanical effect in motor imagery. Exp Brain Res 226, 441–449 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-013-3456-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-013-3456-x

Keywords

Navigation