Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Moving further moves things further away in visual perception: position-based movement planning affects distance judgments

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We examined how different characteristics of planned hand movements affect visual perception of distances in reachable space. Participants planned hand movements of certain amplitude. Before execution of the movement, certain visual distances had to be judged. Distances were judged as larger the larger the amplitude of the concurrently prepared hand movements was. On top of that, with constant movement amplitude, distances were judged as larger, the further away the start point of the planned movement was located from the body. These results indicate that distinct variables specified during motor planning, such as effector’s final position, are linked to the visual perception of environmental characteristics.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Manipulating movement start position when movement amplitude is prescribed is essentially equivalent to manipulating the required end position. Yet, we prefer to describe this manipulation as one of start position, because this position is under full experimental control, whereas movement end positions depend on the way these movements are eventually carried out, and are thus less rigorously controlled.

  2. Note, the lack of a significant effect of factor distance does not indicate that participants could not discriminate between the given target distances because the reported analyses were based on deviations of estimated magnitude from the real distance. When the magnitude of distance estimates was considered a distance effect was evident, F(3, 60) = 183. In our previous study (Kirsch and Kunde in press) we observed distance effects also in perceptual errors indicating an increase of an optical illusion with an increase in distance. In the present study, this effect was not observed probably due to a much smaller target range (11 vs. 41 mm).

References

  • Battaglia-Mayer A, Caminiti R, Lacquaniti F, Zago M (2003) Multiple levels of representation of reaching in the parieto-frontal network. Cereb Cortex 13:1009–1022

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bekkering H, Neggers SFW (2002) Visual search is modulated by action intentions. Psychol Sci 13(4):370–374

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berti A, Frassinetti F (2000) When far space becomes near: remapping of space by tool use. J Cogn Neurosci 12(3):415–420

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bhalla M, Proffitt DR (1999) Visual-motor recalibration in geographical slant perception. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 25(4):1076–1096

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bock O, Eckmiller R (1986) Goal-directed arm movements in absence of visual guidance: evidence for amplitude rather than position control. Exp Brain Res 62:451–458

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Collins T, Schicke T, Röder B (2008) Action goal selection and motor planning can be dissociated by tool use. Cognition 109:363–371

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Farnè A, Làdavas E (2000) Dynamic size-change of hand peripersonal space following toll use. NeuroReport 11(8):1645–1649

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gordon J, Ghilardi MF, Ghez C (1994) Accuracy of planar reaching movements: I. Independence of direction and extent variability. Exp Brain Res 99(1):97–111

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gutteling TP, Kenemans JL, Neggers SFW (2011) Grasping preparation enhances orientation change detection. PLoS ONE 6(3):e17675. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0017675

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Heuer H (1981) Fast aiming movements with the left and right arm: evidence for two-process theories of motor control. Psychol Res 43:81–96

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Heuer H (2002) The effects of weak perturbations on rapid finger oscillations. Hum Mov Sci 21:119–130

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heuer H (2006) Multiple frames of reference for bimanual co-ordination. Exp Brain Res 175:485–498

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heuer H, Klein W (2006) Intermanual interactions related to movement amplitudes and endpoint locations. J Mot Behav 38(2):126–138

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heuer H, Sangals J (1998) Task-dependent mixtures of coordinate systems in visuomotor transformations. Exp Brain Res 119:224–236

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kirsch W, Kunde W (in press) Visual near space is scaled to parameters of current action plans. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. doi:10.1037/a0031074

  • Kirsch W, Herbort O, Butz MV, Kunde W (2012) Influence of motor planning on distance perception within the peripersonal space. PLoS ONE 7(4):e34880. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0034880

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lee Y, Lee S, Carello C, Turvey MT (2012) An archer’s perceived form scales the “hitableness” of archery targets. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 38(5):1125–1131

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Linkenauger SA, Witt JK, Stefanucci JK, Bakdash JZ, Proffitt DR (2009) The effects of handedness and reachability on perceived distance. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 35(6):1649–1660

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Longo MR, Lourenco SF (2006) On the nature of near space: effects of tool use and the transition to far space. Neuropsychologia 44:977–981

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lourenco SF, Longo MR (2009) The plasticity of near space: evidence for contraction. Cognition 112:451–456

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Polit A, Bizzi E (1979) Characteristics of motor programs underlying arm movements in monkeys. J Neurophysiol 42(1):183–194

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Proffitt DR (2008) An action specific approach to spatial perception. In: Klatzky RL, MacWhinney B, Behrmann M (eds) Embodiment, ego-space, and action. Psychology Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Proffitt DR, Linkenauger SA (2013) Perception viewed as a phenotypic expression. In: Prinz W (ed) Action science, MIT Press, Cambridge

  • Proffitt DR, Stefanucci J, Banton T, Epstein W (2003) The role of effort in perceiving distance. Psychol Sci 14(2):106–112

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbaum DA, Meulenbroek RJ, Vaughan J (1999) Remembered positions: stored locations or stored postures? Exp Brain Res 124(4):503–512

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sanders AF (1980) Stage analysis of reaction processes. In: Stelmach GE, Requin J (eds) Tutorials in motor behavior. North Holland, Amsterdam, pp 331–354

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt RA, Sherwood DE, Walter CB (1988) Rapid movements with reversals in direction. I. The control of movement time. Exp Brain Res 69:344–354

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Vishton PM, Stephens NJ, Nelson LA, Morra SE, Brunick KL, Stevens JA (2007) Planning to reach for an object changes how the reacher perceives it. Psychol Sci 18:713–719

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Walsh WD, Russell DG, Imanaka K, James B (1979) Memory for constrained and preselected movement location and distance: effect of starting position and length. J Mot Behav 11:201–214

    Google Scholar 

  • Witt JK (2011) Tool use influences perceived shape and perceived parallelism, which serve as indirect measure of perceived distance. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. doi:10.1037/a0021933

  • Witt JK, Dorsch TE (2009) Kicking to bigger uprights: field goal kicking performance influences perceived size. Perception 38(9):1328–1340

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Witt JK, Proffitt DR (2008) Action-specific influences on distance perception: a role for motor simulation. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 34(6):1479–1492

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Witt JK, Proffitt DR, Epstein W (2004) Perceiving distance: a role of effort and intend. Perception 33:577–590

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Witt JK, Proffitt DR, Epstein W (2005) Tool use affects perceived distance but only when you intend to use it. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 31(5):880–888

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Witt JK, Linkenauger SA, Bakdash JZ, Proffitt DR (2008) Putting to a bigger hole: golf performance relates to perceived size. Psychon Bull Rev 15(3):581–585

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Witt JK, Proffitt DR, Epstein W (2010) When and how are spatial perceptions scaled? J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 36(5):1153–1160

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by grant KI 1620/1-1 awarded to W. Kirsch by the German Research Council (DFG).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Wladimir Kirsch.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Kirsch, W., Kunde, W. Moving further moves things further away in visual perception: position-based movement planning affects distance judgments. Exp Brain Res 226, 431–440 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-013-3455-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-013-3455-y

Keywords

Navigation