Skip to main content
Log in

Number generation bias after action observation

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Recent studies have demonstrated that conceptual and abstract knowledge could rely on and could be influenced by sensory-motor processing of usual goal-directed actions. In line with this, interactions have been reported between number magnitude and finger grip with, for example, small-magnitude numbers priming grip closure and large-magnitude numbers priming grip aperture. Here, we assessed whether observing a closing or opening grip was able to influence the magnitude of the numbers produced in a random number generation task, and we tested whether this effect was specific to biological hand actions by using non-biological fake hands with the same closure or aperture amplitude. The participants were asked to produce as randomly as possible numbers between 1 and 10 after they observed a change in posture (i.e. grip closing or grip opening) or in colour (i.e. red or blue hand). The results revealed that the participants produced more often small numbers than large ones after observing a grip closing, whereas they produced equally often small and large numbers after observing a grip opening or colour changes. Importantly, this effect was only present for the biological hands but not for the non-biological fake hands. This finding demonstrates that observing a biological grip closing conveys small-magnitude information, which, in turn, influences the mental selection of a numerical response. We discuss our results in the light of the internal random generator process proposed in the domain of numerical cognition and argue that number semantics is stored with a code governed by sensory-motor mechanisms.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adams JA (1986) Use of model’s knowledge of results to increase the observer’s performance. J Hum Mov Stud 12:89–98

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Andres M, Davare M, Pesenti M, Olivier E, Seron X (2004) Number magnitude and grip aperture interaction. NeuroReport 15:2773–2777

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Badets A, Pesenti M (2010) Creating number semantics through finger movement perception. Cognition 115:46–53

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Badets A, Pesenti M (2011) Finger-number interaction an ideomotor account. Exp Psychol 58:287–292

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Badets A, Andres M, Di Luca S, Pesenti M (2007) Number magnitude potentiates action judgements. Exp Brain Res 180:525–534

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou LW (1999) Perceptual symbol systems. Behav Brain Sci 22:577–609

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Binkofski F, Buccino G, Posse S, Seitz RJ, Rizzolatti G, Freund HJ (1999) A fronto-parietal circuit for object manipulation in man: evidence from an fMRI-study. Eur J Neurosci 11:3276–3286

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bueti D, Walsh V (2009) The parietal cortex and the representation of time, space, number and other magnitudes. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 364:1831–1840

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Coull JT, Nobre AC (1998) Where and when to pay attention: the neural systems for directing attention to spatial locations and to time intervals as revealed by both PET and fMRI. J Neurosci 18:7426–7435

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Culham JC, Danckert SL, DeSouza JFX, Gati JS, Menon RS, Goodale MA (2003) Visually guided grasping produces fMRI activation in dorsal but not ventral stream brain areas. Exp Brain Res 153:180–189

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene S, Bossini S, Giraux P (1993) The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. J Exp Psychol Gen 122:371–396

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodale MA, Milner AD (2004) Sight unseen: an exploration of consciousness and unconscious vision. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Grezes J, Decety J (2001) Functional anatomy of execution, mental simulation, observation, and verb generation of actions: a meta-analysis. Hum Brain Mapp 12:1–19

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hartmann M, Grabherr L, Mast FW (2011) Moving along the mental number line: Interactions between whole-body motion and numerical cognition. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. doi:10.1037/a0026706

  • Kunde W, Koch I, Hoffmann J (2004) Anticipated action effects affect the selection, initiation, and execution of actions. Q J Exp Psychol A 57:87–106

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lindemann O, Abolafia JA, Girardi G, Bekkering H (2007) Getting a grip on numbers: numerical magnitude priming in object grasping. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 33:1400–1409

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loetscher T, Brugger P (2007) Exploring number space by random digit generation. Exp Brain Res 180:655–665

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loetscher T, Schwarz U, Schubiger M, Brugger P (2008) Head turns bias the brain’s internal random generator. Curr Biol 18:R60–R62

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Loetscher T, Bockisch CJ, Nicholls MER, Brugger P (2010) Eye position predicts what number you have in mind. Curr Biol 20:R264–R265

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Malfait N, Valyear KF, Culham JC, Anton J-L, Brown LE, Gribble PL (2010) fMRI Activation during observation of others’ reach errors. J Cogn Neurosci 22:1493–1503

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pesenti M, Thioux M, Seron X, De Volder A (2000) Neuroanatomical substrates of Arabic number processing, numerical comparison, and simple addition: a PET study. J Cogn Neurosci 12:461–479

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Song J-H, Nakayama K (2008) Numeric comparison in a visually-guided manual reaching task. Cognition 106:994–1003

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van der Linden M, Beerten A, Pesenti M (1998) Age-related differences in random generation. Brain Cogn 38:1–16

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Walsh V (2003) A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity. Trends Cogn Sci 7:483–488

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

La Maison des Sciences de l’Homme et de la Société de Poitiers (France) financially supported this study. We are grateful to Yves Almecija (CNRS-UMR-7295) for his valuable comments for the editing of the pictures used in the present study. MP is a research associate at the National Fund for Scientific Research (Belgium).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Arnaud Badets.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Badets, A., Bouquet, C.A., Ric, F. et al. Number generation bias after action observation. Exp Brain Res 221, 43–49 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-012-3145-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-012-3145-1

Keywords

Navigation