Skip to main content
Log in

To know or not to know: influence of explicit advance knowledge of occlusion on interceptive actions

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study examined how explicit advance knowledge might influence adaptive behavior to visual occlusions. Catching performance and kinematics of good ball catchers were compared between no, early and late occlusion trials. Discrete visual occlusions of 400 ms, occurring early or late in the ball’s approach trajectory, were randomly interspersed between no occlusion trials. In one condition, the presence and type of occlusion were announced a priori (expected), whereas in another condition no such information was provided (unexpected). Expectation of occlusion resulted in an adapted limb transport and increased grasping time, whereas in the unexpected condition a higher peak of wrist velocity was evident for all occlusion conditions. The observed different adaptations cannot be explained by trial-by-trial adaptations alone and instead provide evidence for the influence of explicit advance knowledge in the motor response of interceptive actions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. If this analog signal failed to detect the moment of ball–hand contact, it was derived from the 3D visual reconstruction of the catching movement in the Qualisys software program. A clearly visible sudden jerky backward movement of the index and thumb marker as a consequence of the ball impact was recognized as the moment of contact (Mazyn et al. 2007a).

  2. For most of the participants, a clear peak near the end of the catch characterized hand aperture. However, 5 participants had a plateau shape or a double peak. PHA was then corrected so that it reflects the start of the grasping phase (i.e., a second peak) at the final closure of the hand.

References

  • Bennett S, van der Kamp J, Savelsbergh GJP, Davids K (1999) Timing a one-handed catch I. Effects of telestereoscopic viewing. Exp Brain Res 129:362–368

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bennett SJ, de Xivry JJO, Lefevre P, Barnes GR (2010) Oculomotor prediction of accelerative target motion during occlusion: long-term and short-term effects. Exp Brain Res 204:493–504

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Button C, Davids K, Bennett SJ, Taylor MA (2000) Mechanical perturbation of the wrist during one-handed catching. Acta Psychol 105:9–30

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Button C, Davids K, Bennett SJ, Savelsbergh GJP (2002) Anticipatory responses to perturbation of co-ordination in one-handed catching. Acta Psychol 109:75–93

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlton LG (1981) Processing visual feedback information for movement control. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 7:1019–1030

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Daum MM, Huber S, Krist H (2007) Controlling reaching movements with predictable and unpredictable target motion in 10-year-old children and adults. Exp Brain Res 177:483–492

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Davids K, Button C (2000) The cognition-dynamics interface and performance in sport. Int J Sport Psychol 31:515–521

    Google Scholar 

  • de Lussanet MHE, Smeets JBJ, Brenner E (2002) The relation between task history and movement strategy. Behav Brain Res 129:51–59

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dessing JC, Wijdenes LO, Peper CE, Beek PJ (2009) Adaptations of lateral hand movements to early and late visual occlusion in catching. Exp Brain Res 192:669–682

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Elliott D (1988) The influence of visual target and limb information on manual aiming. Can J Exp Psychol 42:57–68

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Elliott D, Lee TD (1995) The role of target information on manual-aiming bias. Psychol Res 58:2–9

    Google Scholar 

  • Elliott D, Chua R, Pollock BJ, Lyons J (1995) Optimizing the use of vision in manual aiming—the role of practice. Q J Exp Psychol 48:72–83

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fukui T, Inui T (2006) The effect of viewing the moving limb and target object during the early phase of movement on the online control of grasping. Hum Mov Sci 25:349–371

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hansen S, Glazebrook CM, Anson JG, Weeks DJ, Elliott D (2006) The influence of advance information about target location and visual feedback on movement planning and execution. Can J Exp Psychol 60:200–208

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jakobson LS, Goodale MA (1991) Factors affecting higher-order movement planning: a kinematic analysis of human prehension. Exp Brain Res 86:199–208

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Keele SW, Posner MI (1968) Processing of visual feedback in rapid movements. J Exp Psychol 77:155–158

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Khan MA, Elliott D, Coull J, Chua R, Lyons J (2002) Optimal control strategies under different feedback schedules: kinematic evidence. J Mot Behav 34:45–57

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lacquaniti F, Maioli C (1989) Adaptation to suppression of visual information during catching. J Neurosci 9:149–159

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lamb KL, Burwitz L (1988) Visual restriction in ball-catching: a re-examination of early findings. J Hum Mov Stud 14:93–99

    Google Scholar 

  • Laurent M, Montagne G, Savelsbergh GJP (1994) The control and coordination of one-handed catching—the effect of temporal constraints. Exp Brain Res 101:314–322

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Marigold DS, Patla AE (2002) Strategies for dynamic stability during locomotion on a slippery surface: effects of prior experience and knowledge. J Neurophysiol 88:339–353

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marinovic W, Plooy AM, Tresilian JR (2009) The Utilisation of visual information in the control of rapid interceptive actions. Exp Psychol 56:265–273

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mazyn LIN, Montagne G, Savelsbergh GJP, Lenoir M (2006) Reorganization of catching coordination under varying temporal constraints. Mot Control 10:143–159

    Google Scholar 

  • Mazyn LIN, Lenoir M, Montagne G, Savelsbergh GJ (2007a) Spatial and temporal adaptations that accompany increasing catching performance during learning. J Mot Behav 39:491–502

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mazyn LIN, Montagne G, Savelsbergh GJP, Lenoir M (2007b) Planning and on-line control of catching as a function of perceptual-motor constraints. Acta Psychol 126:59–78

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moore SP (1984) Systematic removal of visual feedback. J Hum Mov Stud 10:165–173

    Google Scholar 

  • Polman RCJ, Whiting HTA, Savelsbergh GJP (1996) The spatiotemporal structure of control variables during catching. Exp Brain Res 109:483–494

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Savelsbergh GJP, Whiting HTA, Bootsma RJ (1991) Grasping Tau. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 17:315–322

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Savelsbergh GJP, Whiting HTA, Pijpers JR, Van Santvoord AAM (1993) The visual guidance of catching. Exp Brain Res 93:148–156

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Scheidt RA, Dingwell JB, Mussa-Ivaldi FA (2001) Learning to move amid uncertainty. J Neurophysiol 86:971–985

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schenk T, Mair B, Zihl J (2004) The use of visual feedback and on-line target information in catching and grasping. Exp Brain Res 154:85–96

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sharp RH, Whiting HTA (1974) Exposure and occlude duration effects in a ball-catching skill. J Mot Behav 6:139–147

    Google Scholar 

  • Sharp RH, Whiting HTA (1975) Information-processing and eye movement behaviour in a ball catching skill. J Hum Mov Stud 1:124–131

    Google Scholar 

  • Song JH, Nakayama K (2007) Automatic adjustment of visuomotor readiness. J Vis 7:1–9

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tijtgat P, Bennett SJ, Savelsbergh GJP, De Clercq D, Lenoir M (2010) Advance knowledge effects on kinematics of one-handed catching. Exp Brain Res 201:875–884

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van der Kamp J, Savelsbergh G, Smeets J (1997) Multiple information sources in interceptive timing. Hum Mov Sci 16:787–821

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Soest AJ, Casius LJR, de Kok W, Krijger M, Meeder M, Beek PJ (2010) Are fast interceptive actions continuously guided by vision? Revisiting Bootsma and van Wieringen (1990). J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 36:1040–1055

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vince MA (1948) Corrective movements in a pursuit task. Q J Exp Physiol Cogn Med Sci 1:85–103

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Whiting HTA, Sharp RH (1974) Visual occlusion factors in a discrete ball-catching task. J Mot Behav 6:11–16

    Google Scholar 

  • Whiting HTA, Gill EB, Stephenson JM (1970) Critical time intervals for taking in flight information in a ball-catching task. Ergonomics 13:265–272

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whiting HTA, Alderson GJK, Sanderson FH (1973) Critical time intervals for viewing and individual differences in performance of a ball-catching task. Int J Sports Q 4:155–164

    Google Scholar 

  • Whitwell RL, Goodale MA (2009) Updating the programming of a precision grip is a function of recent history of available feedback. Exp Brain Res 194:619–629

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Whitwell RL, Lambert LM, Goodale MA (2008) Grasping future events: explicit knowledge of the availability of visual feedback fails to reliably influence prehension. Exp Brain Res 188:603–611

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Willingham DB (1998) A neuropsychological theory of motor skill learning. Psychol Rev 105:558–584

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Willingham DB, Nissen MJ, Bullemer P (1989) On the development of procedural knowledge. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 15:1047–1060

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Willingham DB, Salidis J, Gabrieli JDE (2002) Direct comparison of neural systems mediating conscious and unconscious skill learning. J Neurophysiol 88:1451–1460

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wing AM, Turton A, Fraser C (1986) Grasp size and accuracy of approach in reaching. J Mot Behav 18:245–260

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Winges SA, Weber DJ, Santello M (2003) The role of vision on hand preshaping during reach to grasp. Exp Brain Res 152:489–498

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Woodworth RS (1899) The accuracy of voluntary movement. Psychol Rev 3:1–114

    Google Scholar 

  • Zago M, Iosa M, Maffei V, Lacquaniti F (2010) Extrapolation of vertical target motion through a brief visual occlusion. Exp Brain Res 201:365–384

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zelaznik HN, Hawkins B, Kisselburgh L (1983) Rapid visual feedback processing in single-aiming movements. J Mot Behav 15:217–236

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful to Cindy Lowyck and Arnout Sercu for their help in data collection. We also thank two anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments on earlier versions of this paper.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Pieter Tijtgat.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Tijtgat, P., Bennett, S.J., Savelsbergh, G.J.P. et al. To know or not to know: influence of explicit advance knowledge of occlusion on interceptive actions. Exp Brain Res 214, 483–490 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-011-2846-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-011-2846-1

Keywords

Navigation