Skip to main content
Log in

Extending Fitts’ Law to three-dimensional obstacle-avoidance movements: support for the posture-based motion planning model

  • Research Note
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

According to Fitts’ Law, the time (MT) to move to a target is a linear function of the logarithm of the ratio between the target’s distance and width. Although Fitts’ Law accurately predicts MTs for direct movements, it does not accurately predict MTs for indirect movements, as when an obstacle intrudes on the direct movement path. To address this limitation, Jax et al. (2007) added an obstacle-intrusion term to Fitts’ Law. They accurately predicted MTs around obstacles in two-dimensional (2-D) workspaces, but their model had one more parameter than Fitts’ Law did and was merely descriptive. In this study, we addressed these concerns by turning to the mechanistic, posture-based (PB) movement planning model. The PB-based model accounted for almost as much MT variance in a 3-D movement task as the model of Jax et al., with only two parameters, the same number of parameters as in Fitts’ Law.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. In the foregoing, B must be at least as large as the distance the obstacle intrudes (OI) into the workspace. For convenience of exposition, we assume that B simply equals OI.

  2. The six positional degrees of freedom of a rigid object are the x, y, and z values of a reference point within the object such as its center of gravity, and the object’s pitch, roll, and yaw.

  3. It is not unusual for a target to be an obstacle. When reaching for a glass, for example, the glass is an obstacle vis à vis the dorsal side of the hand and fingers. This is why the hand must move around the glass before closing in on it. The PB model was designed to generate such behaviors and simulates them accurately (Rosenbaum et al. 2001).

References

  • Beamish D, Bhatti S, Chubbs CS, MacKenzie IS, Wu J, Jing Z (2009) Estimation of psychomotor delay from the Fitts’ law coefficients. Biol Cyber 101:279–296

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bullock D, Bongers RM, Lankhorst M, Beek PJ (1999) A vector-integration-to-endpoint model for performance of viapoint movements. Neural Netw 12:1–29

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cruse H, Brüwer M, Dean J (1993) Control of three- and four-joint arm movement: strategies for a manipulator with redundant degrees of freedom. J Motor Behav 25:131–139

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elliot D, Helsen WF, Chua R (2001) A century later: Woodworth’s (1899) two component model of goal-directed aiming. Psychol Bull 127:342–357

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer MH, Rosenbaum DA, Vaughan J (1997) Speed and sequential effects in reaching. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 23:404–428

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fitts PM (1954) The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. J Exp Psychol 47:381–391

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fitts PM, Peterson JR (1964) Information capacity of discrete motor responses. J Exp Psychol 67:103–112

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jax SA, Rosenbaum DA, Vaughan (2007) Extending Fitts’ Law to manual obstacle avoidance. Exp Brain Res 180:775–779

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer DE, Abrams RA, Kornblum S, Wright CE, Smith JE (1988) Optimality in human motor performance: ideal control of rapid aimed movements. Psychol Rev 95:340–370

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Murata A, Iwase H (2001) Extending Fitts’ law to a three-dimensional pointing task. Hum Mov Sci 20:791–805

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbaum DA, Loukopoulos LD, Meulenbroek RGJ, Vaughan J, Engelbrecht SE (1995) Planning reaches by evaluating stored postures. Psychol Rev 102:28–67

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbaum DA, Meulenbroek RJ, Vaughan J, Jansen C (2001) Posture-based motion planning: applications to grasping. Psychol Rev 108:709–734

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbaum DA, Cohen R, Dawson A, Jax SA, Meulenbroek RGJ, van der Wel RPRD, Vaughan J (2009) The posture-based motion planning framework: New findings related to object manipulation, moving around obstacles, moving in three spatial dimensions, and haptic tracking. In: Sternad D (ed) Progress in motor control–a multidisciplinary perspective. Springer, Berlin, pp 485–498

    Google Scholar 

  • Sabes PN, Jordan MI, Wolpert DM (1998) The role of inertial sensitivity in motor planning. J Neurosci 18:5948–5957

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shannon C (1948) A mathematical theory of communication. Bell Sys Tech J 27:379–423

    Google Scholar 

  • Tipper SP, Howard LA, Jackson SR (1997) Selective reaching to grasp: evidence for distractor interference effects. Vis Cogn 4:1–38

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vaughan J, Rosenbaum DA, Meulenbroek RGJ (2001) Planning reaching and grasping movements: the problem of obstacle avoidance. Mot Control 5:116–135

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Vaughan J, Rosenbaum DA, Meulenbroek RGJ (2006) Modeling reaching and manipulating in 2- and 3-D workspaces: the posture-based model. In: Proceedings of fifth international conference on development and learning, Indiana University

  • Worringham CJ (1993) Predicting motor performance from variability measures. In: Newell KM, Corcos DM (eds) Variability and motor control. Human Kinetics, Champaign, pp 53–63

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jonathan Vaughan.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Vaughan, J., Barany, D.A., Sali, A.W. et al. Extending Fitts’ Law to three-dimensional obstacle-avoidance movements: support for the posture-based motion planning model. Exp Brain Res 207, 133–138 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-010-2431-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-010-2431-z

Keywords

Navigation