Skip to main content
Log in

Target width scaling in a repetitive aiming task: switching between cyclical and discrete units of action

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

An aiming task was used to identify the processes whereby the motor system adapted a repetitive aiming action to systematic changes in ID (ID = log2 (2A/W), Fitts in J Exp Psychol 47:381–391, 1954) within a single trial. Task ID was scaled in a trial by moving the outside edge of two stationary targets to produce nine different target IDs in a trail. The ID within a trial was scaled in one of two directions: (1) an increasing ID condition, starting with an ID = 3.07 and ending with an ID = 5.91; and (2) a decreasing ID condition, starting with an ID = 5.91 and ending with an ID = 3.07. An index of movement harmonicity (Guiard in Acta Psychol 82:139–159, 1993) revealed that the repetitive aiming action was harmonic in nature when task ID was 3.07, and consisted of a series of discrete segments when task ID was 5.91. This finding provides evidence for the existence of discrete and cyclical units of action that are irreducible and that may be employed independently to assemble longer continuous actions. The scaling of ID within a trial promoted a transition in repetitive aiming motions assembled from discrete and cyclical units of action. A variety of kinematic measures (e.g., movement harmonicity, time spent accelerating the limb) revealed a critical ID (IDc) region (4.01–4.91) separating aiming motions governed by the different units of action. Enhancement of fluctuations before the transition were found in the movement harmonicity data and in the distance traveled to peak velocity data, with variability in these measures highest in the IDc region. The enhancement of fluctuations indicates that loss of stability in the limb’s motion acted as a key mechanism underlying the transition between units of action. The loss of stability was associated with the transition from cyclical to discrete actions and with the transition from discrete to cyclical actions. The transition between units of action may be conceptualized as a transition from a limit cycle attractor (cyclical unit of action) to a shift between two fixed-point attractors (discrete unit of action) when ID was increased, with the transition occurring in the opposite direction when ID was decreased.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Fig. 8

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Movement amplitude (A) is measured in a Fitts task from target center to target center. To keep A fixed based on this measurement approach would have required the inside and outside edges of the targets to change together. This may have produced a large visual perturbation that may have been interpreted as a change in target position. For this reason, the inside edges of the targets were fixed at a constant amplitude to reduce the perceptual impact of scaling W within a trial. As a result, the change in W by the same 0.87° by moving the back edge of the target produced < a 1 bit change for each change in target size, but produced approximately a 3 bit change across the experiment. Even though A varied in the task, since shifting the target’s outer edge changed W, the change in A was limited to a 3% change for each change in W.

  2. In the current task, the change in ID was manipulated to control for visual perturbations of the target’s position that have been shown to influence aiming behavior (Elliott et al. 1999; Heath et al. 1998). In order to provide more support for the conclusion that the scaling of task ID resulted in transitions between units of action, it is necessary to utilize other scaling techniques that produce equal changes in ID around the IDc. For example, moving both edges of the target such that ID increases in equal bit steps such as 2, 2.5, 3, 3.5 etc. An equal step size increase in ID may produce more abrupt shifts in measures like movement harmonicity as predicted by static ID conditions (Guiard 1997; Mottet et al. 2001).

References

  • Abend W, Bizzi E, Morasso P (1982) Human arm trajectory formation. Brain 105:331–348

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Adam JJ, Paas FGWC (1996) Dwell time in reciprocal aiming tasks. Hum Mov Sci 15:1–24

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Adam JJ, van der Bruggen DPW, Bekkering H (1993) The control of discrete and reciprocal target-aiming responses: evidence for the exploitation of mechanics. Hum Mov Sci 12:353–364

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Adamovich SV, Levin MF, Feldman AG (1994) Merging different motor patterns: coordination between rhythmical and discrete single-joint movements. Exp Brain Res 99:325–337

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Asatryan DG, Feldman AG (1965) Functional tuning of nervous system with control of movement or maintenance of steady posture, I. Mechanographic analysis of the work of the joint on execution of a postural task. Biophysics 10:925–934

    Google Scholar 

  • Athenes S, Sallagoity I, Zanone PG, Albaret JM (2004) Evaluating the coordination dynamics of handwriting. Hum Mov Sci 23:621–641

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Billon M, Bootsma RJ, Mottet D (2000) The dynamics of human isometric pointing movements under varying accuracy requirements. Neurosci Lett 286:49–52

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Buchanan JJ, Kelso JAS (1993) Posturally induced transitions in rhythmic multijoint limb movements. Exp Brain Res 94:131–142

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Buchanan JJ, Ryu YU (2005) The interaction of tactile information and movement amplitude in a multijoint bimanual circle-tracing task: phase transitions and loss of stability. Q J Exp Psychol 50A:769–787

    Google Scholar 

  • Buchanan JJ, Kelso JAS, deGuzman GD (1997a) Self-organization of trajectory information. Biol Cybern 76:257–273

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Buchanan JJ, Kelso JAS, deGuzman GD, Ding M (1997b) The spontaneous recruitment and suppression of degrees of freedom in rhythmic hand movements. Hum Mov Sci 16:1–32

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buchanan JJ, Park J-H, Ryu YU, Shea CH (2003) Discrete and cyclical units of action in a mixed target pair aiming task. Exp Brain Res 150:473–489

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buchanan JJ, Park J-H, Shea CH (2004) Systematic scaling of target width: dynamics, planning, and feedback. Neurosci Lett 367:317–322

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Byblow WD, Carson RG, Goodman D (1994) Expressions of asymmetries and anchoring in bimanual coordination. Hum Mov Sci 13:3–28

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carson RG, Goodman D, Kelso JAS, Elliott D (1995) Phase transitions and critical fluctuations in rhythmic coordination of ipsilateral hand and foot. J Mot Behav 27:211–224

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen J (1971) Synchronous bimanual movements performed by homologous and non-homologous muscles. Percept Mot Skills 32:639–644

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Crossman ERFW, Goodeve PJ (1983) Feedback control of hand-movement and Fitts’ law. Q J Exp Psychol 35A:251–278

    Google Scholar 

  • Cullen JD, Helsen WF, Buekers MJ, Hesketh KL, Starkes JL, Elliott D (2001) The utilization of visual information in the control of reciprocal aiming movements. Hum Mov Sci 20:807–828

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • deGuzman GD, Kelso JAS, Buchanan JJ (1997) Self-organization of trajectory formation. Biol Cybern 76:275–284

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Elliott D, Binsted G, Heath M (1999) The control of goal-directed limb movements: correcting error in the trajectory. Hum Mov Sci 18:121–136

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman AG (1986) Once more on the equilibrium point hypothesis (λ-model) for motor control. J Mot Behav 18:17–54

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fitts PM (1954) The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. J Exp Psychol 47:381–391

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Grafton ST, Mazziotta JC, Woods RP, Phelps ME (1992) Human functional anatomy of visually guided finger movements. Brain 115:565–587

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Guiard Y (1993) On Fitts’s and Hooke’s laws: simple harmonic movement in upper-limb cyclical aiming. Acta Psychol 82:139–159

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Guiard Y (1997) Fitts’ law in the discrete vs. cyclical paradigm. Hum Mov Sci 16:97–131

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haken H (1983) Synergetics. Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Haken H, Kelso JAS, Bunz H (1985) A theoretical model of phase transitions in human hand movements. Biol Cybern 51:347–356

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Haken H, Peper CE, Beek PJ, Daffertshofer A (1996) A model for phase transitions in human hand movements during multifrequency tapping. Physica D 90:179–196

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heath M, Hodges NJ, Chua R, Elliott D (1998) On-line control of rapid aiming movements: Unexpected target perturbations and movement kinematics. Can J Exp Psychol 52:163–173

    Google Scholar 

  • Hollerbach JM (1981) An oscillation theory of handwriting. Biol Cybern 39:139–156

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jirsa VK, Kelso JAS (2004) The excitator as a minimal model for the coordination dynamics of discrete and rhythmic movement generation. J Mot Behav 37:35–51

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelso JAS (1984) Phase transitions and critical behavior in human bimanual coordination. Am J Physiol [Reg Integ Comp] 15:R1000–R1004

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelso JAS (1992) Theoretical concepts and strategies for understanding perceptual-motor skill: from information capacity in closed systems to self-organization in open, nonequilibrium systems. J Exp Psychol Gen 121:260–262

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kelso JAS (1995) Dynamic patterns: the self-organization of brain and behavior. The MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelso JAS, Jeka JJ (1992) Symmetry breaking dynamics of human multilimb coordination. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 18:645–668

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kelso JAS, Scholz JAS, Schöner G (1986) Nonequilibrium phase transitions in coordinated biological motion: critical fluctuations. Phys Lett A 118:279–284

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelso JAS, Buchanan JJ, Wallace SA (1991) Order parameters for the neural organization of single limb, multijoint movement patterns. Exp Brain Res 85:432–445

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kelso JAS, Buchanan JJ, deGuzman GC, Ding M (1993). Spontaneous recruitment and annihilation of degrees of freedom in biological coordination. Phys Lett A 179:364–371

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Khan MA, Franks IM (2000) The effect of practice on component submovements is dependent on the availability of visual feedback. J Mot Behav 32:227–240

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Khan MA, Franks IM (2003) Online versus offline processing of visual feedback in the production of component submovements. J Mot Behav 35:285–295

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Khan MA, Franks IM, Goodman D (1998) The effect of practice on the control of rapid aiming movements: evidence for an interdependency between programming and feedback processing. Q J Exp Psychol 51A:425–444

    Google Scholar 

  • Krebs HI, Aisen ML, Volpe BT, Hogan N (1999) Quantization of continuous arm movements in humans with brain injury. Neurobiology 96:4645–4649

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer DE, Kornblum S, Abrams RA, Wright CE, Smith JEK (1988) Optimality in human motor performance: ideal control of rapid aimed movements. Psychol Rev 95:340–370

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Morasso P (1981) Spatial control of arm movements. Exp Brain Res 42:223–227

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mottet D, Bootsma RJ (1999) The dynamics of goal-directed rhythmical aiming. Biol Cybern 80:235–245

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mottet D, Guiard Y, Ferrand T, Bootsma RJ (2001) Two-handed performance of a rhythmical Fitts’ task by individuals and dyads. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 27:1275–1286

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mourik AM, Beek PJ (2004) Discrete and cyclical movements: unified dynamics or separate control. Acta Psychol 117:121–138

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peper CE, Beek PJ, van Wieringen PCW (1995) Multifrequency coordination in bimanual tapping: asymmetrical coupling and signs of supercriticality. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 21:1117–1138

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plamondon R, Alimi AM (1997) Speed/accuracy trade-offs in target-directed movements. Behav Brain Sci 20:279–349

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • de Rugy A, Sternad D (2003) Interaction between discrete and rhythmic movements: reaction time and phase of discrete movement initiation during oscillatory movements. Brain Res 994:160–174

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ryu YU, Buchanan JJ (2004) Amplitude scaling in a bimanual circle drawing task: pattern switching and end-effector variability. J Mot Behav 36:265–275

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schaal S, Sternad D, Osu R, Kawato M (2004) Rhythmic arm movement is not discrete. Nat Neurosci 7:1137–1144

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt RC, Carello C, Turvey MT (1990) Phase transitions and critical fluctuations in the visual coordination of rhythmic movements between people. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 16:227–247

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schöner G (1990) A dynamic theory of coordination of discrete movement. Biol Cybern 63:257–270

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schöner G (1994) From interlimb coordination to trajectory formation: common dynamical principles. In: Swinnen SP, Heuer H, Massion J, Casaer P (eds) Interlimb coordination: neural, dynamical, and cognitive constraints. Academic, Boston, pp 339–368

    Google Scholar 

  • Schöner G, Kelso JAS (1988) Dynamic pattern generation in behavioral and neural systems. Science 239:1513–1520

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schöner G, Haken H, Kelso JAS (1986) A stochastic theory of phase transitions in human hand movement. Biol Cybern 53:247–257

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Soechting JF, Terzuolo CA (1987) Organization of arm movements in three-dimensional space. Wrist motion is piecewise planar. Neuroscience 23:53–61

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sternad D, Dean WJ (2003) Rhythmic and discrete elements in multi-joint coordination. Brain Res 989:152–171

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sternad D, Schaal S (1999) Segmentation of endpoint trajectories does not imply segmented control. Exp Brain Res 124:118–136

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sternad D, Dean WJ, Schaal S (2000) Interaction of rhythmic and discrete pattern generators in single-joint movements. Hum Mov Sci 19:627–664

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thoroughman KA, Shadmehr R (2000) Learning of action through adaptive combination of motor primitives. Nature 407:742–747

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Turvey MT (1990) Coordination. Am Psychol 45:938–953

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Winstein CJ, Pohl PS (1995) Effects of unilateral brain damage on the control of goal-directed hand movements. Exp Brain Res 105:163–174

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Winstein CJ, Grafton ST, Pohl PS (1997) Motor task difficulty and brain activity: investigation of goal-directed aiming using positron emission tomography. J Neurophysiol 77:1581–1594

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to John J. Buchanan.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Buchanan, J.J., Park, JH. & Shea, C.H. Target width scaling in a repetitive aiming task: switching between cyclical and discrete units of action. Exp Brain Res 175, 710–725 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-006-0589-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-006-0589-1

Keywords

Navigation