Skip to main content
Log in

Aiming for the future: prospective action difficulty, prescribed difficulty, and Fitts’ law

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

A main purpose of the current investigation was to determine if Fitts’ index of difficulty [log2(2A/W)] could be taken as an index of subjective difficulty in prospective action. In two experiments, participants viewed 12 target displays with values of log2(2A/W) (prescribed difficulty) ranging between 1.0 and 6.5 bits. Following each 15 s trial, participants provided magnitude estimates reflecting the difficulty that someone else would experience if they actually had to make targeted movements during the trial. The prospective difficulty estimates were always made in the absence of movement. In Experiment 1, target displays were presented to participants on their own video monitor, while in Experiment 2, all participants concurrently viewed scaled-up target displays projected onto a large screen. There were three main findings: First, in both experiments, the prospective-prescribed relation was strong and positive. This finding warrants two conclusions: Fitts’ index of difficulty can be taken as an index of subjective difficulty in prospective action, and subjective estimates of performance difficulty result from the monitoring of feedforward control signals generated in the absence of movement-related feedback. Second, even with the large differences in the target display scale of Experiments 1 and 2, difficulty estimates were equivalent at common prescribed difficulty levels. In other words, the results of Experiment 1 were successfully replicated in Experiment 2. This finding demonstrates the generality of the prospective-prescribed relation. Third, nonlinearities in the prospective-prescribed relation resembled those seen in functions describing the increases in movement time that accompany increases in prescribed difficulty (Fitts’ law). This observation suggests that the prospective difficulty estimates were based on the value of a temporal parameter in an implicit mental simulation of the action.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The original purpose of instructing participants to make prospective difficulty judgments from a third-person perspective was to maximize the implicit nature of the task. We thought that if participants were asked to make judgments from a first-person perspective, then this would increase the probability of them engaging in explicit action simulation. However, because prospective action judgment is known to engage implicit action simulation (e.g. Johnson 2000), which in all likelihood involves a first-person perspective, and because we did not provide a specific third-person model, it seems probable that our participants defaulted to a first-person perspective. In contrast, in other studies on perspective taking in imagined action, the experimenter has served as an explicit third-person model for participants’ action simulations (Ruby and Decety 2001; Sirigu and Duhamel 2001). In those studies, as compared with the current study, it would have been easier for participants to form a third-person, experimenter image that was distinct from the participants’ first-person image.

References

  • Baird JC, Lewis C, Romer D (1970) Relative frequencies of numerical responses in ratio estimation. Percept Psychophys 8:358–362

    Google Scholar 

  • Blakemore SJ, Wolpert DM, Frith CD (2002) Abnormalities in the awareness of action. Trends Cognit Sci 6:237–242

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Borg G (1978) Subjective aspects of physical and mental load. Ergonomics 21:215–220

    Google Scholar 

  • Danckert D, Ferber S, Doherty T, Steinmetz H, Nicolle D, Goodale M (2002) Selective, non-lateralized impairment of motor imagery following right parietal damage. Neurocase 8:194–204

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Decety J, Jeannerod M (1996) Mentally simulated movements in virtual reality: does Fitts’s law hold in motor imagery? Behav Brain Res 72:127–134

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deligniéres D (1993) La perception de l’effort et de la difficulte. In: Famose JP (eds) Cognition et performance. Publications INSEP, Paris, pp 183–218

    Google Scholar 

  • Deligniéres D, Brisswalter J (1996) The perception of difficulty and exertion in motor tasks: what can be known about perceptual continua through individual psychophysical exponents? J Hum Mov Studies 30:213–239

    Google Scholar 

  • Desmurget M, Grafton S (2000) Forward modeling allows feedback control for fast reaching movements. Trends Cognit Sci 4:423–431

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de’Sperati C, Stucchi N (2000) Motor imagery and visual event recognition. Exp Brain Res 133:273–278

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fitts PM (1954) The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. J Exp Psychol 47:381–391

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Frak V, Paulignan Y, Jeannerod M (2001) Orientation of the opposition axis in mentally simulated grasping. Exp Brain Res 136:120–127

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gopher D, Braune R (1984) On the psychophysics of workload: why bother with subjective measures? Hum Factors 26:519–532

    Google Scholar 

  • Jagacinski RL, Monk DL (1985) Fitts’ law in two dimensions with hand and head movements. J Mot Behav 17:77–95

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Jeannerod M (1997) The cognitive neuroscience of action. Blackwell, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Jeannerod M (2001) Neural simulation of action: a unifying mechanism for motor cognition. Neuroimage 14:103–109

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jeannerod M, Frak V (1999) Mental imaging of motor activity in humans. Curr Opin Neurobiol 9:735–739

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson SH (1998) Cerebral organization in motor imagery: contralateral control of grip selection in mentally represented prehension. Psychol Sci 9:219–222

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson SH (2000) Thinking ahead: the case for motor imagery in prospective judgments of prehension. Cognition 74:33–70

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kawato M (1999) Internal models for motor control and trajectory planning. Curr Opin Neurobiol 9:718–727

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Keele SW (1968) Movement control in skilled performance. Psychol Bull 70:387–403

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kerr B (1973) Movement time in an underwater environment. J Mot Behav 5:175–178

    Google Scholar 

  • Langolf GD, Chaffin DB, Foulke JA (1976) An investigation of Fitts’ law using a wide range of movement amplitudes. J Mot Behav 8:113–128

    Google Scholar 

  • Maruff P, Velakoulis D (2000) The voluntary control of motor imagery. Imagined movements in individuals with feigned motor impairment and conversion disorder. Neuropsychologia 38:1251–1260

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer DE, Abrams RA, Kornblum S, Wright CE, Smith JEK (1988) Optimality in human motor performance: ideal control of rapid aimed movements. Psychol Rev 89:449–482

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parsons LM (1987) Imagined spatial transformations of one’s hands and feet. Cognit Psychol

  • Parsons LM (1994) Temporal and kinematic properties of motor behavior reflected in mentally simulated action. J Exp Psychol: Hum Percept Perform 20:709–730

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Plamondon R, Alimi AM (1997) Speed/accuracy trade-offs in target-directed movements. Behav Brain Sci 20:279–349

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ruby P, Decety J (2001) Effect of subjective perspective taking during simulation of action: a PET investigation of agency. Nat Neurosci 4:546–550

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt RA, Lee TD (1999) Motor control and learning: a behavioral emphasis, 3rd edn. Human Kinetics, Champaign, IL

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwoebel J, Boronat CB, Coslett HB (2002) The man who executed “imagined” movements. Brain Cogn 50:1–16

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sirigu A, Duhamel JR (2001) Motor and visual imagery as two complementary but neurally dissociable mental processes. J Cogn Neurosci 13:910–919

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sirigu A, Cohen L, Duhamel JR, Pillon B, Dubois B, Agid Y, Pierrot-Deseilligny C (1995) Congruent unilateral impairments for real and imagined hand movements. Neuroreport 6:997–1001

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sirigu A, Duhamel JR, Cohen L, Pillon B, Dubois B, Agid Y (1996) The mental representation of hand movements after parietal cortex damage. Science 273:1564–1568

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Slifkin AB, Brener J (1998) Control of operant response force. J Exp Psychol: Anim Behav Process 24:431–438

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Stevens SS (1956) The direct estimation of sensory magnitudes—loudness. The Am J Psychol 69:1–25

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Stevens SS (1957) On the psychophysical law. Psychol Rev 64:153–181

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Stevens SS (1975) Psychophysics: introduction to its perceptual, neural, and social prospects. Wiley, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Welford AT (1968) Fundamentals of skill. Methuen, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson PH, Maruff P, Ives S, Currie J (2001) Abnormalities of motor and praxis imagery in children with DCD. Hum Mov Sci 20:135–159

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wolpert DM, Flanagan JR (2001) Motor prediction. Curr Biol 11:R729–R732

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Andrew B. Slifkin.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Slifkin, A.B., Grilli, S.M. Aiming for the future: prospective action difficulty, prescribed difficulty, and Fitts’ law. Exp Brain Res 174, 746–753 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-006-0518-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-006-0518-3

Keywords

Navigation