Skip to main content
Log in

Switching, plasticity, and prediction in a saccadic task-switch paradigm

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Several cognitive processes are involved in task-switching. Using a prosaccade/antisaccade paradigm, we manipulated both the interval available for preparation between the cue and the target and the predictability of trial sequences, to isolate the contributions of foreknowledge, an active switching (reconfiguration) process, and passive inhibitory effects persisting from the prior trial. We tested 15 subjects with both a random and a regularly alternating trial sequence. Half of the trials had a short cue–target interval of 200 ms, and half a longer cue–target interval of 2,000 ms. When there was only a short preparatory interval, switching increased the latencies for both prosaccades and antisaccades. With a long preparatory interval, switching was associated with a smaller latency increase for prosaccades and, importantly, a paradoxical reduction in latency for antisaccades. Foreknowledge of a predictable sequence did not allow subjects to reduce switch costs in the manner that a long preparatory cue–target interval did. In the trials with short preparatory intervals, the effects on latency attributable to active reconfiguration processes were similar for prosaccades and antisaccades. We propose a model in which the passive inhibitory effects that persist from the prior saccadic trial are due not to task-set inertia, in which one task-set inhibits the opposite task-set, but to inhibition of the saccadic response-system by the antisaccade task, to account for the paradoxical set-switch benefit for antisaccades at long cue–target intervals. Our findings regarding foreknowledge show that previous studies used to support task-set inertia may have conflated the effects of both active reconfiguration and passive inhibitory processes on latency. While our model of response-system plasticity can explain a number of effects of dominance asymmetry in switching, other models fail to account for the paradoxical set-switch benefit for antisaccades.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Allport A, Styles E, Hsieh S (1994) Shifting intentional set: exploring the dynamic control of tasks. In: Umiltà C, Moscovitch M (eds) Attention and performance, 15th edn. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp 421–452

    Google Scholar 

  • Barton J, Cherkasova M, Lindgren K, Goff D, Manoach D (2005) What is perseverated in schizophrenia? Evidence of abnormal response plasticity in the saccadic system. J Abnorm Psychol 114:75–84

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cherkasova M, Manoach D, Intriligator J, Barton J (2002) Antisaccades and task-shifting: interactions in controlled processing. Exp Brain Res 144:528–537

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Jong R (2000) An intention-activation account of residual switch costs. In: Monsell S, Driver J (eds) Control of cognitive processes: attention and performance, 18th edn. MIT, Cambridge, MA, pp 35–70

    Google Scholar 

  • Dorris M, Munoz D (1998) Saccadic probability influences motor preparation signals and time to saccadic inhibition. J Neurosci 18:7015–7026

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Dorris M, Paré M, Munoz D (2000) Immediate neural plasticity shapes motor performance. J Neurosci 20:RC52

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Everling S, Munoz DP (2000) Neuronal correlates for preparatory set associated with pro-saccades and anti-saccades in the primate frontal eye field. J Neurosci 20:387–400

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Everling S, Dorris MC, Klein RM, Munoz DP (1999) Role of primate superior colliculus in preparation and execution of anti-saccades and pro-saccades. J Neurosci 19:2740–2754

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fecteau J, Munoz D (2003) Exploring the consequences of the previous trial. Nat Rev Neurosci 4:435–443

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fecteau J, Au C, Armstrong I, Munoz D (2004) Sensory biases produce alternation advantage found in sequential saccadic eye movement tasks. Exp Brain Res 159:84–91

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Forbes K, Klein R (1996) The magnitude of the fixation offset effect with endogenously and exogenously controlled saccades. J Cogn Neurosci 8:344–352

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hallett P (1978) Primary and secondary saccades to goals defined by instructions. Vision Res 18:1279–1296

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hunt A, Klein R (2002) Eliminating the costs of task set reconfiguration. Mem Cogn (in press)

  • Kalesnykas R, Hallett P (1987) The differentiation of visually guided and anticipatory saccades in gap and overlap paradigms. Exp Brain Res 68:115–121

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Manoach DS, Lindgren KA, Cherkasova MV, Goff DC, Halpern EF, Intriligator J, Barton JJS (2002) Schizophrenic subjects show deficient inhibition but intact task-switching on saccadic tasks. Biol Psychiatry 51:816–825

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Manoach D, Lindgren K, Barton J (2004) Deficient saccadic inhibition in Asperger’s disorder and the social-emotional processing disorder. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 75:1719–1726

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Meiran N (1996) Reconfiguration of processing mode prior to task performance. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 22:1423–1442

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meiran N (2000) Modeling cognitive control in task switching. Psychol Res 63:234–249

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Meiran N, Chorev Z, Sapir A (2000) Component processes in task switching. Cognit Psychol 41:211–253

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Monsell S, Yeung N, Azuma R (2000) Reconfiguration of task-set: is it easier to switch to the weaker task?. Psychol Res 63:250–264

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Munoz D, Everling S (2004) Look away: the anti-saccade task and the voluntary control of eye movement. Nat Rev Neurosci 5:218–228

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Nieuwenhuis S, Monsell S (2002) Residual costs in task switching: testing the failure-to-engage hypothesis. Psychon Bull Rev 9:86–92

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers RD, Monsell S (1995) Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. J Exp Psychol Gen 124:207–231

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sohn M-H, Anderson J (2001) Task preparation and task repetition: two-component model of task-switching. J Exp Psychol Gen 130:764–778

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Tornay FJ, Milan EG (2001) A more complete task-set reconfiguration in random than in predictable task switch. Q J Exp Psychol A 54:785–803

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wylie G, Allport A (2000) Task switching and the measurement of “switch costs”. Psychol Res 63:212–233

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Yeung N, Monsell S (2003) Switching between tasks of unequal familiarity: the role of stimulus- attribute and response-set selection. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 29:455–469

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jason J. S. Barton.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Barton, J.J.S., Greenzang, C., Hefter, R. et al. Switching, plasticity, and prediction in a saccadic task-switch paradigm. Exp Brain Res 168, 76–87 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-005-0091-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-005-0091-1

Keywords

Navigation