Skip to main content
Log in

School friendship networks, homophily and multiculturalism: evidence from European countries

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Population Economics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This paper investigates the determinants of school friendship networks among adolescents, proposing a model of network formation and estimating it using a sample of about 10,000 secondary school students in four countries: England, Germany, the Netherlands and Sweden. We test the idea that networks arise according to homophily along many characteristics (gender, school achievement and ethnic and cultural backgrounds), and assess the relative importance of each factor. In addition to gender, we find that country of origin, generational status and religion predict friendship for foreign-born students. For individuals born in the survey country, ties depend on a broader set of factors, including socioeconomic status and school achievement. In sum, homophilic preferences go considerably beyond ethnicity. Multiculturalism, which gives prominence to ethnic backgrounds, risks emphasising the differences in that dimension at the expense of affinity in others.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. http://edition.cnn.com/2011/US/01/27/pennsyation

  2. This way of operationalizing socioeconomic conditions takes inspiration from the PISA survey (OECD 2017), which collects information on students’ parental education in years, highest parental occupation and number of home possessions (including books). The CILS4EU survey did not ask students the number of years spent by their parents in education but only their highest level of educational attainment.

  3. The ceiling on the number of nominations is a possible limitation of the data since it may prevent identification of an individual’s entire reference group. It also means that students’ weak ties (i.e. ties that reflect less frequent, often instrumental relationships) are not considered. About 94% and 97% of students were linked to less than five friends at wave 1 and wave 2, respectively; 62% and 69% indicated fewer than five names.

  4. Additional estimates by gender are show in Appendix A.1.1. The role of socioeconomic factors is stronger for males than for females, and the coefficient on second-generation immigration status is significant for males only.

  5. Variations in sample size are due to unanswered survey questions.

References

  • Alesina A, La Ferrara E (2000) Participation in heterogeneous communities. Q J Econ 115(3):847–904

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alesina A, Murard E, Rapoport H (2019) Immigration and preferences for redistribution in europe. NBER Working Paper no. 25562

  • Algan Y, Hémet C, Laitin DD (2016) The social effects of ethnic diversity at the local level: A natural experiment with exogenous residential allocation. J Polit Econ 124(3):696–733

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baum CF, Schaffer ME, Stillman S (2003) Instrumental variables and gmm: Estimation and testing. Stata J 3(1):1–13

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baum CF, Schaffer ME, Stillman S (2007) Enhanced routines for instrumental variables/generalized method of moments estimation and testing. Stata J 7(4):465–506

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Block P, Grund T (2014) Multidimensional homophily in friendship networks. Netw Sci 2(2):189–212

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Böhlmark A (2008) Age at immigration and school performance: A siblings analysis using swedish register data. Labour Econ 15(6):1366–1387

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bramoullé Y, Djebbari H, Fortin B (2009) Identification of peer effects through social networks. J Econ 150(1):41–55

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Calvó-Armengol A, Patacchini E, Zenou Y (2009) Peer effects and social networks in education. Rev Econ Stud 76(4):1239–1267

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cameron AC, Gelbach JB, Miller DL (2011) Robust inference with multiway clustering. J Bus Econ Stat 29(2):238–249

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cameron AC, Miller DL (2014) Robust inference for dyadic data. working paper

  • Comola M, Fafchamps M (2013) Testing unilateral and bilateral link formation. Econ J 124(579):954–976

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Comola M, Mendola M (2015) Formation of migrant networks. Scand J Econ 117(2):592–618

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Currarini S, Jackson MO, Pin P (2009) An economic model of friendship: Homophily, minorities, and segregation. Econometrica 77(4):1003–1045

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Currarini S, Jackson MO, Pin P (2010) Identifying the roles of race-based choice and chance in high school friendship network formation. Proc Natl Acad Sci 107(11):4857–4861

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Currarini S, Matheson J, Vega-Redondo F (2016) A simple model of homophily in social networks. Eur Econ Rev 90:18–39

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Melo G (2014) Peer effects identified through social networks: Evidence from uruguayan schools. no. 2014/05

  • Dollmann J, Konstanze J (2016) Examining the diversity of youth in europe: A classification of generations and ethnic origins using cils4eu data. a wave 2-update. Working Paper, Mannheim University

  • DuBois DL, Hirsch BJ (1990) School and neighborhood friendship patterns of blacks and whites in early adolescence. Child Dev 61(2):524–536

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dustmann C, Frattini T, Lanzara G, Algan Y (2012) Educational achievement of second-generation immigrants: an international comparison. Econ Policy 27(69):143–185

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Epstein JL (1986) Friendship selection: Developmental and environmental influences. In: Mueller EC, Cooper CR (eds) Process and Outcome in Peer Relationships. Academic Press, pp 129–160

  • Eurostat (2019) Migration and migrant population statistics. http://ec.europa.eu/eurostat/statistics-explained/index.php/Migration_and_migrant_population_statistics. Retrieved: 03/2019

  • Fafchamps M, Gubert F (2007) The formation of risk sharing networks. J Dev Econ 83(2):326–350

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frattini T, Meschi E (2019) The effect of immigrant peers in vocational schools. Eur Econ Rev 113:1–22

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodreau SM, Kitts JA, Morris M (2009) Birds of a feather, or friend of a friend? using exponential random graph models to investigate adolescent social networks. Demography 46(1):103–125

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Granovetter MS (1973) The strength of weak ties. Am J Sociol 78(6):1360–1380

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Granovetter MS (1983) The strength of weak ties: A network theory revisited. Sociol Theory 1:201–233

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hallinan MT, Teixeira RA (1987) Students’ interracial friendships: Individual characteristics, structural effects, and racial differences. Am J Educ 95 (4):563–583

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hartup WW (1983) Peer relations. In: Hetherington E M, Mussen P H (eds) Handbook of child psychology, vol. 4: Socialization, Personality, and Social Development. Wiley, pp 129–160

  • Kalter F, Heath AF, Hewstone M, Jonsson JO, Kalmijn M, Kogan I, van Tubergen F (2016) Children of immigrants longitudinal survey in four european countries (cils4eu) – reduced version. https://doi.org/10.4232/cils4eu.5656.2.3.0

  • Karweit N, Hansell S (1983) Sex differences in adolescent relationships: Friendship and status. In: Epstein JL, Karweit N (eds) Friends in School: Patterns of Selection and Influence in Secondary Schools. Academic Press, pp 115–130

  • Kim YY (2001) Becoming intercultural: An integrative theory of communication and cross-cultural adaptation. Sage Publications

  • Kleibergen F, Paap R (2006) Generalized reduced rank tests using the singular value decomposition. J Econ 133(1):97–126

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kymlicka W (1995) Multicultural citizenship: A liberal theory of minority rights. Clarendon Press

  • Kymlicka W (2007) Multicultural odysseys. Ethnopolitics 6 (4):585–597

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lazarsfeld PF, Merton RK (1954) Friendship as a social process: A substantive and methodological analysis. In: Berger M, Abel T, Page CH (eds) Freedom and Control in Modern Society. Van Nostrand, pp 18–66

  • Lin X (2010) Identifying peer effects in student academic achievement by spatial autoregressive models with group unobservables. J Labor Econ 28 (4):825–860

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Malik K (2015) The failure of multiculturalism: Community versus society in europe. Foreign Aff 94:21

    Google Scholar 

  • Manski CF (1993) Identification of endogenous social effects: The reflection problem. Rev Econ Stud 60(3):531–542

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McPherson M, Smith-Lovin L, Cook JM (2001) Birds of a feather: Homophily in social networks. Annu Rev Sociol 427:415–444

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • OECD (2017) Pisa 2015 technical report. http://www.oecd.org/pisa/sitedocument/PISA-2015-technical-report-final.pdf. Retrieved: 03/2019

  • Patacchini E, Rainone E, Zenou Y (2017) Heterogeneous peer effects in education. J Econ Behav Organ 134:190–227

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Putnam RD (2007) E pluribus unum: Diversity and community in the twenty-first century. the 2006 johan skytte prize lecture. Scand Polit Stud 30 (2):137–174

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rangvid BS (2007) Sources of immigrants-underachievement: Results from pisa-copenhagen. Educ Econ 15(3):293–326

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rapallini C, Rustichini A (2016) Elective affinities matter as much as ethnicity in multi-ethnic schools. J Econ Behav Organ 131:243–262

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rapallini C, Rustichini A (2019) Personality and cognitive skills in network of friends, for multi-ethnic schools. Res Econ 73(1):1–14

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rattansi A (2011) Multiculturalism: A very short introduction. Oxford University Press

  • Rogers EM, Bhowmik DK (1970) Homophily-heterophily: Relational concepts for communication research. Publ Opinion Quart 34(4):523–538

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Senik C, Stichnoth H, van der Straeten K (2009) Immigration and natives-attitudes towards the welfare state: Evidence from the european social survey. Soc Indic Res 91:345–370

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith S, Maas I, Van Tubergen F (2014) Ethnic ingroup friendships in schools: Testing the by-product hypothesis in england, germany, the netherlands and sweden. Soc Netw 39:33–45

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Staiger D, Stock JH (1997) Instrumental variables regression with weak instruments. Econometrica 65(3):557–586

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tabord-Meehan M (2019) Inference with dyadic data: Asymptotic behavior of the dyadic-robust t-statistic. J Bus Econ Stat 37(4):671–680

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van der Meer T, Tolsma J (2014) Ethnic diversity and its effects on social cohesion. Annu Rev Sociol 40:459–478

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer A, Lewis K (2010) Beyond and below racial homophily: Erg models of a friendship network documented on facebook. Am J Sociol 116(2):583–642

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zilliacus H, Holm G, Sahlström F (2017) Taking steps towards institutionalising multicultural education–the national curriculum of finland. Multicult Educ Rev 9(4):231–248

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Chiara Rapallini.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Responsible editor: Klaus F. Zimmermann

Publisher’s note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Earlier versions of this paper were presented at the 2nd Florence-Konstanz workshop on Behavioural and Experimental Social Sciences (University of Florence) and the 2nd International CILS4EU User Conference (Utrecht University). We thank participants at these meetings, Paolo Brunori, Editor Klaus F. Zimmermann and two anonymous referees for their comments and suggestions. Aldo Rustichini thanks the National Science Foundation for the grant SES 1728056, “Rules based policies, and Intelligence in Strateic Behavior”.

Appendix

Appendix

1.1 A.1 Additional estimates

1.1.1 A.1.1 Estimates by gender

Table 12 Benchmark estimates by gender

1.1.2 A.1.2 First-stage regressions

Table 13 Estimates (cultural traits, first stage)
Table 14 Consistency check (cultural traits, first stage)

1.2 A.2 Questionnaire items

The CILS4EU questionnaire is structured in several sections. Our variables were constructed from information assessed in the sections named ‘Youth Main’ (YM), ‘Youth Classmates’ (YC) and ‘Youth Parents’ (YP).

  • YM-1 Are you a boy or a girl?

  • YM-2 When were you born?

  • YM-3 In which country were you born?

  • YM-4 How old were you when you moved to <survey country>?

  • YM-19 Which grades did you get in the last school year in the following subjects?

    • (a) Mathematics

    • (b) <Survey country language>

  • YM-27 Did your mother complete university? [Yes; No; Don’t know]

  • YM-33 Did your father complete university? [Yes; No; Don’t know]

  • YM-63 In a family, who should do the following?

    • (a) Earn money [Mostly the man; Mostly the woman; Both about the same]

    • (b) Clean the house [Mostly the man; Mostly the woman; Both about the same]

  • YM-66 How strongly do you feel <survey country member>? [Very strongly; Fairly strongly; Not very strongly; Not at all strongly]

  • YM-71 What is your religion? [No religion; Buddhism; Christianity; Christianity: Catholic; Christianity: Protestant; Hinduism; Islam; Judaism; Sikh; Other: specify]

  • YM-75 How much do you agree or disagree with each of these statements?

    • (a) The <survey country> people should be open to the customs and traditions of immigrants [Strongly agree; Agree; Neither agree nor disagree; Disagree; Strongly disagree]

    • (b) Immigrants should do all they can to keep their customs and traditions [Strongly agree; Agree; Neither agree nor disagree; Disagree; Strongly disagree]

  • YM-95 How many rooms are there in your home (not counting kitchen and bathroom)?

  • YM-96 About how many books are there in your home? [0-25; 26-100; 101-200; 201-500; More than 500]

  • YC-1 Who are your best friends in class? Here you may write down no more than five names.

  • YC-2 Who is your best friend in class? Here you may write down no more than one number.

  • YC-9 Which classmates live within a 5 minute walk from your home?

  • YP-9 How strongly do you feel <survey country member>? [Very strongly; Fairly strongly; Not very strongly; Not at all strongly]

1.3 A.3 Variables description

Dependent variables and controls for confounding factors

  • Undirected link (five best friends). Binary: 1 if a friendship nomination from Ego to Alter is reciprocated, as assessed by question YC-1.

  • Undirected link (very best friends). Binary: 1 if a friendship nomination from Ego to Alter is reciprocated, as assessed by question YC-2.

  • Shares of students with same characteristics. Share of Ego’s (Alter’s) classmates similar to her in each characteristic considered.

  • Five minutes distance. Binary: 1 if Ego and Alter live within a five-minute walking distance, as assessed by question YC-9.

  • Number of common friends. Number of friends that Ego and Alter share, as assessed by question YC-1.

Demographic characteristics

  • Same country of origin; Both first/second/third-generation immigrants. The information used to construct these variables was retrieved from previous work by Dollmann and Konstanze (2016).

  • Same gender; Same religion; Difference: age; Difference: years of residence. Variables were constructed from the answers to questions YM-1, YM-2, YM-3, YM-4, and YM-71.

Socioeconomic characteristics

  • Both have a parent with a university degree. Binary: 1 if both Ego and Alter have at least one parent who completed tertiary education, as assessed by questions YM-27 and YM-33.

  • Difference: number of books. Absolute difference in the number of books in Ego’s and Alter’s houses, as assessed by question YM-95 .

  • Difference: number of rooms. Absolute difference in the number of rooms in Ego’s and Alter’s houses, as assessed by question YM-96.

Normative beliefs and subjective integration

  • Both think men should be the breadwinners. Binary: 1 if both Ego and Alter answered ‘mostly the man’ to question YM-63a.

  • Both think women should clean the house. Binary: 1 if both Ego and Alter answered ‘mostly the woman’ to question YM-63b.

  • Both think natives should open to immigrants’ customs. Binary: 1 if both Ego and Alter answered ‘strongly agree’ to question YM-75a.

  • Both think immigrants should keep their customs. Binary: 1 if both Ego and Alter answered ‘strongly agree’ to question YM-75b.

  • Subjective integration: both strong. Binary: 1 if both Ego and Alter answered ‘very strongly’ to question YM-66.

  • Subjective integration: both weak. Binary: 1 if both Ego and Alter answered ‘not very strongly’ or ‘not at all strongly’ to question YM-66.

Normative beliefs and subjective integration (instruments)

  • Men should be the breadwinners (Ego and Ego’s IFs); Men should be the breadwinners (Alter and Alter’s IFs). Binary: 1 if both Ego (Alter) and the majority of Alter’s friends who are not friends of Ego (Ego’s friends who are not friends of Alter) answered ‘mostly the man’ to question YM-63a.

  • Women should clean the house (Ego and Ego’s IFs); Women should clean the house (Alter and Alter’s IFs). Binary: 1 if both Ego (Alter) and the majority of Alter’s friends who are not friends of Ego (Ego’s friends who are not friends of Alter) answered ‘mostly the woman’ to question YM-63b.

  • Natives should open to immigrants’ customs (Ego and Ego’s IFs); Natives should open to immigrants’ customs (Alter and Alter’s IFs). Binary: 1 if both Ego (Alter) and the majority of Alter’s friends who are not friends of Ego (Ego’s friends who are not friends of Alter) answered ‘strongly agree’ to question YM-75a.

  • Immigrants should keep their customs (Ego and Ego’s IFs); Immigrants should keep their customs (Alter and Alter’s IFs). Binary: 1 if both Ego (Alter) and the majority of Alter’s friends who are not friends of Ego (Ego’s friends who are not friends of Alter) answered ‘strongly agree’ to question YM-75b.

  • Subjective integration: strong (Ego and Ego’s IFs); Subjective integration: strong (Alter and Alter’s IFs). Binary: 1 if both Ego (Alter) and the majority of Alter’s friends who are not friends of Ego (Ego’s friends who are not friends of Alter) answered ‘very strongly’ to question YM-66.

  • Subjective integration: weak (Ego and Ego’s IFs); Subjective integration: weak (Alter and Alter’s IFs). Binary: 1 if both Ego (Alter) and the majority of Alter’s friends who are not friends of Ego (Ego’s friends who are not friends of Alter) answered ‘not very strongly’ or ‘not at all strongly’ to question YM-66.

  • Subjective integration: strong (Ego and Alter’s parent); Subjective integration: strong (Alter and Ego’s parent). Binary: 1 if both Ego (Alter) and Alter’s parent (Ego’s parent) answered ‘very strongly’ to questions YM-66 and YP-9.

  • Subjective integration: weak (Ego and Alter’s parent); Subjective integration: weak (Alter and Ego’s parent). Binary: 1 if both Ego (Alter) and Alter’s parent (Ego’s parent) answered ‘not very strongly’ or ‘not at all strongly’ to questions YM-66 and YP-9.

School grades

  • Math grade: both higher than class median. Binary: 1 if both Ego’s and Alter’s grades in mathematics (as assessed by question YM-19a) are above the class median value.

  • Math grade: both lower than class median. Binary: 1 if both Ego’s and Alter’s grades in mathematics (as assessed by question YM-19a) are below the class median value.

  • SCL grade: both higher than class median. Binary: 1 if both Ego’s and Alter’s grades in <survey country language> (as assessed by question YM-19b) are above the class median value.

  • SCL grade: both lower than class median. Binary: 1 if both Ego’s and Alter’s grades in <survey country language> (as assessed by question YM-19b) are below the class median value.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Campigotto, N., Rapallini, C. & Rustichini, A. School friendship networks, homophily and multiculturalism: evidence from European countries. J Popul Econ 35, 1687–1722 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00148-020-00819-w

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00148-020-00819-w

Keywords

JEL Classification

Navigation