Skip to main content
Log in

Peer effects of low-ability students in the classroom: evidence from China’s middle schools

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Population Economics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This paper examines the causal effects of the proportion of low-ability students in the classroom on the academic performance of regular students, exploiting random assignment of students to classes within middle schools in China. We show that the share of students in a class who are low achievers has a significant negative impact on the academic achievement of regular students in the seventh grade. The peer effects are heterogeneous along their achievement distribution, with the strongest adverse impact at the bottom end but no discernable impact at the top end. In contrast, there is no evidence that low-ability students influence any part of the achievement distribution of regular students in the ninth grade. Therefore, peer effects in academic outcomes can vary with the length of regular students’ exposure to the same group of low-ability classmates. We further show that the differences in peer effects of low-ability students in seventh and ninth grades are driven by the adjustments of students’ friendship formation and learning environment when approaching the completion of middle school.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. For example, Burke and Sass (2013) find small to non-existent spillover effects from mean peer ability, while Sojourner (2013) finds substantial positive average effects of peer achievement. Antecol et al. (2016) show that the average classroom peer achievement negatively affects own student academic achievement. Using the same data as in Sojourner (2013) and Bietenbeck (2020) shows that the peer impact of low-ability classmates can be positive in the long run.

  2. For example, if parents of high-ability students send their children to the same class, we cannot conclude that there is a causal linkage between high-ability students and their classmates’ learning outcomes.

  3. Using rich data from middle schools in one county of China’s Jiangsu Province, Ding and Lehrer (2007) utilize extensive controls to capture the selection process and establish the link between peer performance and student achievement. Carman and Zhang (2012) share a similar limitation in the representativeness of data by using student records from only one middle school in a North China province. Students are not randomly assigned into classes in the data used in these two studies. Moreover, neither of the two investigates into the mechanisms through which their estimated peer impacts may operate.

  4. Using data from the Tennessee Student/Teacher Achievement Ratio experiment (Project STAR), Bietenbeck (2020) shows that the short-term peer influence of exposure to repeaters in kindergarten on academic performance is negative. However, the long-term impact is positive, when peers at kindergarten are no longer in the same classroom anymore. The changes in peer effects estimated in Bietenbeck (2020) are different from ours because the classroom composition of repeaters and non-repeaters stays the same over time in middle schools in our sample. Other studies such as Bifulco et al. (2011) and Bifulco et al. (2014) analyze peer effects in high schools and their persistent influence in later years.

  5. As discussed in Manski (1993) and Lin (2010), in the presence of endogenous effects, offering academic support to low achievers will have social multiplier effects. They benefit from the support and will subsequently affect the performance of their peers, which in turn will affect the achievement of the former, and so on. However, if only contextual effects exist, regular students are responding to socioeconomic composition of low-ability students, so offering learning support to repeaters will generate no multiplier influence.

  6. Since 1986, China has implemented the policy of nine-year compulsory education that includes six years in primary school and three years in middle school. Students covered in the CEPS data were all receiving compulsory education at the time of the survey.

  7. Exploiting the random assignment of students in the CEPS data, Hu (2018) and Wang et al. (2018) examine the academic peer effects of migrant students on urban students; Gong et al. (2018) and Gong et al. (2019) analyze the effects of teacher gender and classmates’ gender composition on academic and non-cognitive outcomes of students, respectively.

  8. We begin with 19,487 students from 112 schools in our data, including (i) 3065 students from 16 schools that used non-random assignment; (ii) 2854 students from 15 schools that randomly assigned students in grade seven but rearranged classes for grades eight and nine; (iii) 13,046 students from 78 schools that randomly assigned students in grade seven and did not rearrange classes for grades eight and nine; and (iv) 522 students from 3 schools that did not report how they assigned students to classrooms.

  9. As indicated in CEPS, none of these 78 middle schools requires students to repeat a grade if they fail the annual exam.

  10. Results by subject are reported and discussed in Section 4.2.1.

  11. For seventh graders, the correlation coefficients are 0.66 for Chinese and mathematics, 0.78 for Chinese and English, and 0.74 for mathematics and English. The three correlation coefficients for ninth graders are 0.74, 0.76, and 0.78, respectively.

  12. We report the detailed PCA results in Appendix Table 13. For seventh graders, the eigenvalues for the transformed three components are 2.45, 0.34, and 0.21, respectively. The eigenvalues for ninth graders are 2.52, 0.26, and 0.21, respectively. Appendix Table 14 shows that the correlation coefficients between the first component and the original three test scores are between 0.56 and 0.60 for seventh graders. The correlation coefficients are between 0.57 and 0.59 for ninth graders. Therefore, our principle component is a good summary index of the three test scores for each grade.

  13. From January 2014, the Ministry of Education of China requires students who finish elementary school education to be admitted to a middle school located close to the neighborhood of their residential address (see http://old.moe.gov.cn//publicfiles/business/htmlfiles/moe/s3321/201401/163246.html). Reshuffling of students into classes still happens in the transition to middle school. However, there may be a small chance that some students who were classmates at primary school will still be classmates in middle school because of this policy. In our sample, students were not affected by this policy. Ninth graders and seventh graders were admitted to their middle school in the autumn semester of the 2011 and 2013 academic year, respectively.

  14. We also have checked whether class indicator predicts students’ predetermined repeater status within schools. Following the logic described in Chetty et al. (2011) and Bietenbeck (2020), if assignment to classes is indeed random, then the class to which students are assigned within a school should not be related to students’ repeater status. To test this empirically, we generate an indicator taking a value of 1 if a student is a repeater and 0 otherwise. For each grade in each school, as two classes are typically selected, we randomly select one class with a value of 1 assigned to it and a value of 0 assigned to the other class. Using the pooled sample of seventh and ninth graders, we regress the repeater indicator on the class indicator, controlling for grade-by-school fixed effects. The estimated coefficient on the class indicator represents the difference in the share of repeaters between two classes within the same grade of the same school. If the assignment of students to classes is random within schools, then the coefficient estimated for the class indicator should be small and statistically insignificant. The regression results show that this estimated coefficient is –0.001 with a standard error of 0.006 (p value= 0.893), suggesting that the class to which students are assigned cannot predict student predetermined repeater status within schools. We have also performed the same regressions for seventh graders and ninth graders separately. The coefficient estimates of the class indicator are both very small and statistically insignificant (0.010 and –0.014, with p values of 0.247 and 0.161, respectively). These results suggest that repeater status is indeed balanced across classes within the same grade of the same school.

  15. The education system in China ranks teachers in primary and secondary schools by levels from intern teachers (the lowest) to third class, second-class, first-class, and superior-class teachers (the highest). Teachers’ salaries are largely determined by the ranking and years of working experience in the education sector.

  16. We perform two additional tests of random assignment as robustness checks in Sections 4.4.1 and 4.4.2.

  17. In Section 4.1, we attempt to empirically separate endogenous peer effects from contextual peer effects.

  18. As discussed in Footnote 8, 3065 students are from the 16 schools that use non-random assignment. After dropping 99 observations with missing information on variables used for panel B of Table 4, we are left with 2966 students, consisting of 2552 regular students and 414 low-ability students.

  19. Lee (2007) considers the identification and estimation of structural interaction effects in a social interaction model, which shows that endogenous effects and contextual effects can be separately identified in the presence of sufficient variations in group sizes. Nakajima (2007) presents a peer effect model allowing different responses across different types of peers.

  20. We thank one anonymous referee for this suggestion.

  21. The sample sizes in Table 5 become smaller because some classes do not have any students who were a grade-repeater in primary school.

  22. Table 5 shows some evidence of contextual peer effects of low-achieving students. The proportion of boys among low achievers has a significant negative impact on the academic performance of regular students in both grade seven and grade nine. The average paternal years of schooling of low achievers generates no peer influence, in either grade seven or grade nine. In contrast, the average maternal years of schooling of low-ability students exerts a significant positive effect in the ninth grade.

  23. Hsieh and van Kippersluis (2018) have developed an extended spatial autoregressive (SAR) model to overcome the problem of disentangling endogenous peer effects from unobserved contextual effects. However, their approach requires detailed friendship nominations, which seems to be applicable only to the Add Health data.

  24. Consequently, the negative mean academic impact of repeaters found in Xu et al. (2020) is mainly driven by the large adverse influence of repeaters on low-performing regular students in grade seven.

  25. Similar evidence is found by Booij et al. (2017) from a randomized experiment at the University of Amsterdam and by Feld and Zolitz (2017) from the causal analysis using data from Maastricht University.

  26. We also have included additionally the five characteristics of teachers for each subject in the regressions focusing on subject-level achievement. Results are similar to the estimates shown in Table 6.

References

  • Altonji JG, Elder TE, Taber CR (2005) Selection on observed and unobserved variables: assessing the effectiveness of catholic schools. J Polit Econ 113:151–184

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Angrist J (2014) The perils of peer effects. Labour Econ 30:98–108

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Antecol H, Eren O, Ozbeklik S (2016) Peer effects in disadvantaged primary schools: evidence from a randomized experiment. J Hum Resour 51:95–132

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bietenbeck J (2020) The long-term impacts of low-achieving childhood peers: evidence from Project STAR. J Eur Econ Assoc 18:392–426

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bifulco R, Fletcher JM, Oh SJ, Ross SL (2014) Do high school peers have persistent effects on college attainment and other life outcomes? Labour Econ 29:83–90

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bifulco R, Fletcher JM, Ross SL (2011) The effect of classmate characteristics on post-secondary outcomes: evidence from the Add Health. American Economic Journal: Economic Policy 3:25–53

    Google Scholar 

  • Booij A, Leuven E, Oosterbeek H (2017) Ability peer effects in university: evidence from a randomized experiment. Rev Econ Stud 84:547–578

    Google Scholar 

  • Burke MA, Sass TR (2013) Classroom peer effects and student achievement. J Labor Econ 31:51–82

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carman KG, Zhang L (2012) Classroom peer effects and academic achievement: evidence from a Chinese middle school. China Econ Rev 23:223–237

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carrell S, Fullerton R, West J (2009) Does your cohort matter? Measuring peer effects in college achievement. J Labor Econ 27:439–464

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carrell SE, Sacerdote BI, West JE (2013) From natural variation to optimal policy? The importance of endogenous peer group formation. Econometrica 81:855–882

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chetty R, Friedman JN, Hilger N, Saez E, Schanzenbach DW, Yagan D (2011) How does your kindergarten classroom affect your earnings? Evidence from Project STAR. Q J Econ 126:1593–1660

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ding W, Lehrer SF (2007) Do peers affect student achievement in China’s secondary schools? Rev Econ Stat 89:300–312

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duflo E, Dupas P, Kremer M (2011) Peer effects, teacher incentives, and the impact of tracking: evidence from a randomized evaluation in Kenya. Am Econ Rev 101:1739–1774

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eisenberg D, Golberstein E, Whitlock JL (2014) Peer effects on risky behaviors: new evidence from college roommate assignments. J Health Econ 33:126–138

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Epple D, Romano RE (2011) Peer effects in education: a survey of the theory and evidence. In: Benhabib J, Bisin A, Jackson MO (eds) Handbook of Social Economics, vol 1, North Holland, pp 1053–1163

  • Feld J, Zolitz U (2017) Understanding peer effects: on the nature, estimation, and channels of peer effects. J Labor Econ 35:387–428

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feng H, Li J (2016) Head teachers, peer effects, and student achievement. China Econ Rev 41:268–283

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Firpo S, Fortin MN, Lemieux T (2009) Unconditional quantile regressions. Econometrica 77:953–973

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gaviria A, Raphael S (2001) School-based peer effects and juvenile behavior. Rev Econ Stat 83:257–268

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gong J, Lu Y, Song H (2018) The effect of teacher gender on students’ academic and noncognitive outcomes. J Labor Econ 36:743–778

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gong J, Lu Y, Song H (2019) Gender peer effects on students’ academic and noncognitive outcomes: evidence and mechanisms. Journal of Human Resources, forthcoming

  • Guryan J, Kroft K, Notowidigdo MJ (2009) Peer effects in the workplace: evidence from random groupings in professional golf tournaments. Am Econ J Appl Econ 1:34–68

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hesketh T, Zhen Y, Lu L, Dong ZX, Jun YX, Xing ZW (2010) Stress and psychosomatic symptoms in Chinese school children: cross-sectional survey. Arch Dis Child 95:136–140

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hsieh C-S, van Kippersluis H (2018) Smoking initiation: peers and personality. Quant Econ 9:825–863

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hu F (2018) Migrant peers in the classroom: is the academic performance of local students negatively affected? J Comp Econ 46:582–597

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lavy V, Paserman MD, Schlosser A (2012) Inside the black box of ability peer effects: evidence from variation in the proportion of low achievers in the classroom. Econ J 122:208–237

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lazear EP (2001) Educational production. Q J Econ 116:777–803

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee L. -f. (2007) Identification and estimation of econometric models with group interactions, contextual factors and fixed effects. J Econ 140:333–374

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lin X (2010) Identifying peer effects in student academic achievement by spatial autoregressive models with group unobservables. J Labor Econ 28:825–860

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Manski CF (1993) Identification of endogenous social effects: the reflection problem. Rev Econ Stud 60:531–540

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nakajima R (2007) Measuring peer effects on youth smoking behaviour. Rev Econ Stud 74:897–935

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oster E (2019) Unobservable selection and coefficient stability: theory and evidence. J Bus Econ Stat 37:187–204

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sacerdote B. (2011) Peer effects in education: how might they work, how big are they, and how much do we know thus far?. In: Hanushek E.A., Machin S., Woessman L. (eds) Handbook of the Economics of Education, vol 3, North Holland, pp 249–277

  • Sojourner A (2013) Identification of peer effects with missing peer data: evidence from Project STAR. Economic Journal 123:574–605

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sun J, Dunne MP, Hou X, Xu A (2013) Educational stress among Chinese adolescents: individual, family, school and peer influences. Educ Rev 65:284–302

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang H, Cheng Z, Smyth R (2018) Do migrant students affect local students’ academic achievements in urban China? Econ Educ Rev 63:64–77

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang H, Zhu R (2019) China’s only children and their spillover effects on academic performance in the classroom. The B E. Journal of Economic Analysis & Policy, 20190058

  • Xu D, Zhang Q, Zhou X (2020) The impact of low-ability peers on cognitive and non-cognitive outcomes: random assignment evidence on the effects and operating channels. Journal of Human Resources, forthcoming

Download references

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to the Editors, Shuaizhang Feng and Madeline Zavodny, and two anonymous referees for helpful comments. Thanks also go to the Chinese National Survey Data Archive (http://cnsda.ruc.edu.cn) for providing access to the China Education Panel Survey data.

Funding

Bin Huang acknowledges financial support from China’s National Social Science Foundation in Education (Project Number: BFA190054). Rong Zhu acknowledges financial support from the Australian Research Council Linkage Project (Project Number: LP170100718).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Rong Zhu.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Responsible editor: Shuaizhang Feng

Publisher’s note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendix

Appendix

Table 12 Summary statistics of students used in the estimation sample and students excluded from the estimation sample
Table 13 Principle component analysis of test scores
Table 14 Contribution of variables to components in PCA
Table 15 Summary statistics of mechanism variables
Table 16 Summary statistics of subject teachers

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Huang, B., Zhu, R. Peer effects of low-ability students in the classroom: evidence from China’s middle schools. J Popul Econ 33, 1343–1380 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00148-020-00780-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00148-020-00780-8

Keywords

JEL Classification

Navigation