Skip to main content
Log in

Does Japan really have robot mania? Comparing attitudes by implicit and explicit measures

  • Original article
  • Published:
AI & SOCIETY Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Japan has more robots than any other country with robots contributing to many areas of society, including manufacturing, healthcare, and entertainment. However, few studies have examined Japanese attitudes toward robots, and none has used implicit measures. This study compares attitudes among the faculty of a US and a Japanese university. Although the Japanese faculty reported many more experiences with robots, implicit measures indicated both faculties had more pleasant associations with humans. In addition, although the US faculty reported people were more threatening than robots, implicit measures indicated both faculties associated weapons more strongly with robots than with humans. Despite the media’s hype about Japan’s robot ‘craze,’ response similarities suggest factors other than attitude better explain robot adoption. These include differences in history and religion, personal and human identity, economic structure, professional specialization, and government policy. Japanese robotics offers a unique reference from which other nations may learn.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. In other words, robots that could pass the Total Turing Test (Harnad 1989).

  2. Ironically, Western philosophy has progressively backed away from substance dualism (MacDorman 2004).

  3. Nevertheless, Buddhism does make a distinction between sentient and nonsentient beings, and prohibits the slaughter of sentient beings.

  4. The Economist, December 1, 2001, p. 96.

  5. Sweden ranks first of 128 countries with the narrowest gender gap. The US is listed at 31 compared to 91 for Japan (Hausmann et al. 2007).

  6. In 2005, Japan’s population was 127,756,000, and there were 1,392 murders, 5,988 robberies, 2,076 rapes, and 25,815 acts of violence (Japan Statistical Yearbook 2008, Chap. 25, Justice and police, p. 773, Statistics Bureau, Ministry of Internal Affairs and Communications). In 2005, the USA’s population was 296,507,061 and there were 16,740 murders, 417,438 robberies, 94,347 rapes, and 1,390,745 acts of violence (2006 Crime in the United States. Federal Bureau of Investigation, Department of Justice).

  7. Personal communication.

  8. IFI Patent Intelligence Issues Annual Rankings of Top U.S. Patent Assignees, PR Newswire, January 11, 2008.

References

  • Ashburn-Nardo L, Knowles ML, Monteith MJ (2003) Black Americans implicit racial associations and their implications for intergroup judgment. Soc Cogn 21:61–87

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Banaji MR (2001) Implicit attitudes can be measured. Roediger HL III, Nairne JS, Neath I, Surprenant A (eds) The nature of remembering: essays in honor of Robert G. Crowder. American Psychological Association Press, Washington, DC, pp 117–150

    Google Scholar 

  • Bargh JA, Chaiken S, Govender R, Pratto F (1992) The generality of the automatic attitude activation effect. J Pers Soc Psychol 62:893–912

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barry B (2005) Better than people: why the Japanese want their robots to act more like humans. The Economist, December 24

  • Bartneck C, Nomura T, Kanda T, Suzuki T, Kato K (2005) A cross-cultural study on attitude towards robots. In: Proceedings of the 11th international conference on human–computer interaction

  • Bartneck C, Suzuki T, Kanda T, Nomura T (2007) The influence of people’s culture and prior experiences with Aibo on their attitude towards robots. AI Soc 21:217–230

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Capek K (1921/2004) Rossum’s universal robots (R.U.R.). Penguin, London

  • Castells M (2000) The rise of the network society, 2nd edn. Blackwell, Malden

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooley M (2007) From judgment to calculation. AI Soc 21(4):395–409

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cowley SJ, MacDorman KF (2006) What baboons, babies, and Tetris players tell us about interaction: a biosocial view of norm-based social learning. Connect Sci 18(4):363–378

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dasgupta N, McGhee D, Greenwald A, Banaji M (2000) Automatic preference for white Americans: eliminating the familiarity explanation. J Exp Soc Psychol 36:316–328

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davis W (1996) The moral and political naturalism of Baron Kato Hiroyuki. Institute for East Asian Studies, Berkeley

    Google Scholar 

  • De Houwer J, Moors A (2007) How to define and examine the implicitness of implicit measures. In: Wittenbrink B, Schwarz N (eds) Implicit measures of attitudes: procedures and controversies. Guilford Press, New York, pp 179–194

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett DC (1991) Consciousness explained. Little, Brown and Co., Boston

    Google Scholar 

  • Douglas M (1966) Purity and danger: an analysis of concepts of pollution and taboo. Routledge, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Engardio P (2005) Engineering: is the U.S. really falling? Businessweek, December 27

  • Fazio RH, Olson MA (2003) Implicit measures in social cognition research: their meaning and use. Annu Rev Psychol 54:297–327

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feil-Seifer D, Skinner K, Mataric MJ (2007) Benchmarks for evaluating socially assistive robotics. Interact Stud 8(3):423–439

    Google Scholar 

  • Freud S (1919/2003) The uncanny [das Unheimliche] (D. McClintock, Trans.). Penguin, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald AG, Banaji MR (1995) Implicit social cognition: attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychol Rev 102(1):4–27

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald AG, McGhee DE, Schwartz LK (1998) Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test. J Pers Soc Psychol 74(6):1464–1480

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald AG, Nosek BA, Banaji MR (2003) Understanding and using the implicit association test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. J Pers Soc Psychol 85:197–216

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harnad S (1989) Minds, machines, and Turing. J Exp Theor Artif Intell 1(1):5–25

    Article  MathSciNet  Google Scholar 

  • Hausmann R, Tyson LD, Zahidi S (2007) The global gender gap report. World Economic Forum, Geneva

    Google Scholar 

  • Hornyak TN (2006) Loving the machine: the art and science of Japanese robotics. Kodansha, Tokyo

    Google Scholar 

  • Houben K, Wiers RW (2007) Are drinkers implicitly positive about drinking alcohol? Personalizing the alcohol-IAT to reduce negative extrapersonal contamination. Alcohol Alcohol 42(4):301–307

    Google Scholar 

  • Ji L, Peng K, Nisbett RE (2000) Culture, control, and perception of relationships in the environment. J Pers Soc Psychol 5:943–955

    Google Scholar 

  • Kagan RA (1994) Do lawyers cause adversarial legalism? A preliminary inquiry. Law Soc Inq 19(1):1–62

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahn PH, Ishiguro H, Friedman B, Kanda T, Freier NG, Severson RL (2007) What is a human? Toward psychological benchmarks in the field of human-robot interaction. Interact Stud 8(3):363–390

    Google Scholar 

  • Karpinski A, Hilton JL (2001) Attitudes and the implicit association test. J Pers Soc Psychol 1(5):774–788

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kincaid DL (1987) Communication East and West: points of departure. In: Kincaid DL (ed) Communication theory: Eastern and Western perspectives. Academic Press, San Diego, pp 331–340

    Google Scholar 

  • Komiya N (1999) A cultural study of the low crime rate in Japan. Br J Criminol 39(3):369–390

    Article  MathSciNet  Google Scholar 

  • Lynn LH (2002) Engineers and engineering in the U.S. and Japan: a critical review of the literature and suggestions for a new research agenda. IEEE Trans Eng Manage 41(1):95–106

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacDorman KF (2004) Extending the medium hypothesis: the Dennett–Mangan controversy and beyond. J Mind Behav 25(3):237–257

    Google Scholar 

  • MacDorman KF, Cowley SJ (2006) Long-term relationships as a benchmark for robot personhood. In: Proceedings of the 15th IEEE international symposium on robot and human interactive communication. Hatfield, UK, September 6–9

  • MacDorman KF, Ishiguro H (2006) The uncanny advantage of using androids in social and cognitive science research. Interact Stud 7(3):297–337

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mazlish B (1993) The fourth discontinuity: the co-evolution of humans and machines. Yale University Press, New Haven

    Google Scholar 

  • Mori M (1970) Bukimi no tani (in Japanese). [The uncanny valley] (MacDorman KF, Minato T). Energy 7(4):33–35

    Google Scholar 

  • Mori M (1982) The Buddha in the robot: a robot engineer’s thoughts on science and religion. Tuttle, Tokyo

    Google Scholar 

  • National Science Board (2006) Science and engineering indicators 2006, vol 1. National Science Foundation, Arlington

  • Nomura T, Kanda T, Suzuki T, Kato K (2005) People’s assumptions of robots: investigation of their relationships with attitudes and emotions toward robots. In: Proceedings of the 14th IEEE international workshop on robot and human interactive communication, pp 125–130

  • Nomura T, Kanda T, Suzuki T (2006) Experimental investigation into influence of negative attitudes toward robots on human–robot interaction. AI Soc 20(2):138–150

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nomura T, Tasaki T, Kanda T, Shiomi M, Ishiguro H, Hagita N (2007) Questionnaire-based social research on opinions of Japanese visitors for communication robots at an exhibition. AI Soc 21(1):167–183

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nosek BA, Banaji MR (2001) The go/no-go association task. Soc Cogn 19(6):161–176

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olson MA, Fazio RH (2004) Reducing the influence of extra-personal associations on the implicit association test: personalizing the IAT. J Pers Soc Psychol 86(5):653–667

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Penke L, Eichstaedt J, Asendorpf JB (2006) Single attribute implicit association tests (SA-IAT) for the assessment of unipolar constructs: the case of sociosexuality. Exp Psychol 53(4):283–291

    Google Scholar 

  • Plato (360 BCE/1888) The republic, 3rd edn. In: Jowett B (Trans.). Clarendon Press, Oxford

  • Putnam H (1967) The mental life of some machines. In: Castaneda H (ed) Intentionality, minds and perception. Wayne State University Press, Detroit, pp 439–460

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramey CH (2005) The uncanny valley of similarities concerning abortion, baldness, heaps of sand, and humanlike robots. In: Proceedings of the views of the uncanny valley workshop, IEEE-RAS international conference on humanoid robots. Tsukuba, Japan

  • Reubenfien E (1989) U.S. police walk different beat in Japan. The Asian Wall Street Journal, January 13–14

  • Ross D, Dumouchel P (2004) Emotions as strategic signals. Rational Soc 16(3):251–286

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sander RH, Williams ED (1989) Why are there so many lawyers? Perspectives on a turbulent market. Law Soc Inq 14(3):431–479

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scanlon C (2003) Japan considers jury system. BBC News, March 12

  • Schodt FL (1988) Inside the robot kingdom: Japan, mechatronics, and the coming robotopia. Kodansha, Tokyo

    Google Scholar 

  • Shibata T, Wada K, Tanie K (2004) Tabulation and analysis of questionnaire results of subjective evaluation of seal robot in Japan, UK, Sweden and Italy. In: Proceedings of the IEEE international conference on robotics and automation. New Orleans

  • Singer CJ, Williams H, Adelmann B (1954) A history of technology. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Solomon S, Greenberg J, Pyszczynski T (1998) Tales from the crypt: the role of death in life. Zygon J Relig Sci 33:9–43

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turkle S (2007) Authenticity in the age of digital companions. Interact Stud 8(3):501–517

    Google Scholar 

  • Weil MM, Rosen LD (1995) A study of technological sophistication and technophobia in university students from 23 countries. Comput Hum Behav 11(1):95–133

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woelfel J (1987) Development of the Western model: toward a reconciliation of Eastern and Western perspectives. In: Kincaid DL (ed) Communication theory: Eastern and Western perspectives. Academic Press, San Diego, pp 299–314

    Google Scholar 

  • Xi Z (1967) In: Chu H, Lui T-C, Chan W-T (eds). Relections on things at hand: the neo-Confucian anthology. Columbia University Press, New York

  • Yamamoto S (1983) Naze nihonjin niha robotto arerugi ga nai no ka? (in Japanese) [Why aren’t Japanese allergic to robots?]. Gendai No Espuri 187:136–143

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We are grateful for the helpful advice and kind assistance of Leslie Ashburn-Nardo, Mahzarin Banaji, Stephen J. Cowley, Anthony Faiola, Sara A. Hook, Hiroshi Ishiguro, Josette Jones, Peter H. Kahn, Sara Kiesler, Tatsuya Nomura, Satoshi V. Suzuki, Hiroaki Yamano, Hiroki Yokota, and three anonymous reviewers.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Karl F. MacDorman.

Appendix: Questionnaire

Appendix: Questionnaire

Robot-related experiences

  1. 1.

    How many times in the past one (1) year have you read robot-related stories, comics, news articles, product descriptions, conference papers, journal papers, blogs, or other material? (6-point scale) 0, 1, 2, 3, 4, 5 or more

  2. 2.

    How many times in the past one (1) year have you watched robot-related programs on film, television, DVD, the Internet, or other media?

  3. 3.

    How many times in the past ten (10) years have you had physical contact with a robot?

  4. 4.

    How many times in the past ten (10) years have you attended robot-related lectures, exhibitions, trade shows, competitions, or other events?

  5. 5.

    How many times in your life have you built or programmed a robot?

Attitudes toward robots

  1. 1.

    Select the statement that best describes your opinion. (7-point scale)

    • I strongly prefer robots to people. (+3)

    • I like robots and people equally. (0)

    • I strongly prefer people to robots. (−3)

  2. 2.

    Rate how warm or cold you feel toward robots. (11-point scale)

    • Very cold (−5)

    • Neutral (0)

    • Very warm (+ 5)

  3. 3.

    Rate how warm or cold you feel toward people. (11-point scale)

  4. 4.

    Select the statement that best describes your opinion. (7-point scale)

    • Robots are much more threatening than people. (+ 3)

    • Robots and people are equally threatening. (0)

    • People are much more threatening than robots. (−3)

  5. 5.

    Rate how safe or threatening you feel robots are. (11-point scale)

  6. 6.

    Rate how safe or threatening you feel people are. (11-point scale)

  7. 7.

    How familiar are you with robots? (6-point scale)

    • Not at all familiar (0)

    • Completely familiar (+5)

  8. 8.

    How interested are you in robots? (6-point scale)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

MacDorman, K.F., Vasudevan, S.K. & Ho, CC. Does Japan really have robot mania? Comparing attitudes by implicit and explicit measures. AI & Soc 23, 485–510 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00146-008-0181-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00146-008-0181-2

Keywords

Navigation