Skip to main content
Log in

The loomingness of danger: Does it discriminate focal phobia and general anxiety from depression?

  • Published:
Cognitive Therapy and Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In three studies, we examine the apparent loomingness, or rapid forward motion, that anxious individuals may perceive in danger. Results of the current studies show that individuals who are phobic of specific focal stimuli—spiders—may perceive loomingness in the dangers they typically fear but not in other dangers. Conversely, individuals who are generally anxious in mood perceive loomingness in dangers which are related to general anxiety but not in those related to spiders. Although the apparent loomingness of dangers is positively related to anxiety and specific phobic fears, it appears to be unrelated or negatively related to depression. Results of path analyses (Studies 1 and 2) and a study with an experimental methodology (Study 3) appear to be consistent with a proposed mediated sequence in which the perception of loomingness in danger helps to activate threat cognitions that lead to anxiety and fear.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ball, W., & Tronick, E. (1971). Infant responses to impending collision: Optical and real.Science, 171 818–820.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T., & Emery, G. (1985).Anxiety disorders and phobias: A cognitive perspective. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. G., & Emery, G. (1979).Cognitive therapy of depression. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Garbin, M. G. (1988). Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory: 25 years of evaluation.Clinical Psychology Review, 8 77–100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1990). Origins and functions of positive and negative affect: A control-process view.Psychological Review, 97 19–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Costello, C. G., & Comrey, A. L. (1967). Scales for measuring depression and anxiety,Journal of Psychology, 66 303–313.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dobson, K. S. (1985). The relationship between anxiety and depression.Clinical Psychology Review, 5 307–324.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (1984).Social cognition. London: Addison-Wesley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foa, E. B., & Kozak, M. J. (1986). Emotional processing of fear: Exposure to corrective information.Psychological Bulletin, 99 20–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freyd, J. J., & Rinke, R. A. (1984). Representational momentum.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 10 126–132.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geer, J. H. (1965). The development of a scale to measure fear.Behavior Research and Therapy, 3 45–53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gotlib, I. H. (1984). Depression and general psychopathology in university students.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 90 521–530.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hekmat, H. (1987). Origins and development of human fear reactions.Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 1 197–218.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hsee, C. K., & Abelson, R. P. (1991). The velocity relation: Satisfaction as a function of the first derivative of outcome over time.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60 341–347.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ingram, R. E. (1989). Affective confounds in social-cognitive research.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57 715–722.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, L. R., & Brett, J. M. (1984). Mediators, moderators, and tests for mediation.Journal of Applied Psychology, 69 307–321.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karoly, P., & Ruehlman, L. (1983). Affective meaning and depression: A semantic differential analysis.Cognitive Therapy and Research, 7 41–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kendall, P. C., & Watson, D. (Eds.). (1989).Anxiety and depression: Distinctive and overlapping features. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McNally, R. J., & Steketee, G. S. (1985). The etiology and maintenance of severe animal phobias.Behavior Research and Therapy, 23 431–435.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mineka, S. (1987). A primate model of phobic fears. In H. Eysenck & I. Martin (Eds.).Theoretical foundations of behavior therapy (pp. 81–111). New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riskind, J. H. (1989). The “Harm-Looming” Questionnaire: A measure of cognitive features of anxiety that discriminates anxiety from depression. Unpublished manuscript, George Mason university.

  • Riskind, J. H. (1991). Perceptions of loomingness discriminate anxiety from depression: A “harm-looming” model of cognitive scripts. Unpublished manuscript.

  • Riskind, J. H., Castellon, C. S., and Beck, A. T. (1989). Spontaneous causal explanations in unipolar depression and generalized anxiety: Content analyses of Dysfunctional-Thought Diaries.Cognitive Therapy and Research, 13 97–108.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riskind, J. H., & Maddux, J. (in press). Loomingness, helplessness, and fearfulness: An integration of harm-looming and self-efficacy models of fear and anxiety.Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology.

  • Riskind, J. H., & Maddux, J. (1992). The loomingness of danger and the fear of AIDS. Manuscript submitted for publication.

  • Riskind, J. H., & Wahl, O. (in press). Moving makes it worse: The role of rapid movement in fears of psychiatric patients.Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology.

  • Ruehlman, L. S. (1985). Depression and affective meaning for current concerns.Cognitive Therapy and Research, 9 533–560.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schank, R., & Abelson, R. P. (1977).Scripts, plans, goals, and understanding: An inquiry into human knowledge structures. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seligman, M. E. P. (1971). Phobias and preparedness.Behavior Therapy, 2 307–320.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schiff, W., Caviness, A., & Gibson, J. J. (1962). Persistent fear responses in rhesus monkeys in response to the optical stimulus of “looming.”Science 136, 982–983.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., & Lushene, R. E. (1970).Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (self-evaluation questionnaire). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tanaka-Matsumi, J., & Kameoka, V. A. (1986). Reliabilities and concurrent validities of popular self-report measures of depression, anxiety, and social desirability.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54 328–333.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tellegan, A. (1985). Structures of mood and personality and their relevance to assessing anxiety, with an emphasis on self report. In A. H. Tuma & J. D. Maser (Eds.),Anxiety and the anxiety disorders (pp. 681–706). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watts, F. N., & Sharrock, R. (1984). Questionnaire dimensions of spider phobia.Behavior Research and Therapy, 22 575–580.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weerts, T. C., & Lang, P. J. (1978). Psychophysiology of fear imagery: Differences between focal phobia and social performance anxiety.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 46 1157–1159.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

We extend our thanks to William S. Rholes, James E. Maddux, Otto Wahl, and several anonymous reviewers for helpful comments on an earlier manuscript.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Riskind, J.H., Kelley, K., Harman, W. et al. The loomingness of danger: Does it discriminate focal phobia and general anxiety from depression?. Cogn Ther Res 16, 603–622 (1992). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01175402

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01175402

Key words

Navigation