Skip to main content
Log in

The 3P Model: A General Theory of Subjective Well-Being

  • Research Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Happiness Studies Aims and scope Submit manuscript

“Everyone wants continuous and genuine happiness.”

Baruch Spinoza (1677/1985)

Abstract

Empirical research focusing on the field of subjective well-being has resulted in a range of theories, components, and measures, yet only a modicum of work leans towards the establishment of a general theory of subjective well-being. I propose that a temporal model of subjective well-being, called the 3P Model, is a parsimonious, unifying theory, which accounts for, as well as unites, disparate theories and measurements. The 3P Model categorizes the components of subjective well-being under the temporal states of the Present, the Past, and the Prospect (Future). The model indicates how each state is important to a global evaluation of subjective well-being and how each state is distinct yet connected to the other states. Additionally, the model explains how measures of subjective well-being are affected by cognitive biases (e.g., peak-end rule, impact bias, retrospective bias), which factor into evaluations of the temporal states, and meta-biases (e.g., temporal perspectives), which factor into global evaluations of life satisfaction. Finally, future research is recommended to further support the model as well as create interventions that can be chosen based on an individual’s temporal preference or that can be designed to counteract certain biases.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abrahamson, L. Y., Seligman, M. E. P., & Teasdale, J. D. (1978). Learned helplessness in humans: Critique and reformulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87, 49–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Addis, D. R., Wong, A. T., & Schacter, D. L. (2007). Remembering the past and imagining the future: Common and distinct neural substrates during event construction and elaboration. Neuropsychologia, 45, 1363–1377.

    Google Scholar 

  • Andrews, F. M., & Withey, S. B. (1976). Social indicators of well-being. New York, NY: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Argyle, M. (1987). The psychology of happiness. London, England: Methuen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Austin, J. T., & Vancouver, J. B. (1996). Goal constructs in psychology: Structure, process, and content. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 338–375.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (2000). Self-efficacy: The foundation of agency. In W. J. Perrig & A. Grob (Eds.), Control of human behavior, mental processes, and consciousness (pp. 17–34). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ben-Shahar, T. (2007). Happier. New York, NY: McGraw Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bentham, J. (1948). An introduction to the principles of morals and legislation. New York, NY: Hafner. (Original work published in 1789).

    Google Scholar 

  • Berridge, K. C. (1999). Pleasure, pain, desire, and dread: Hidden core processes of emotion. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, et al. (Eds.), Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 525–557). New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bohart, A. C. (1993). Emphasizing the future in empathy responses. Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 33, 12–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boniwell, I., Osin, E., Linley, P. A., & Ivanchenko, G. (2010). A question of balance: Time perspective and well-being in British and Russian samples. Journal of Positive Psychology, 5(1), 24–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boniwell, I., & Zimbardo, P. G. (2003). Time to find the right balance. The Psychologist, 16, 129–131.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36, 129–148.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, J. N., & Zimbardo, P. G. (1997). Constructing time after death: The transcendental-future time perspective. Time and Society, 6, 5–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyer, P. (2008). Evolutionary economics of mental time travel? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(6), 219–224.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyle, P. A., Barnes, L. L., Buchman, A. S., & Bennett, D. A. (2009). Purpose in life is associated with mortality among community-dwelling older persons. Psychosomatic Medicine, 71, 574–579.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradburn, N. M., & Caplovitz, D. (1965). Reports of happiness. Chicago, IL: Aldine.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brickman, P., & Campbell, D. T. (1971). Hedonic relativism and planning the good society. In M. H. Appley (Ed.), Adaptation level theory: A symposium (pp. 287–302). New York, NY: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brickman, P., Coates, D., & Janoff-Bulman, R. (1978). Lottery winners and accident victims: Is happiness relative? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 917–927.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brief, A. P., Butcher, A. H., George, J. M., & Link, K. E. (1993). Integrating bottom-up and top-down theories of subjective well-being: The case of health. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 646–653.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brunstein, J. C. (1993). Personal goals and subjective well-being: A longitudinal study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 1061–1070.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bryant, F. B. (1989). A four-factor model of perceived control: Avoiding, coping, obtaining and savouring. Journal of Personality, 57, 773–797.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bryant, F. B. (2003). Savoring beliefs inventory (SBI): A scale for measuring beliefs about savoring. Journal of Mental Health, 12, 175–196.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bryant, F. B., Smart, C. M., & King, S. P. (2005). Using the past to enhance the present: Boosting happiness through positive reminiscence. Journal of Happiness Studies, 6, 227–260.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., & Schacter, D. L. (2008). The brain’s default network: Anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124(1), 1–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buckner, R. L., & Carroll, D. C. (2007). Self-projection and the brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 49–57.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, A., Converse, P. E., & Rodgers, W. (1976). The quality of American life. New York, NY: Russell Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S., Lawrence, J. W., & Scheier, M. F. (1996). A control-process perspective on the origins of affect. In L. L. Martin & A. Tesser (Eds.), Striving and feeling: Interactions among goals, affect, and regulation (pp. 11–52). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohn, B. (1999). The lay theory of happiness: Illusions and biases in judging others. Undergraduate dissertation, Princeton University.

  • Collins, A. M., & Quillian, M. R. (1969). Retrieval time from semantic memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8, 240–248.

    Google Scholar 

  • Costa, P., & McCrae, R. (1980). Influence of extraversion and neuroticism on subjective well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 668–678.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cottle, T. J., & Klineberg, S. L. (1974). The present of things future: Explorations of time in human experience. New York, NY: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Craik, F. I. M., & Lockhart, R. S. (1972). Levels of processing: A framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 671–684.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crawford-Solberg, E., Diener, E., Wirtz, D., Lucas, R. E., & Oishi, S. (2002). Wanting, having, and satisfaction: Examining the role of desire discrepancies in satisfaction with income. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 725–734.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. New York, NY: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Hunter, J. (2003). Happiness in everyday life: The uses of Experience Sampling. Journal of Happiness Studies, 4, 185–189.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cummins, R. A. (2010). Subjective wellbeing, homeostatically protected mood and depression: A synthesis. Journal of Happiness Studies, 11, 1–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. R. (2000). A neurobiology for consciousness. In T. Metzinger (Ed.), Neurol correlates of consciousness: Empirical and conceptual questions (pp. 111–120). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, R. J. (1992). Anterior cerebral asymmetry and the nature of emotion. Brain and Cognition, 20, 125–151.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, R. J. (1994). Asymmetric brain function, affective style, and psychopathology: The role of early experience and plasticity. Development and Psychopathology, 6, 741–758.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Volder, M., & Lens, W. (1982). Academic achievement and future time perspective as a cognitive-motivational concept. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 566–571.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E. (1984). Subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 542–575.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E. (2006). Guidelines for national indicators of subjective well-being and ill-being. Journal of Happiness Studies, 7, 397–404.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., & Emmons, R. A. (1984). The independence of positive and negative affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 1105–1117.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 71–75.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., & Fujita, F. (1995). Resources, personal strivings, and subjective well-being: A nomothetic and idiographic approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 926–935.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., Sandvik, E., Seidlitz, L., & Diener, M. (1993). The relationship between income and subjective well-being: Relative or absolute. Social Indicators Research, 28, 195–223.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas, R. E., & Smith, H. L. (1999). Subjective well-being: Three decades of progress. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 276–302.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., Wirtz, D., & Oishi, S. (2001). End effects of rated life quality: The James Dean Effect. Psychological Science, 12, 124–128.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., Wolsic, B., & Fujita, F. (1995). Physical attractiveness and subjective well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 120–129.

    Google Scholar 

  • Drake, L., Duncan, E., Sutherland, F., Abernethy, C., & Henry, C. (2008). Time perspective and correlates of well-being. Time and Society, 17(1), 47–61.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2002). Motivational beliefs, values, and goals. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 109–132.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellmore, T. M., Stouffer, K., & Nadel, L. (2008). Divergence of explicit and implicit processing speed during associative memory retrieval. Brain Research, 1229, 155–166.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elster, J., & Loewenstein, G. (1992). Utility from memory and anticipation. In G. Loewenstein & J. Elster (Eds.), Choice over time (pp. 213–234). New York, NY: Russell Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Emmons, R. A. (1986). Personal strivings: An approach to personality and subjective well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1058–1068.

    Google Scholar 

  • Erikson, E. (1963). Childhood and society. New York, NY: W. W. Norton Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fallot, R. D. (1980). The impact on mood on verbal reminiscing in later adulthood. International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 10, 385–400.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feist, G. J. (1995). Psychology of science and history of psychology: Putting behavioral generalizations to the test. Psychological Inquiry, 6(2), 119–123.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feist, G. J., Bodner, T. E., Jacobs, J. F., Miles, M., & Tan, V. (1995). Integrating top-down and bottom-up structural models of subjective well-being: A longitudinal investigation. The Journal of Personality, 68(1), 138–150.

    Google Scholar 

  • Festinger, L., & Carlsmith, J. M. (1959). Cognitive consequences of forced compliance. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 58(2), 203–210.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fordyce, M. W. (1977). The happiness measures: A sixty-second index of emotional well-being and mental health. Manuscript submitted for publication.

  • Fordyce, M. W. (1988). A review of research on the happiness measures: A sixty-second index of happiness and mental health. Social Indicators Research, 20, 355–381.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fraisse, P. (1963). The psychology of time. New York, NY: Harper and Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fredrickson, B. L., & Kahneman, D. (1993). Duration neglect in retrospective evaluations of affective episodes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 45–55.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fredrickson, B. L., & Losada, M. F. (2005). Positive affect and the complex dynamics of human flourishing. American Psychologist, 60, 678–686.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fujita, F. (1991). An investigation of the relation between extroversion, neuroticism, positive affect, and negative affect. Unpublished masters thesis, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign.

  • Gilbert, D. (2006). Stumbling on happiness. New York, NY: Vintage Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, S. J., Dumontheil, I., Simons, J. S., Frith, C. D., & Burgess, P. W. (2007). Comment on wandering minds: The default network and stimulus-independent thought. Science, 317, 43.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, D. T., Pinel, E. C., Wilson, T. D., Blumberg, S. J., & Wheatley, T. P. (1998). Immune neglect: A source of durability bias in affective forecasting. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 617–638.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, D. T., & Wilson, T. D. (2007). Prospection: Experiencing the future. Science, 317, 1351–1354.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottman, J. M. (1994). What predicts divorce? The relationship between marital processes and marital outcomes. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guignon, C. (Ed.). (1999). The good life. Indianapolis, IN: Hackett.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gusnard, D. A., Akbudak, E., Shulman, G. L., & Raichle, M. E. (2001). Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: Relation to a default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 98, 4259–4264.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harley, T. A. (1995). The psychology of language: From data to theory. East Sussex, UK: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Havighurst, R. J., & Glasser, A. (1972). An exploratory study of reminiscence. Journals of Gerontology, 27, 245–253.

    Google Scholar 

  • Headey, B., Holmstrom, E., & Wearing, A. (1985). Models of well-being and ill-being. Social Indicators Research, 17, 211–234.

    Google Scholar 

  • Headey, B., Veenhoven, R., & Wearing, A. (1991). Top-down versus bottom-up theories of subjective well-being. Social Indicators Research, 24, 81–100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Headey, B., & Wearing, A. (1989). Personality, life events, and subjective well-being: Toward a dynamic equilibrium model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 731–739.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology (Vol. 1). New York, NY: Dover Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D. (1999). Objective happiness. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 3–25). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D. (2000). Evaluation by moments: Past and future. In D. Kahneman & A. Tversky (Eds.), Choices, values and frames (Vol. 38). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press and Russell Sage Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., Fredrickson, B., Schreiber, C. A., & Redelmeier, D. A. (1993). When more pain is preferred to less: Adding a better end. Psychological Science, 4(6), 401–405.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Krueger, A. B. (2006). Developments in the measurement of subjective well-being. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 20(1), 3–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., Krueger, A. B., Schkade, D. A., Schwarz, N., & Stone, A. A. (2004). A survey method for characterizing daily life experience: The day reconstruction method. Science, 306, 1776–1780.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., Krueger, A. B., Schkade, D. A., Schwarz, N., & Stone, A. A. (2006). Would you be happier if you were richer? A focusing illusion. Science, 312(5782), 1908–1910.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Miller, D. T. (1986). Norm theory: Comparing reality to its alternatives. Psychological Review, 93, 136–153.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Riis, J. (2005). Living, and thinking about it: Two perspectives on life. In F. A. Huppert, N. Baylis, & B. Keverne (Eds.), The science of well-being (pp. 285–304). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., Slovic, P., & Tversky, A. (Eds.). (1982). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kamvar, S., Mogilner, C., & Aaker, J. L. (2009). The meaning(s) of happiness. Stanford University Graduate School of Business Research Paper No. 2026. Retrieved from SSRN: http://ssrn.com/abstract=1418195.

  • Kassam, K., Gilbert, D. T., Boston, A., & Wilson, T. D. (2008). Future anhedonia and time discounting. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(6), 1533–1537.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (1993). A dark side of the American dream: Correlates of financial success as a life aspiration. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 410–422.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (1996). Further examining the American dream: Differential correlates of intrinsic and extrinsic goals. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 80–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keyes, C. L. M. (1998). Social well-being. Social Psychology Quarterly, 61, 121–140.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keyes, C. L. M. (2002). The mental health continuum: From languishing to flourishing in life. Journal of Health and Behavior Research, 43, 207–222.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kilpatrick, F. P., & Cantril, H. (1960). Self-anchoring scaling: A measure of individuals’ unique reality worlds. Journal of Individual Psychology, 16, 158–173.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kim-Prieto, C., Diener, E., Tamir, M., Scollon, C., & Diener, M. (2005). Integrating the diverse definitions of happiness: A time-sequential framework of subjective well-being. Journal of Happiness Studies, 6, 261–300.

    Google Scholar 

  • King, L. A., Eells, J. E., & Burton, C. M. (2004). The good life, broadly defined. In A. Linley & S. Joseph (Eds.), Positive psychology in practice (pp. 35–52). Hoboken, New Jersey: John Wiley and Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klinger, E., & Cox, W. M. (1987–1988). Dimensions of thought flow in everyday life. Imagination, cognition, and personality, 72, 105–128.

  • Koo, M., Algoe, S. B., Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2008). It’s a wonderful life: Mentally subtracting positive events improves people’s affective states, contrary to their affective forecasts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(5), 1217–1224.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kozma, A., Stone, S., & Stones, M. J. (2000). Stability in components and predictors of subjective well-being (SWB): Implications for SWB structure. In E. Diener & D. R. Rahtz (Eds.), Advances in quality of life theory and research (pp. 13–30). Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lazarus, R. S., Kanner, A. D., & Folkman, S. (1980). Emotions: A cognitive-phenomenological analysis. In R. Plutchik & H. Kellerman (Eds.), Theories of emotion (pp. 189–217). New York, NY: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lennings, C. J. (1996). Self-efficacy and temporal orientation as predictors of treatment outcome in severely dependent alcoholics. Alcoholism Treatment Quarterly, 14, 71–79.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewin, K. (1942). Time perspective and moral. New York, NY: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loewenstein, G., & Schkade, D. (1999). Wouldn’t it be nice? Predicting future feelings. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 85–105). New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lucas, R. E., Diener, E., & Suh, E. M. (1996). Discriminant validity of well-being measures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 616–628.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lykken, D., & Tellegen, A. (1996). Happiness is a stochastic phenomenon. Psychological Science, 7, 186–189.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyubomirsky, S., Sheldon, K. M., & Schkade, D. (2005). Pursuing happiness: The architecture of sustainable change. Review of General Psychology, 9, 111–131.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maddux, J. E. (2002). Self-efficacy: The power of believing you can. In C. R. Snyder & S. J. Lopez (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychology (pp. 277–287). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Magnus, K., & Diener, E. (1991). A longitudinal analysis of personality, life events, and subjective well-being. Paper presented at the 63rd annual meeting of the Midwestern Psychological Association, Chicago.

  • Marcus, G. (2008). Kluge. Boston, MD: Houghton Mifflin Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markus, H., & Nuris, P. (1986). Possible selves. American Psychologist, 41, 954–969.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maslow, A. H. (1943). A theory of human motivation. Psychological Review, 50(4), 370–396.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maslow, A. H. (1971). Farther reaches of human nature. New York, NY: Viking Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • McAdams, D. P. (1985). Power, intimacy, and the life story: Personological inquiries into identity. New York, NY: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGaugh, J. L. (2000). Memory—a century of consolidation. Science, 287, 248–251.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mclntosh, W. D., & Martin, L. L. (1992). The cybernetics of happiness: The relation between goal attainment, rumination, and affect. In M. S. Clark (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology (pp. 222–246). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Medvec, V. H., Madey, S. F., & Gilovich, T. (1995). When less is more: Counterfactual thinking and satisfaction among Olympic medalists. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 603–610.

    Google Scholar 

  • Metcalfe, J., & Mischel, W. (1999). A hot/cool system analysis of delay of gratification: Dynamics of willpower. Psychological Review, 106(1), 3–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Michalos, A. C. (1985). Multiple discrepancies theory (MDT). Social Indicators Research, 16, 347–413.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, R. B., Debacker, T. K., & Greene, B. A. (1999). Perceived instrumentality and academics: The links to task valuing. Journal of Instructional Psychology, 26, 250–260.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moran, S, Tirri, K., Ulisses, A., & Bundick, M. (2009). Finding purpose in three societies. Paper presented at the international positive psychology conference, Philadelphia, PA.

  • Myers, D. G., & Diener, E. (1995). Who is happy? Psychological Science, 6, 10–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norton, D. L. (1976). Personal destinies. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oishi S. (2004), On-line versus retrospective recall of emotions in the prediction of the longevity of dating relationships. University of Virginia. Unpublished manuscript.

  • Oishi, S., Diener, E., Choi, D. W., Kim-Prieto, C., & Choi, I. (2007). The dynamics of daily events and well-being across cultures: When less is more. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 685–698.

    Google Scholar 

  • Okun, M. A., & George, L. K. (1984). Physician- and self-ratings of health, neuroticism and subjective well-being among men and women. Personality and Individual Differences, 5, 533–539.

    Google Scholar 

  • Onoda, K., Okamoto, Y., & Yamawaki, S. (2009). Neural correlates of associative memory: The effects of negative emotion. Neuroscience Research, 64(1), 50–55.

    Google Scholar 

  • Park, N., Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. E. (2004). Strengths of character and well-being. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 23(5), 603–619.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pavot, W., Diener, E., Colvin, C. R., & Sandvik, E. (1991). Further validation of the satisfaction with life scale: Evidence for the cross-method convergence of well-being measures. Journal of Personality Assessment, 57, 149–161.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pavot, W., Diener, E., & Suh, E. (1998). The temporal satisfaction with life scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 70, 340–354.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, C. (2006). A primer in positive psychology. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, C., Park, N., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2005). Orientations to happiness and life satisfaction: The full life versus the empty life. Journal of Happiness Studies, 6, 25–41.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, C., Ruch, W., Beermann, U., Park, N., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2007). Strengths of character, orientations to happiness, and life satisfaction. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 3, 149–156.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riis, J., Loewenstein, G., Baron, J., Jepson, C., Fagerlin, A., & Ubel, P. A. (2005). Ignorance of hedonic adaptation to hemo-dialysis: A study using ecological momentary assessment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134(1), 3–9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, B. W., & Robins, R. W. (2000). Broad dispositions, broad aspirations: The intersection of the big five dimensions and major life goals. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 1284–1296.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roese, N. J. (1997). Counterfactual thinking. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 133–149.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roese, N. J., & Olson, J. M. (1995). Outcome controllability and counterfactual thinking. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 620–628.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rozin, P. (2008). MAPP On-Site2 Presentation [PowerPoint slides]. Retrieved from e-College @ UPenn Web site: http://www.advance.program.upenn.edu/ec/dcs/DocView.learn?CourseID=3039161&47=5001241&dt=10%2F16%2F2008+7%3A40%3A53+PM&DocID=16983338&DocCollab_PK=17234580&Name=MAPPclass08short.ppt.

  • Rozin, P. (2008). New uncertain vs. familiar-positive experiences [PowerPoint slides]. Retrieved from e-College @ UPenn Web site: http://www.sas.upenn.edu/lps/graduate/mapp/login.

  • Rozin, P., & Royzman, E. (2001). Negativity bias, negativity dominance, and contagion. Personality and social psychology review, 5(4), 296–320.

    Google Scholar 

  • Russell, B. (1930). The conquest of happiness. New York, NY: Liveright.

    Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J. A. (2003). Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. Psychological Review, 110(1), 145–172.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55(1), 68–78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryff, C. D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 1069–1081.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1985). Optimism, coping, and health: Assessment and implications of generalized outcome expectancies. Health Psychology, 4, 219–247.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schimmack, U. (2003). Affect measurement in experience sampling research. Journal of Happiness Studies, 4, 79–106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schimmack, U., Oishi, S., & Diener, E. (2002). Cultural influences on the relation between pleasant emotions and unpleasant emotions: Asian dialectic philosophies or individualism-collectivism. Cognition & Emotion, 16(6), 705–719.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schreiber, C. A., & Kahneman, D. (2000). Determinants of the remembered utility of aversive sounds. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129, 27–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1983). Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: Informative and directive functions of affective states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 513–523.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N., & Strack, F. (1991). Evaluating one’s life: A judgment model of subjective well-being. In F. Strack, M. Argyle, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Subjective well-being: An interdisciplinary perspective (pp. 27–47). Oxford, UK: Pergamon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N., Strack, F., Kommer, D., & Wagner, D. (1987). Soccer, rooms, and the quality of your life: Mood effects on judgments of satisfaction with life in general and with specific domains. European Journal of Social Psychology, 17, 69–79.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scollon, C. N., Diener, E., Oishi, S., & Biswas-Diener, R. (2005). An experience sampling and cross-cultural investigation of the relation between pleasant and unpleasant affect. Cognition and Emotion, 19(1), 27–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seidlitz, L., & Diener, E. (1993). Memory for positive versus negative life events: Theories for the differences between happy and unhappy persons. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 654–664.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seligman, M. E. (2006). Learned Optimism: How to change your mind and your life. New York, NY: Vintage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seligman, M. E., Steen, T. A., Park, N., & Peterson, C. (2005). Positive psychology progress: Empirical validation of interventions. American Psychologist, 60, 410–421.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sheldon, K. M., & Kasser, T. (1998). Pursuing personal goals: Skills enable progress, but not all progress is beneficial. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 1319–1331.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shulman, G. L., Fiez, J. A., Corbetta, M., Buckner, R. L., & Miezin, F. M. (1997). Common blood flow changes across visual tasks: II. Decreases in cerebral cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 648–663.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simons, J., Vansteenkiste, M., Lens, W., & Lacante, M. (2004). Placing motivation and future time perspective theory in a temporal perspective. Educational Psychology Review, 16(2), 121–139.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snyder, C. R. (2000). Handbook of hope: Theory, measures, and applications. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spinoza. (1985). Ethics. In Spinoza, The collected writings of Spinoza (Vol. 1, pp. 408–620) Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. (Original work published in 1677).

  • Staats, A. (1991). Unified positivism and unification psychology. American Psychologist, 46, 899–912.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stallings, M. C., Dunham, C. C., Gatz, M., Baker, L. A., & Bengtson, V. L. (1997). Relationships among life events and psychological well-being: More evidence for a two-factor theory of well-being. Journal of Applied Gerontology, 16, 104–119.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steger, M. F., Frazier, P., Oishi, S., & Kaler, M. (2006). The meaning in life questionnaire: Assessing the presence of and search for meaning in life. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 53, 80–93.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steger, M., Kashdan, T. B., Sullivan, B. A., & Lorentz, D. (2008). Understanding the search for meaning in life: Personality, cognitive style, and the dynamic between seeking and experiencing meaning. Journal of Personality, 76(2), 199–228.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stones, M. J., & Kozma, A. (1991). A magical model of happiness. Social Indicators Research, 25, 31–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strack, F., Schwarz, N., & Gschneidinger, E. (1985). Happiness and reminiscing: The role of time perspective, affect, and mode of thinking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 1460–1469.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stroebe, W., Stroebe, M., Abakoumkin, G., & Schut, H. (1996). The role of loneliness and social support in adjustment to loss: A test of attachment versus stress theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 1241–1249.

    Google Scholar 

  • Suh, D., Diener, E., & Fujita, F. (1996). Events and subjective well-being: Only recent events matter: Erratum. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 842.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, S. E. (1991). Asymmetrical effects of positive and negative events: The mobilization-minimization hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 67–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, D. L., & Diener, E. (1990). Memory accuracy in the recall of emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 291–297.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Praag, B. M. S., Frijters, P., & Ferrer-i-Carbonell, A. (2003). The anatomy of subjective well-being. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 51, 29–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Veenhoven, R. (1988). The utility of happiness. Social Indicators Research, 20, 333–354.

    Google Scholar 

  • Veenhoven, R. (1999). World database of happiness—continuous register of research on subjective appreciation of life. Rotterdam: Erasmus University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Veenhoven, R. (2004). Happy life years: A measure of gross national happiness. In K. Ura & K. Galay (Eds.), Gross national happiness and development (pp. 287–318). Thimphu, Bhutan: The Centre for Bhutan Studies.

    Google Scholar 

  • Waterman, A. S. (1993). Two conceptions of happiness: Contrasts of personal expressiveness (eudaimonia) and hedonic enjoyment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64(4), 678–691.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. (1895). Hedonic theories from Aristippus to Spencer. New York, NY: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, W. (1967). Correlates of avowed happiness. Psychological Bulletin, 67, 294–306.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, T. D., Centerbar, D. B., Kermer, D. A., & Gilbert, D. T. (2005). The pleasures of uncertainly: Prolonging positive moods in ways people do not anticipate. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 5–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, T. D., Meyers, J., & Gilbert, D. T. (2003). “How happy was I, anyway?” A retrospective impact bias. Social Cognition, 21, 421–446.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wirtz, D., Kruger, J., Scollon, C. N., & Diener, E. (2004). What to do on spring break? Predicting future choice from online versus recalled affect. Psychological Science, 14, 520–524.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wyvell, C. L., & Berridge, K. C. (2000). Intra-accumbens amphetamine increases the conditioned incentive salience of sucrose reward: Enhancement of reward “wanting” without enhanced “liking” or response reinforcement. The Journal of Neuroscience, 20(21), 8122–8130.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zaleski, Z., Cycon, A., & Kurc, A. (2001). Future time perspective and subjective well-being in adolescent samples. In P. Schmuck & K. M. Sheldon (Eds.), Life goals and well-being: Towards a positive psychology of human striving (pp. 58–67). Goettingen, Germany: Hogrefe & Huber Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimbardo, P. G. (2002). Just think about it: Time to take our time. Psychology Today, 35, 62.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimbardo, P. G., & Boyd, J. N. (1999). Putting time in perspective: A valid, reliable individual-differences metric. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(6), 1271–1288.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimbardo, P. G., & Boyd, J. N. (2008). The time paradox. New York, NY: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimbardo, P. G., Keough, K. A., & Boyd, J. N. (1997). Present time perspective as a predictor of risky driving. Personality and Individual Differences, 23, 1007–1023.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Adorée Durayappah.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Durayappah, A. The 3P Model: A General Theory of Subjective Well-Being. J Happiness Stud 12, 681–716 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-010-9223-9

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-010-9223-9

Keywords

Navigation