Skip to main content
Log in

Toward a behavioral analysis of joint attention

  • Published:
The Behavior Analyst Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Joint attention (JA) initiation is defined in cognitive-developmental psychology as a child’s actions that verify or produce simultaneous attending by that child and an adult to some object or event in the environment so that both may experience the object or event together. This paper presents a contingency analysis of gaze shift in JA initiation. The analysis describes reinforcer-establishing and evocative effects of antecedent objects or events, discriminative and conditioned reinforcing functions of stimuli generated by adult behavior, and socially mediated reinforcers that may maintain JA behavior. A functional analysis of JA may describe multiple operant classes. The paper concludes with a discussion of JA deficits in children with autism spectrum disorders and suggestions for research and treatment.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bakeman, R., & Adamson, L. (1984). Coordinating attention to people and objects in mother-infant and peer-infant interaction. Child Development, 55, 1278–1289.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). Perceptual role-taking and protodeclarative pointing in autism. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 7, 113–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Baldwin, D. A., & Crowson, M. (1997). Do children with autism use the speaker’s direction of gaze strategy to crack the code of language? Child Development, 68, 48–57.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bijou, S. W., & Ghezzi, P. M. (1999). The behavioral interference theory of autistic behavior in young children. In P. M. Ghezzi, W. L. Williams, & J. E. Carr (Eds.), Autism: Behavior-analytic perspectives (pp. 33–43). Reno, NV: Context Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, M., Pennington, B. E, & Rogers, S. J. (2002). Interrelations among social-cognitive skills in young children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32, 91–106.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Charman, T. (2003). Why is joint attention a pivotal skill in autism? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B—Biological Sciences, 358, 315–324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Charman, T., Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Cox, A., Baird, G., & Drew, A. (1997). Infants with autism: An investigation of empathy, pretend play, joint attention, and imitation. Developmental Psychology, 33, 781–789.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, G., Meltzoff, A., Osterling, J., Rinaldi, J., & Brown, E. (1998). Children with autism fail to orient to naturally occurring social stimuli. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28, 479–485.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, G., Munson, J., Estes, A., Osterling, J., McPartland, J., Toth, K., et al. (2002). Neurocognitive function and joint attention ability in young children with autism spectrum disorder versus developmental delay. Child Development, 73, 345–358.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Delgado, C. E. F., Mundy, P., Crowson, M, Markus, J., Yale, M., & Schwartz, H. (2002). Responding to joint attention and language development: A comparison of target locations. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45, 715–719.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Donahoe, J. W., & Palmer, D. C. (1989). The interpretation of complex human behavior: Some reactions to Parallel Distributed Processing, edited by J. L. McClelland, D. E. Rumelhart, and the PDP Research Group. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 51, 399–416.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Dube, W. V., & McIlvane, W. J. (1999). Reduction of stimulus overselectivity with nonverbal differential observing responses. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32, 25–33.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gewirtz, J. L., & Peláez-Nogueras, M. (1992). Infant social referencing as a learned process. In S. Feinman (Ed.), Social referencing and the social construction of reality in infancy (pp. 151–173). New York: Plenu

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Golinkoff, R. M., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Bailey, L. M., & Wenger, N. R. (1992). Young children and adults use lexical principles to learn new nouns. Developmental Psychology, 28, 99–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Green, G. (1996). Early behavioral intervention for autism: What does research tell us? In C. Maurice, G. Green, & S. C. Luce (Eds.), Behavioral intervention for young children with autism (pp. 29–44). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gutowski, S., & Stromer, R. (2003). Delayed matching to two-picture samples by individuals with and without disabilities: An analysis of the role of naming. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 487–504.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Harding, J. W, Wacker, D. P., Berg, W. K., Cooper, L. J., Asmus, J., Mlela, K., et al. (1999). An analysis of choice making in the assessment of young children with severe behavior problems. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32, 63–82.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Horner, R. H., Carr, E. G., Strain, P. S., Todd, A. W, & Reed, H. K. (2002). Problem behavior interventions for young children with autism: A research synthesis. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32, 423–446.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, E. A., & Carr, E. G. (2004). Joint attention in children with autism: Theory and intervention. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 19, 13–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kasari, C. (2002). Assessing change in early intervention programs for children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32, 447–461.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kasari, C, Freeman, S. F N., & Paparella, T (2001). Early intervention in autism: Joint attention and symbolic play. In L. M. Glidden (Ed.), International review of research in mental retardation: Autism (Vol. 23, pp. 207–237). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koegel, R. L., & Schreibman, L. (1977). Teaching autistic children to respond to simultaneous multiple cues. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 24, 299–311.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Laraway, S., Snycerski, S., Michael, J., & Poling, A. (2003). Motivating operations and terms to describe them: Some further refinements. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 407–414.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Leekam, S., Hunnisett, E., & Moore, C. (1998). Targets and cues: Gaze following in children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 951–962.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C, Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C, & Risi, S. (1999). Autism diagnostic observation schedule. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lovaas, O. I. (1987). Behavioral treatment and normal educational and intellectual functioning in young autistic children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 3–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lovaas, O. I., Freitag, G., Kinder, M. I., Rubenstein, B. D., Schaeffer, B., & Simmons, J. Q. (1966). Establishment of social reinforcers in two schizophrenic children on the basis of food. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 4, 109–125.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lovaas, O. I., Koegel, R. L., & Schreibman, L. (1979). Stimulus overselectivity in autism: A review of research. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 1236–1254.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lowenkron, B. (1998). Some logical functions of joint control. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 69, 327–354.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Markman, E. M. (1989). Categorization and naming in children. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McEachlin, J. J., Smith, T, & Lovaas, O. I. (1993). Long term outcome for children with autism who received early intensive behavioral treatment. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 97, 359–372.

    Google Scholar 

  • Michael, J. (1982). Distinguishing between discriminative and motivational functions of stimuli. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37, 149–155.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Michael, J. (1993). Establishing operations. The Behavior Analyst, 16, 191–206.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., & Crowson, M. (1997). Joint attention and early social communication: Implications for research on intervention with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27, 653–676.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., & Neal, A. R. (2001). Neural plasticity, joint attention, and a transactional social-orienting model of autism. International review of research in mental retardation (Vol. 23, pp. 139–168). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1990). A longitudinal study of joint attention and language development in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, 115–128.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1994). Joint attention, developmental level, and symptom presentation in autism. Development and Psychopathology, 6, 389–401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., Sigman, M. D., Ungerer, J., & Sherman, T. (1986). Defining the social deficits of autism: The contribution of non-verbal communication measures. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines, 27, 657–669.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Novak, G., & Peláez, M. (2004). Child and adolescent development: A behavioral systems approach. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, S. J. (2001). Diagnosis of autism before the age of 3. In L. M. Glidden (Ed.), International review of research in mental retardation (Vol. 23, pp. 1–31). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scaife, M., & Bruner, J. S. (1975). The capacity for joint visual attention in the infant. Nature, 253, 265–266.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schreibman, L. (1997). The study of stimulus control in autism. In D. M. Baer & E. M. Pinkston (Eds.), Environment and behavior (pp. 203–209). Boulder, CO: Westview.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seibert, J., Hogan, A., & Mundy, P. (1984). Mental age and cognitive stage in young handicapped and at-risk children. Intelligence, 8, 11–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. E (1957). Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. E (1969). Contingencies of reinforcement. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (1995). Joint attention in social cognition. In C. Moore & P. J. Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention (pp. 103–130). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., Call, J., & Hare, B. (1998). Five primate species follow the visual gaze of conspecifics. Animal Behaviour, 55, 1063–1069.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Whalen, C, & Schreibman, L. (2003). Joint attention training for children with autism using behavior modification procedures. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 456–468.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilkinson, K. M., Dube, W. V, & Mcllvane, W. J. (1996). A crossdisciplinary perspective on studies of rapid word mapping in psycholinguistics and behavior analysis. Developmental Review, 16, 125–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to William V. Dube.

Additional information

Partial support for manuscript preparation was provided by Grant HD046666 from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. The contents of this paper are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official views of NICHD.

We thank Shelly Cota for help with data collection and Bill McIlvane, Jennifer Andersen, June Sanchez, Kristine Wiltz, and Ellyn South for their comments.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Dube, W.V., MacDonald, R.P.F., Mansfield, R.C. et al. Toward a behavioral analysis of joint attention. BEHAV ANALYST 27, 197–207 (2004). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03393180

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03393180

Key words

Navigation