Skip to main content
Log in

Parental contribution to child’s early language and interest in books

  • Published:
European Journal of Psychology of Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The relationships between parents’ age, education, literacy activities and shared reading with the child and children’s language skills and early interest in books were examined in a longitudinal study of 108 children. Parents reported on their children’s lexical and grammatical development by using the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories (the CDIs) at the ages of 14 and 24 months. The Bayley Scales of Infant Development were administered to the children in a laboratory setting at 24 months. Information on parental background variables was obtained through a questionnaire before the children’s birth. Book reading habits were inquired when the children were 2 years of age. Mothers’ education, literacy activities and shared reading with the child were shown to be more strongly associated with the 2-year-olds’ lexical and grammatical skills than were those of father. A corresponding association to parental background variables emerged regardless of whether parental report data or scores on the structured test were employed as the child language measure. Shared reading with the father was found to be linked to children’s early interest in books. The children who exhibited greater interest in books were likely to be read to by mothers and fathers more frequently than other children. These children also had larger vocabularies than did children with low interest in books. The role of endogenous and exogenous variables in explaining children’s language skills and early book reading interest are discussed.

Résumé

Les liens entre, d’une part, l’âge des parents, leur niveau de formation, leurs activités en matière d’alphabétisation et les moments de lecture partagés avec l’enfant et, d’autre part, les aptitudes langagières des enfants et leur intérêt précoce pour les livres ont été examinés dans une étude longitudinale portant sur 108 enfants. Les parents ont fait un compte rendu sur le développement lexical et grammatical de leurs enfants en utilisant les Inventaires de développement communicationnel (Communicative Development Inventories, CDI) de MacArthur, à l’âge de 14 et de 24 mois. L’échelle de développement infantile de Bayley a été appliquée aux enfants dans des conditions de laboratoire à l’âge de 24 mois. Des renseignements sur les paramètres du milieu parental ont été recueillis par voie de questionnaire avant la naissance de l’enfant. Il a été dressé un relevé des habitudes de lecture de livres une fois que les enfants ont atteint l’âge de deux ans. Il ressort que le niveau de formation de la mère, ses activités en matière d’alphabétisation de l’enfant et les moments de lecture en commun avec l’enfant sont plus fortement assiociés avec les aptitudes lexicales et grammaticales de l’enfant de deux ans qui’ils ne le sont pour le père. Il peut être mis en évidence un lien concordant avec les paramètres du milieu parental indépendants de la méthode d’évaluation du langage chez l’enfant — compte rendu des parents ou notes obtenues au test structuré. La probabilité que les enfants témoignant d’un plus grand intérêt pour les livres soient des enfants à qui la mère ou le père ont fait de la lecture est plus grande que chez les autres enfants. Ces enfants avaient également un vocabulaire plus développé que les enfants témoignant d’un moindre intérêt pour les livres. Le rôle des variables endogènes et exogènes dans l’explication des aptitudes langagières des enfants et de leur intérêt précoce pour les livres fait l’objet d’une discussion.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bates, J.E. (1994). Parents as scientific observers of their children’s development. In S.L. Friedman & H.C. Haywood (Eds.),Developmental follow-up. Concepts, domains, and methods (pp. 197–217). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bayley, N. (1993).Bayley Scales of Infant Development (2nd ed.). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation, Harcourt Brace.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bee, H., Barnard, K., Eyres, S., Gray, C., Hammond, M., Spietz, A., Snyder, C., & Clark, B. (1982). Prediction of IQ and language skill from perinatal status, child performance, family characteristics, and mother-infant interaction.Child Development, 53, 1134–1156.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bus, A., van IJzendoorn, M., & Pellegrini, A. (1995). Joint book reading makes for success in learning to read: A meta-analysis on integrational transmission of literacy.Review of Educational Research, 65, 1–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cox, T. (1987). Educational disadvantage: The bearing of the early home background on children’s academic attainment and school progress.Early Child Development and Care, 27, 219–237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dale, P.S. (1996). Parent report assessment of language and communication. In K.N. Cole, P.S. Dale, & D.J. Thal (Eds.),Assessment of communication and language (vol. 6, pp. 161–182). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.

    Google Scholar 

  • Debaryshe, B.D. (1993). Joint picture-book reading correlates of early oral language skill.Journal of Child Language, 20, 455–461.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fenson, L., Dale, P.S., Reznick, J.S., Bates, E., Thal, D., & Pethick, S.J. (1994). Variability in early communicative development.Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59 (5, Serial No 242).

  • Golden, M., & Birns, B. (1976). Social class and infant development. In M. Lewis (Ed.),Origins of intelligence. Infancy and early childhood (pp. 299–351). New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Griffin, E.A., & Morrison, F.J. (1997). The unique contribution of home literacy environment to differences in early literacy skills.Early Child Development and Care, 127–128, 233–243.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jackson-Maldonado, D., Thal, D., Bates, E., Marchman, V., & Guitierrez-Clellen, V. (1993). Early lexical development of Spanish-speaking infants and toddlers.Journal of Child Language, 20, 523–549.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kruper, J.C., & Uzgiris, I.C. (1987). Fathers’ and mothers’ speech to young infants.Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 16, 597–615.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laakso, M.-L. (1995). Mothers’ and fathers’ communication clarity and teaching strategies with their school-aged children.Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 3, 445–462.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Labrell F. (1996). Paternal play with todelers: Recreation and creation.European Journal of Psychology of Education, 11, 43–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lonigan, C. (1994). Reading to preschoolers exposed: Is the emperor really naked?Developmental Review, 14, 303–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lyytinen, P., Poikkeus, A.-M., Leiwo, M., Ahonen, T., & Lyytinen, H. (1996). Parents as informants of their child’s vocal and early language development.Early Child Development and Care, 126, 15–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nugent, J.K. (1991). Cultural and psychological influences on the father’s role in infant development.Journal of Marriage and the Family, 53, 475–485.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pennington B.F. (1991).Diagnosing learning disorders: A neuropsychological framework. New York: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Power, T.G., & Parke, R.D. (1982). Play as a context for early learning. In L.M. Laosa & I.E. Sigel (Eds.),Families as learning environments for children (pp. 147–178). New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reznick, J.S., & Goldfield, B. (1994). Diary vs. representative checklist assessment of productive vocabulary.Journal of Child Language, 21, 465–472.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rowe, K.J. (1991). The influence of reading activity at home on students’ attitudes towards reading, classroom attentiveness and reading achievement: An application of structural equation modelling.British Journal of Educational Psychology, 61, 19–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scarborough, H.S., & Dobrich, W. (1994). On the efficacy of reading to preschoolers.Developmental Review, 14, 245–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shapiro, J., Anderson, A. (1997). Diversity in parental storybook reading.Early Child Development and Care, 127–128, 47–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, L. (1981). Infant tests as predictors of cognitive and language development at two years.Child Development, 52, 545–557.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, L.S., Cooper, D.C., Fitzhardinge, P.M., & Ash, A.J. (1995). The use of the mental development index of the Bayley Scale to diagnose language delay in 2-year-old high-risk infants.Infant Behavior and Development, 18, 483–486.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thal, D.J., & Katich, J. (1996). Predicaments in early identification of specific language impairment: Does the early bird always catch the worm? In K.N. Cole, P.S. Dale, & D.J. Thal (Eds.),Assessment of communication and language (vol. 6, pp 1–28). Baltimore: Paul H. Brooks.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Lyytinen, P., Laakso, M.L. & Poikkeus, A.M. Parental contribution to child’s early language and interest in books. Eur J Psychol Educ 13, 297–308 (1998). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03172946

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03172946

Key words

Navigation