Skip to main content
Log in

The right and wrong places to look for the cognitive locus of reading disability

  • Part II Theoretical Seeds
  • Published:
Annals of Dyslexia Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

A coherent conception of dyslexia has been difficult to arrive at because research findings have continually created logical paradoxes for the psychometric definition of reading disability. One such paradox is that cognitive research has undermined the assumption of specificity in the definition of dyslexia. The assumption of specificity is the underlying premise that the cognitive problems characteristic of the dyslexic child are reasonably specific to the reading task and do not implicate broader domains of cognitive functioning. This paper demonstrates how to develop hypotheses about the cognitive deficits of dyslexic children that do not undermine the assumption of specificity. Phonological awareness is explored as one candidate process.

It is argued that in order to avoid the pitfalls surrounding the assumption of specificity three things are important: 1) We must understand the operation of Matthew effects in education (rich-get-richer and poor-get-poorer effects); 2) Strict psychometric criteria must be used to define dyslexia; 3) We must continually be aware that dyslexia does not demarcate a discrete category, separate from other groups of poor readers, but represents the outcome of the application of an arbitrary criterion in a continuous distribution.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Allington, R. L. 1980. Poor readers don’t get to read much in reading groups.Language Arts 57:872–876.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allington, R. L. 1983. The reading instruction provided readers of differing reading abilities.The Elementary School Journal 83:548–559.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Allington, R. L. 1984. Content coverage and contextual reading in reading groups.Journal of Reading Behavior 16:85–96.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allington, R. L. 1977. If they don’t read much, how they ever gonna get good?Journal of Reading 21:57–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aman, M. and Singh, N. 1983. Specific reading disorders: Concepts of etiology reconsidered.In K. Gadow and I. Bailer (eds.).Advances in Learning and Behavioral Disabilities (Vol. 2, pp. 1–47). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baker, L. 1982. An evaluation of the role of metacognitive deficits in learning disabilities.Topics in Learning and Learning Disabilities 2:27–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron, J. 1978. Intelligence and general strategies.In G. Underwood (ed.).Strategies in Information Processing (pp. 403-450). London:Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bauer, R. 1977. Memory processes in children with learning disabilities: Evidence for deficient rehearsal.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 24:415–430.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bauer, R. 1979. Memory, acquisition, and category clustering in learning-disabled children.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 27:365–383.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bauer, R. 1982. Information processing as a way of understanding and diagnosing learning disabilities.Topics in Learning and Learning Disabilities 2:33–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berger, N. 1978. Why can’t John read? Perhaps he’s not a good listener.Journal of Learning Disabilities 11:633–638.

    Google Scholar 

  • Biemiller, A. 1977–1978. Relationships between oral reading rates for letters, words, and simple text in the development of reading achievement.Reading Research Quarterly 13: 223–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. and Butterworth, G. 1980. Verbal-performance discrepancies: Relationship to both risk and specific reading retardation.Cortex 16:375–389.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bjorklund, D. and Bernholtz, J. 1986. The role of knowledge base in the memory performance of good and poor readers.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 41:367–393.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, L. and Bryant, P. E. 1985.Rhyme and Reason in Reading and Spelling. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brainerd, C., Kingma, J., and Howe, M. 1986. Long-term memory development and learning disability: Storage and retrieval loci of disabled/nondisabled differences.In S. Ceci (ed.).Handbook of Cognitive, Social, and Neuropsychological Aspects of Learning Disabilities (Vol. 1, pp. 161–184). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bristow, P. S. 1985. Are poor readers passive readers?: Some evidence, possible explanations, and potential solutions.The Reading Teacher 39:318–325.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butkowsky, S. and Willows, D. 1980. Cognitive-motivational characteristics of children varying in reading ability: Evidence for learned helplessness in poor readers.Journal of Educational Psychology 72:408–422.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, B. 1981. Deficient syntactic control in poor readers: Is a weak phonetic memory code responsible?Applied Psycholinguistics 2:201–212.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campione, J. and Brown, A. 1978. Toward a theory of intelligence: Contributions from research with retarded children.Intelligence 2:279–304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carr, T. 1981. Building theories of reading ability: On the relation between individual differences in cognitive skills and reading comprehension.Cognition 9:73–113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chall, S. 1983.Stages of Reading Development. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, R. L. 1982. Individual differences in short-term memory.In N. Ellis (ed.).International Review of Research in Mental Retardation (Vol. II, pp. 43–77). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeSoto, J. and DeSoto, C. 1983. Relationship of reading achievement to verbal processing abilities.Journal of Educational Psychology 75:116–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Downing, J. Learning to read with understanding. 1980.In C. M. McCullough (ed.).Persistent Problems in Reading Education (pp. 163–178). Newark, DE: International Reading Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, A. W. 1985. The cognitive neuropsychology of developmental (and acquired) dyslexia: A critical survey.Cognitive Neuropsychology 2:169–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, N. and Large, B. 1987. The development of reading: As you seek so shall you find.British Journal of Psychology 78:1–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fielding, L., Wilson, P., and Anderson 1986. A new focus on free reading: The role of trade books in reading instruction.In T. Raphael and R. Reynolds (eds.).Contexts of Literacy (pp. 149–160). New York: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. 1983.Modularity of Mind. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forell, E. R. 1985. The case for conservative reader placement.The Reading Teacher 35:857–862.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foster, R. and Gavelek, J. 1983. Development of intentional forgetting in normal and reading-delayed children.Journal of Educational Psychology 75:431–440.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, B. and Routh, D. K. 1984. Phonemic analysis and synthesis as word attack skills: Revisited.Journal of Educational Psychology 76:1059–1064.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gambrell, L. B., Wilson, R. M., and Gantt, W. N. (1981). Classroom observations of task-attending behaviors of good and poor readers.Journal of Educational Research 74:400–404.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gough, P. B. 1983. Context, form, and interaction.In K. Rayner (ed.).Eye Movements in Reading (pp. 203–211). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gough, P. B. and Hillinger, M. L. 1980. Learning to read: An unnatural act.Bulletin of the Orton Society 30:171–176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gough, P. B. and Tunmer, W. E. 1986. Decoding, reading, and reading disability.Remedial and Special Education 7:6–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hagen, J., Barclay, C., and Newman, R. 1982. Metacognition, self-knowledge, and learning disabilities: Some thoughts on knowing and doing.Topics in Learning and Learning Disabilities 2:19–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. and Humphreys, M. 1982. Research on specific learning disabilities: Deficits and remediation.Topics in Learning and Learning Disabilities 2:68–78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hallahan, D. and Bryan, T. 1981. Learning disabilities.In J. Kauffman and D. Hallahan (eds.).Handbook of Special Education (pp. 141–164). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jencks, C. 1972.Inequality. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jorm, A. 1983. Specific reading retardation and working memory: A review.British Journal of Psychology 74:311–342.

    Google Scholar 

  • Juel, C. 1988, March.Learning to read and write: A longitudinal study of fifty-four children from first through fourth grade. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, New Orleans.

  • Juel, C., Griffith, P. L., and Gough, P. B. 1986. Acquisition of literacy: A longitudinal study of children in first and second grade.Journal of Educational Psychology 78:243–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kavale, K. and Mattson, P. 1983. “One jumped off the balance beam”: Meta-analysis of perceptual-motor training.Journal of Learning Disabilities 16:165–173.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kotsonis, M. and Patterson, C. 1980. Comprehension-monitoring skills in learning-disabled children.Developmental Psychology 16:541–542.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LaBerge, D. and Samuels, S. 1974. Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading.Cognitive Psychology 6:293–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liberman, I. Y. and Shankweiler, D. 1985. Phonology and the problems of learning to read and write.Remedial and Special Education 6:8–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Licht, B. and Dweck, C. 1984. Determinants of academic achievement: The interaction of children’s achievement orientations with skill area.Developmental Psychology 20:628–636.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lundberg, I. 1987. Are letters necessary for the development of phonemic awareness?Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive 7:472–475.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lundberg, I., Oloffson, A., and Wall, S. 1980. Reading and spelling skills in the first school years predicted from phonemic awareness skills in kindergarten.Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 21:159–173.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maclean, M., Bryant, P., and Bradley, L. 1987. Rhymes, nursery rhymes, and reading in early childhood.Merril-Palmer Quarterly 33:255–281.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mann, V. 1984. Reading skill and language skill.Developmental Review 4:1–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mann, V. 1986. Why some children encounter reading problems.In J. Torgesen and B. Wong (eds.).Psychological and Educational Perspectives on Learning Disabilities (pp. 133–159). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Menyuk, P. and Flood, J. 1981. Linguistic competence, reading, writing problems, and remediation.Bulletin of the Orton Society 31:13–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, D. 1982.The Process of Reading: A cognitive analysis of fluent reading and learning to read. Chichester: John Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morrison, F. 1984 Word decoding and rule-learning in normal and disabled readers.Remedial and Special Education 5:20–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morrison, F. J. 1987. The nature of reading disability: Toward an integrative framework.In S. Ceci (ed.).Handbook of Cognitive, Social, and Neuropsychological Aspects of Learning Disabilities (pp. 33–62). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morrison, F. and Manis, F. 1982. Cognitive processes and reading disability: A critique and proposal.In C. Brainerd and M. Pressley (eds.).Program in Cognitive Development Research (Vol. 2, pp. 59–94). New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nagy, W. E., Herman, P. A., and Anderson, R. C. 1985. Learning words from context.Reading Research Quarterly 20:233–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nagy, W. E. and Anderson, R. C. 1984. How many words are there in printed school English?Reading Research Quarterly 19:304–330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newcomer, P. and Magee, P. 1977. The performance of learning (reading) disabled children on a test of spoken language.The Reading Teacher 30:896–900.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newman, D. S. and Hagen, J. W. 1981. Memory strategies in children with learning disabilities.Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 1:297–312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oka, E. and Paris, S. 1987. Patterns of motivation and reading skills in underachieving children.In S. Ceci (ed.).Handbook of Cognitive, Social, and Neuropsychological Aspects of Learning Disabilities (Vol. 2, pp. 115–145). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olofsson, A. and Lundberg, I. 1985. Evaluation of long term effects of phonemic awareness training in kindergarten.Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 26:21–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olson, R., Kliegl, R., Davidson, B., and Foltz, G. 1985. Individual and developmental differences in reading disability.In T. Waller (ed.)Reading Research: Advances in theory and practice (Vol. 4, pp. 1–64). London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Palmer, J., MacLeod, C., Hunt, E., and Davidson, J. 1985. Information processing correlates of reading.Journal of Memory and Language 24:59–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Patterson, K., Marshall, J., and Coltheart M. 1985.Surface Dyslexia. London: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pennington, B. F. 1986. Issues in the diagnosis and phenotype analysis of dyslexia: Implications for family studies.In S. D. Smith (ed.)Genetics and Learning Disabilities (pp. 69–96). San Diego: College-Hill Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A. 1985.Reading Ability. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A., Beck, I., Bell, L., and Hughes, C. 1987. Phonemic knowledge and learning to read are reciprocal: A longitudinal study of first grade children.Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 33:283–319.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rodgers, B. 1983. The identification and prevalence of specific reading retardation.British Journal of Educational Psychology 53:369–373.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M. 1983. School effects on pupil progress: Research findings and policy implications.Child Development 54:1–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M. and Yule, W. 1975. The concept of specific reading retardation.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 16:181–197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Satz, P., Morris, R., and Fletcher, J. M. 1985. Hypotheses, subtypes, and individual differences in dyslexia: Some reflections.In D. B. Gray and J. F. Kavanagh (eds.).Biobehavorial Measures of Dyslexia (pp. 25–40). Parkton, MD: York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scarborough, H. S. 1984. Continuity between childhood dyslexia and adult reading.British Journal of Psychology 75:329–348.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M. 1985a, Lexicon as module.Behavioral and Brain Sceinces 8:31–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M. 1985b. The time course of information activation and utilization in visual word recognition.In D. Besner, T. Waller, and G. MacKinnon (eds.).Reading Research: Advances in theory and practice (Vol. 5, pp. 199–252). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M. S., Bruck, M., Fornarolo, G., and Backman, J. (1985). Word recognition processes of poor and disabled readers? Do they necessarily differ?Applied Psycholinguists 6:161–180.

    Google Scholar 

  • Semel, E. and Wiig, E. 1975. Comprehension of syntactic structures and critical verbal elements by children with learning disabilities.Journal of Learning Disabilities 8:53–58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shankweiler, D. and Liberman, I. 1972. Misreading: A search for causes.In J. Kavanagh and I. Mattingly (eds.).Language by Ear and Eye (pp. 106–133). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L., Jorm, A. F., Maclean, R., and Matthews, R. 1984. Sources of individual differences in reading acquisition.Journal of Educational Psychology 76:1309–1324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L., McGee, R., McKenzie, D., Williams, S., and Silva, P. A. 1987. Further evidence relating to the distinction between specific reading retardation and general reading backwardness.British Journal of Developmental Psychology 5:35–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L., McGee, R., and Silva, P. A. In press. IQ and reading progress: A test of the “milk and jug” hypothesis.Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry.

  • Share, D. L. and Silva, P. A. In press. Language deficits and specific reading retardation: Cause or effect?British Journal of Disorders of Communication.

  • Shephard, M. J., Gelzheiser, L. M., and Solar, R. A. 1985. How good is the evidence for a production deficiency among learning disabled students?Journal of Educational Psychology 77:553–561.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, L. and Ryan, E. 1984. Reading disability as a language disorder.Remedial and Special Education 5:28–33.

    Google Scholar 

  • Silva, P. A., McGee, R., and Williams S. 1985. Some characteristics of 9-year-old boys with general reading backwardness or specific reading retardation.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 26:407–421.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Singer, M. and Crouse, J. 1981. The relationship of context-use skills to reading: A case for an alternative experimental logic.Child Development 52:1326–1329.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smiley, S., Oakley, D., Worthen, D., Campione, J., and Brown, A. 1977. Recall of thematically relevant material by adolescent good and poor readers as a function of written versus oral presentation.Journal of Educational Psychology 69:381–387.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. 1980. Toward an interactive-compensatory model of individual differences in the development of reading fluency.Reading Research Quarterly 16:32–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. 1982a. Individual differences in the cognitive processes of reading I.: Word decoding.Journal of Learning Disabilities 15:485–493.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. 1982b. Individual differences in the cognitive processes of reading II.: Text-level processes.Journal of Learning Disabilities 14:549–554.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. 1986a. Cognitive processes and the reading problems of learning disabled children: Evaluating the assumption of specificity.In J. Torgesen and B. Wong (eds.).Psychological and Educational Perspectives on Learning Disabilites (pp. 87–131). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. 1986b. Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy.Reading Research Quarterly 21:360–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. 1988. Speculations on the causes and consequences of individual differences in early reading acquisition.In P. Gough (ed.).Reading Acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E., Cunningham, A. E., and Feeman, D. J. 1984. Intelligence, cognitive skills, and early reading progress.Reading Research Quarterly 19:278–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E., Nathan, R. G., and Vala-Rossi, M. 1986. Developmental changes in the cognitive correlates of reading ability and the developmental lag hypothesis.Reading Research Quarterly 21:267–283.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E., Nathan, R. G., West, R. F., and Vala-Rossi, M. 1985. Children’s word recognition in context: Spreading activation, expectancy, and modularity.Child Development 56:1418–1429.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E., Nathan, R. G., and Zolman, J. E. 1988. The developmental lag hypothesis in reading: Longitudinal and matched reading-level comparisons.Child Development 59:71–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. and West, R. F. 1983. On priming by a sentence context.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 112:1–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stein, C. L., Cairns, H. S., and Zurif, E. B. 1984. Sentence comprehension limitations related to syntactic deficits in reading-disabled children.Applied Psycholinguistics 5:305–322.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. 1980. Sketch of a componential subtheory of human intelligence.Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3:573–584.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. 1982. Introduction: Some common themes in contemporary approaches to the training of intelligent performances.In D. Detterman and R. Sternberg (eds.).How and How Much Can Intelligence Be Increased? (pp. 141–146). Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. 1985.Beyond IQ: A triarchic theory of human intelligence. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sticht, T. 1979. Applications of the audred model to reading evaluation and instruction.In L. B. Resnick and P. Weaver (eds.).Theory and Practice of Early Reading (pp. 209–226). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tarver, S. G., Hallahan, D. P., Kauffman, J. M., and Ball, D. W. 1976. Verbal rehearsal and selective attention in children with learning disabilities: A developmental lag.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 22:275–285.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen, J. 1977a. Memorization processes in reading-disabled children.Journal of Educational Psychology 69:571–578.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen, J. 1977b. The role of nonspecific factors in the task performance of learning disabled children: A theoretical assessment.Journal of Learning Disabilities 10:27–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen, J. 1978–79. Performance of reading disabled children on serial memory tasks.Reading Research Quarterly 14:57–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen, J. 1985. Memory processes in reading disabled children.Journal of Learning Disabilities 18:350–357.

    Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen, J. and Goldman, T. 1977. Verbal rehearsal and short-term memory in reading-disabled children.Child Development 48:56–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen, J. and Houck, D. 1980. Processing deficiencies of learning-disabled children who perform poorly on the digit span test.Journal of Educational Psychology 72:141–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen, J. and Licht, B. 1983. The learning disabled child as an inactive learner: Retrospect and prospects.In J. McKinney and L. Feagans (eds.).Topics in Learning Disabilities (Vol. 1, pp. 100–130). Norwood, NJ: Ablex Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Torneus, M. 1984. Phonological awareness and reading: A chicken and egg problem?Journal of Educational Psychology 70:1346–1358.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R. and Baron, J. 1983. Phonemic-analysis training helps children benefit from spelling-sound rules.Memory and Cognition 11:382–389.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tunmer, W. E. and Nesdale, A. R. 1985. Phonemic segmentation skill and beginning reading.Journal of Educational Psychology 77:417–427.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van der Wissel, A. and Zegers, F. E. 1985. Reading retardation revisited.British Journal of Developmental Psychology 3:3–9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vellutino, F. 1979.Dyslexia: Theory and research. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vogel, S. 1974. Syntactic abilities in normal and dyslexic children.Journal of Learning Disabilities 7:103–109.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K. and Torgesen, J. K. 1987. The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills.Psychological Bulletin 101:192–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walberg, H. J., Strykowski, B. F., Rovai, E., and Hung, S. S. 1984. Exceptional performance.Review of Educational Research 54:87–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walberg, H. J. and Tsai, S. 1983. Matthew effects in education.American Educational Research Journal 20:359–373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, J. 1984. Phonemic analysis and how it relates to reading.Journal of Learning Disabilities 17:240–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wong, B. 1984. Metacognition and learning disabilities.In T. Waller, D. Forrest, and E. MacKinnon (eds.).Metacognition, Cognition, and Human Performance (pp. 137–180). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zifcak, M. 1981. Phonological awareness and reading acquisition.Contemporary Educational Psychology 6:117–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Stanovich, K.E. The right and wrong places to look for the cognitive locus of reading disability. Annals of Dyslexia 38, 154–177 (1988). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02648254

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02648254

Keywords

Navigation