Skip to main content
Log in

Conduct-disordered boys' perceptions of their liked peers

  • Published:
Cognitive Therapy and Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Person perception is one of several aspects of the first steps in social information-processing, viz, encoding and mental representation of social cues. To study person perception in conduct-disordered boys, we used the method of free person description. First, age-related effects on the various measures of peer descriptions were studied in normal boys. Second, peer descriptions made by oppositional-defiant and conduct-disordered (CD) boys were compared with those made by a normal control (NC) group and a psychiatric control group with internalizing disorders (ID). Just like younger normal boys when compared with older ones, CD boys, when compared with NC boys, perceived their peers more from an egocentric point of view. They paid less attention to the peers' inner, personal worlds, and instead attended more to the peers' external qualities; more specifically they focused more on items relating to the peers' activities with others. In all these respects ID boys took a position in between CD and NC boys without differing significantly from either group. Findings concerning CD boys' representations of peers are discussed with respect to other aspects of mental representation (perspective-taking, empathy) and the effect of these representations on the generation of nonprosocial and antisocial solutions to social problems with peers.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Achenbach, T., & Edelbrock, C. (1983).Manual for the Child Behavioral Checklist and Revised Child Behavioral Profile. Burlington: University of Vermont.

    Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association (1987).Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed., rev.). Washington, DC: Author.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barenboim, C. (1977). Developmental changes in the interpersonal cognitive system from middle childhood to adolescence.Child Development, 48, 1467–1474.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chandler, M. (1973). Egocentrism and antisocial behavior: The assessment and training of social perspective-taking skills.Developmental Psychology, 9, 326–332.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chandler, M., Greenspan, S., & Barenboim, C. (1974). Assessment and training of role-taking and referential communication skills in institutionalized emotionally disturbed children.Developmental Psychology, 10, 546–553.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, N., Kershner, J., & Wehrspann, W. (1985). Characteristics of social cognition in children with different symptom patterns.Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 6, 277–290.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dodge, K. (1980). Social cognition and children's aggressive behavior.Child Development, 51, 162–170.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dodge, K. (1986). A social information processing model of social competence in children. In M. Perlmutter (Ed.),Minnesota Symposium on Child Psychology (pp. 77–125). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dodge, K. (1993). Social-cognitive mechanisms in the development of conduct disorder and depression.Annual Review of Psychology, 44, 559–584.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dodge, K., & Frame, C. (1982). Social cognitive biases and deficits in aggressive boys.Child Development, 53, 620–635.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dodge, K., McClaskey, C., & Feldman, E. (1985). Situational approach to the assessment of social competence in children.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 53, 344–353.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dodge, K., & Newman, J. (1981). Biased decision making processes in aggressive boys.Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 9, 375–379.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dodge, K., Pettit, G., McClaskey, C., & Brown, M. (1986). Social competence in children.Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 58, 213–251.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dodge, K., & Tomlin, K. (1987). Cue utilization as a mechanism of attributional bias in aggressive children.Social Cognition, 5, 280–300.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eisenberg, N., & Miller, P. (1987). The relations of empathy to prosocial and related behaviors.Psychological Bulletin, 101, 91–119.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fleiss, J. (1971). Measuring nominal scales agreement among many raters (kappa).Psychological Bulletin, 76, 378–382.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gouze, K. (1987). Attention and social problem solving as correlates of aggression in preschool males.Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 15, 181–197.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Guerra, N., & Slaby, R. (1989). Evaluating factors in social problem solving by aggressive boys.Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 17, 277–289.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Honess, T. (1980). Self-reference in children's descriptions of peers: Egocentricity or collaboration?Child Development, 51, 476–480.

    Google Scholar 

  • Honess, T. (1981). Girls' and boys' perception of their peers: Peripheral vs. central and objective vs. interpretative aspects of free descriptions.British Journal of Psychology, 72, 485–497.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lahey, B., Loeber, R., Quay, H., Frick, P., & Grimm, J. (1992). Oppositional defiant and conduct disorders: Issues to be resolved for DMS-IV.Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 31, 539–546.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Livesley, W., & Bromley, D. (1973).Person perception in childhood and adolescence. London: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lochman, J. (1987). Self and peer perceptions and attributional biases of aggressive and nonaggressive boys.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 404–410.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lochman, J., Meyer, B., Rabiner, D., & White K. (1991). Parameters influencing social problem-solving of aggressive children. In R. Prinz (Ed.),Advances in behavioral assessment of children and families (Vol. 5, pp. 31–63). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loeber, R., & Schmaling, K. (1985). Empirical evidence for overt and covert patterns of antisocial conduct problems: a metaanalysis.Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 13, 337–352.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Matthys, W., Cohen-Kettenis, P., & Berkhout, J. (1994). Boys' and girls' perception of peers in middle childhood: Differences and similarities.Journal of Genetic Psychology, 155, 15–24.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McFall, R. (1982). A review and reformulation of the concept of social skills.Behavioral Assessment, 4, 1–33.

    Google Scholar 

  • Milich, R., & Dodge, K. (1984). Social information processing in child psychiatric populations.Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 12, 471–490.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, P., & Eisenberg, N. (1988). The relation of empathy to aggressive and externalizing/antisocial behavior.Psychological Bulletin, 103, 324–344.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Neale, J. (1966). Egocentrism in institutionalized and non-institutionalized children.Child Development, 37, 97–101.

    Google Scholar 

  • O'Mahony, J. (1986). Development of person description over adolescence.Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 15, 389–403.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peevers, S., & Secord, P. (1973). Developmental changes in attribution of descriptive concepts to persons.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 27, 120–128.

    Google Scholar 

  • Quigle, N., Garber, J., Panak, W., & Dodge, K. (1992). Social information processing in aggressive and depressed children.Child Development, 63, 1305–1320.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rubin, K., & Krasnor, L. (1986). Social cognitive and social behavioral perspectives on problem solving. In M. Perlmutter (Ed.),Minnesota Symposium on Child Psychology (pp. 1–68). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scarlett, H., Press, A., & Crockett, W. (1971). Children's descriptions of peers: Wernerian developmental analysis.Child Development, 42, 439–453.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, D., Hastorf, A., & Ellsworth, P. (1979).Person perception. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shantz, S. (1983). Social cognition. In J. Flavell & E. Markman (Eds.),Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Cognitive development (pp. 495–555). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smetana, J. (1990). Morality and conduct disorders. In M. Lewis & S. Miller (Eds.),Handbook of developmental psychopathology (pp. 157–179). New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Underwood, B., & Moore, B. (1982). Perspective taking and altruism.Psychological Bulletin, 91, 143–173.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1974).Manual for the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children — Revised. New York: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

This research was supported by a grant from the Stichting Kinderpostzegels Nederland. The authors wish to thank Dennis Bromley in Liverpool, England, and John Livesley, Vancouver, Canada, for their advice, Leo Njio in Utrecht, The Netherlands, for his support in data analysis, and two anonymous reviewers for their comments on a draft of this article.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Matthys, W., Walterbos, W., Van Engeland, H. et al. Conduct-disordered boys' perceptions of their liked peers. Cogn Ther Res 19, 357–372 (1995). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02230405

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02230405

Key words

Navigation